This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and"

Transcription

1 This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier s archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit:

2 Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Brain and Cognition journal homepage: Viewing artworks: Contributions of cognitive control and perceptual facilitation to aesthetic experience Gerald C. Cupchik a, *, Oshin Vartanian b, Adrian Crawley c, David J. Mikulis c a Department of Psychology, University of Toronto at Scarborough, 1265 Military Trail, Scarborough, Ont., Canada M1C 1A4 b Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, Ont., Canada M3J 1P3 c Department of Medical Imaging, University of Toronto, Toronto Western Hospital, 399 Bathurst Street, Toronto, Ont., Canada M5T 2S8 article info abstract Article history: Accepted 11 January 2009 Available online 14 February 2009 Keywords: Aesthetics Art Attention Emotion Perception When we view visual images in everyday life, our perception is oriented toward object identification. In contrast, when viewing visual images as artworks, we also tend to experience subjective reactions to their stylistic and structural properties. This experiment sought to determine how cognitive control and perceptual facilitation contribute to aesthetic perception along with the experience of emotion. Using functional MRI, we show that aesthetic perception activated bilateral insula which we attribute to the experience of emotion. Moreover, while adopting the aesthetic orientation activated the left lateral prefrontal cortex, paintings that facilitated visuospatial exploration activated the left superior parietal lobule. The results suggest that aesthetic experience is a function of the interaction between top-down orienting of attention and bottom-up perceptual facilitation. Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Everyday perception is pragmatic, oriented toward the identification of objects in visual scenes. Our inclination for assuming this orientation is so strong and automatic that subjects report perceiving objects in pictorial compositions even when those compositions are devoid of recognizable objects (Ishai, Fairhall, & Pepperell, 2007; see also Treisman, 1964). However, when artists create paintings, they seek to provide the viewer with more than an opportunity to engage in object recognition per se. Rather, they attempt to evoke subjective reactions in the viewer to the stylistic and structural properties of their works. In support of this view, a recent literature review demonstrated that the neural systems underpinning aesthetic perception are not limited to those involved in object recognition (Nadal, Munar, Capó, Rosselló, & Cela-Conde, 2008). Our current challenge is to highlight processes that distinguish perception geared toward aesthetic experience from perception geared toward object identification in everyday perception. Aesthetic experience is currently viewed as a special, psychological process involving attention focused on the object and the suppression of everyday concerns (see Cupchik & Winston, 1996). Anthony, Earl of Shaftesbury (1711/1964), a British philosopher of the Enlightenment, contrasted a disinterested enjoyment of something for its own sake and interested enjoyment of anticipated benefits. His student, Frances Hutcheson (1725), proposed a * Corresponding author. address: cupchik@utsc.utoronto.ca (G.C. Cupchik). more fully developed psychological theory of the experience of beauty along with a sharper division between everyday and aesthetic perception. He argued that the experience of beauty involves a special taste faculty, a special criterion of beauty and the disinterested pleasure that results from it. Kant (1790/1914) also used the concept of disinterested satisfaction in which practical concerns played no role, but he rejected the notion of a separate faculty of taste that operated subsequent to ordinary cognitive faculties. For him, the same cognitive faculties used in the experience of ordinary objects would search for a satisfying structure in the object associated with beauty, thereby yielding pleasure. Schopenhauer (1818/1969) elaborated Kantian notions in such a way that a genuine theory of aesthetic attitude began to emerge. According to Schopenhauer, everyday consciousness is in the service of the will, but if the intellect can temporarily subdue the will, a state of disinterested detachment can be achieved in which aesthetic qualities can be suitably contemplated. Beardsley s (see 1958, 1966) theory represents the most fully developed modern notion of aesthetic experience as a distinct process. He follows in the tradition of disinterest, arguing that it involves affect that is slightly detached and a sense of freedom from concerns about past and future. But he did not posit a special mental function or faculty that produces disinterest. Rather, it emerges from existing cognitive functions and processes. Modern psychological aesthetics has incorporated the themes that developed through Shaftesbury, Hutcheson, Kant, Schopenhauer and Beardsley and formulated aesthetic experience as a special kind of experience in which pleasure is produced by the disinterested contemplation of objects /$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: /j.bandc

3 G.C. Cupchik et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) One implication of this formulation is that adopting an aesthetic viewing orientation may require an intentional shift to overcome the automatic cuing of semantic categories and to reinvest attention in sensory experience elicited by the stylistic properties of artworks (Cupchik, 1992; Cupchik & Winston, 1996). The Russian Formalists understood that everyday perceptual activity leads to automaticity and habituation. Shklovsky (1917/1988) expressed these ideas very clearly: The purpose of art is to impart the sensation of things as they are perceived and not as they are known... Art is a way of experiencing the artfulness of an object. The object is not important (p. 20). Because subjects untrained in the visual arts automatically apply the object-identification habit to viewing artworks (Cupchik & Gebotys, 1988; Cupchik, Winston, & Herz, 1992; Winston & Cupchik, 1992), this suggests that top-down control geared toward attenuating this habit may be necessary to engage in aesthetic perception. Much evidence links the lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) to cognitive control (Burgess, Dumontheil, & Gilbert, 2007; Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Niewenhuis, 2004; Sakai & Passingham, 2003; Simons, Schölvinck, Gilbert, Frith, & Burgess, 2006; van Veen & Carter, 2006). Prima facie, in subjects untrained in the visual arts, the lateral PFC would appear to be a critical structure for orienting the cognitive and perceptual apparatus toward aesthetic perception. A second implication of the formulation is that aesthetic perception requires the active integration of physical sensory properties that constitute style to construct unified image structures (Arnheim, 1971). Palmer (1999) has contrasted two processes of region segmentation whereby images are divided into mutually exclusive and therefore distinguishable areas. A boundary-based approach to region segmentation involves the application of edge-detection algorithms that focus on luminance edges to locate closed contours. In contrast, a Gestalt-based treatment of segmentation assumes a more global approach to grouping discrete elements based on common properties and mutual attraction. Precisely this distinction was made in the early 20th century by Wolfflin s (1950/1915) description of the psychological processes underlying the linear (e.g., hard-edge Neoclassical) versus painterly (e.g., soft-edge Baroque) dimension of artistic style. This contrast is between outline and surfaces and mere visual appearance (Wolfflin, 1950/1915, p. 14). In operational terms, this is equivalent to a contrast between edges (Hubel & Weisel, 1962) or luminance, line ends, tilt, curvature (Treisman, 1985) and elongated blobs (Julesz, 1981) or spatial frequencies (Graham, 1981). Multidimensional scaling studies of same-difference judgments involving pairs of paintings representing diverse styles have shown that the hard-edge and soft-edge distinction is fundamental and prior to representational-abstract discriminations (Berlyne & Ogilvie, 1974; Cupchik, 1974, ). Whereas hard-edge paintings use contours to clearly define boundaries and isolate objects, soft-edge paintings have porous boundaries that engage viewers in an attempt to resolve forms. If aesthetic experience were a function of coherent image construction, then one would expect soft-edge paintings by virtue of containing ill-defined forms to facilitate aesthetic experience by stimulating active image construction. Given the role that the parietal lobes are known to play in the generation of abstract mental imagery (see Cavanna & Trimble, 2006; Goebel, Khorram-Sefat, Muckli, Hacker, & Singer, 1998), one would expect their engagement in aesthetic perception. This prediction is consistent with the results of a recent study in which activation in the precuneus was linked to the generation of mental imagery while subjects viewed paintings that lacked recognizable content, compared to a condition involving paintings that were rich with suggestive objects (Fairhall & Ishai, 2008). We contend that the two implications outlined above are not incompatible, but, rather, complementary (see Cupchik & Winston, 1996). The essential idea is that percepts develop from the global in the direction of the particular (Navon, 1977). This was proposed in the 1920s by researchers of the Aktualgenese (perceptual microgenesis) school who believed in the intrinsic structuredness of perception (see Flavell & Draguns, 1957) and reiterated by Neisser s (1967) notion that a pre-attentive stage of global processing is followed by a focal attentive stage during which the details of stimuli are identified. The physical/sensory information, derived from preattentive processing during the first glance, provide an expressive context within which more detailed features are perceived (Cupchik & Winston, 1996; see also Millis & Larson, 2008). This information includes first-order visual elements such as colour, tone, or texture, second-order properties related to collative properties (Berlyne, 1971) such as complexity and orderliness, and Gestalt properties including symmetry, good figure, harmony and contrast. After just a single glance at a painting (50 ms exposure), participants were able to discern the relative orderliness of two paintings and decide against a second viewing of High Arousal paintings with conflicting ordering principles (Cupchik & Berlyne, 1979). After longer exposure durations of 10+ s, participants disposed to absorption in art, music, literature and so on, favored multilayered images that evoked personal memories and thoughts about personal growth (Cupchik & Gignac, 2007). This complementarity is also embodied in Leder, Belke, Oeberst, and Augustin s (2004) recent information-processing model for the computation of aesthetic experience. Briefly, this model is comprised of five information-processing stages (perceptual analyses, implicit memory integration, explicit classification, cognitive mastering, evaluation) that are connected in sequence and through feedback loops. The final stage in this model (i.e., evaluation) involves appraising the meaning or interpretation placed on the artwork during cognitive mastering and generating two outputs: aesthetic judgment and aesthetic emotion. These evaluations constitute the endpoints of aesthetic experience. A critical feature of Leder et al. s model involves the context within which the object is viewed. Specifically, for the predictions of the model to hold, the person must engage the object as a work of art. While engagement is not sufficient for aesthetic evaluation, it is a necessary condition for aesthetic experience to occur. This is in line with our viewpoint that aesthetic experience necessitates an intentional orienting of perception toward distilling the properties of artworks. Moreover, the first stage in the model involves a stimulus-driven analysis of perceptual features, highlighting the role that bottom-up processes play in image construction. Thus, Leder et al. s (2004) model creates a framework within which topdown and bottom-up influences interact to generate aesthetic experiences. We relied on neural evidence to determine the relative contribution of intentional (top-down) and constructive (bottom-up) processes to aesthetic experience. Recently, Höfel and Jacobsen (2007) employed event-related brain potentials to tackle a similar question. In one condition they instructed subjects to simply view graphic patterns, whereas in another condition they instructed subjects to contemplate the beauty of those graphic patterns. The neural data revealed that while subjects engaged spontaneously in symmetry analysis in both conditions, evaluative categorization required contemplation and did not occur spontaneously. This demonstrates that when people contemplate the aesthetic properties (i.e., beauty) of visual images, a different set of neural processes is engaged than when they merely view the same visual images. In the present study, we instructed subjects to view paintings in the scanner for 10 s each under object-identification and aesthetic viewing orientations. We predicted that lateral PFC would be activated relatively more in the aesthetic viewing orientation, due to the increased need to exercise cognitive control to direct attention in that condition. In contrast, we predicted that structures known to mediate object recognition would be activated more under the

4 86 G.C. Cupchik et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) object-identification viewing orientation, particularly the fusiform gyrus (see Fairhall & Ishai, 2008). We also predicted that soft-edge paintings would facilitate aesthetic perception, and that this process would be mediated by the parietal lobes. Finally, we were interested in examining the role of the experience of emotion in aesthetic perception. Given that artists seek to evoke subjective reactions to their works in the viewer, we predicted that aesthetic perception would be more likely to engage the brain s emotional circuitry, especially in structures related to the experience of emotion such as insula (Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman, & Dolan, 2004; Critchley et al., 2005; Damasio et al., 2000) and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (Kringelbach & Rolls, 2004). 2. Method 2.1. fmri study Subjects We recruited 16 right-handed subjects (8 males, 8 females) with normal vision and no formal training in visual arts. The institutional review board at the University of Toronto approved the experimental protocol, and all subjects gave written informed consent Materials Each stimulus was presented once in the course of the experiment. The experimental stimuli consisted of 32 representational paintings in social (8 group portrait and nudes) and non-social (8 still-life and landscape) motifs. To control for the activation of background knowledge, previous studies had employed abstract graphic geometric patterns as stimuli (Höfel & Jacobsen, 2007; Jacobsen, Schubotz, Höfel, & von Cramon, 2006). We chose to rely on representational paintings instead because they are better suited than abstract graphic geometric patterns for affective evocation as well as object recognition, which formed the primary foci of our study. The representational paintings were presented in four blocks, with eight paintings in each block (2 nude, group portrait, landscape, still-life). Two experimental blocks included paintings executed in a linear, hard-edge style emphasizing contour and composition, and two experimental blocks included paintings executed in a painterly, soft-edge, and expressive style (Berlyne & Ogilvie, 1974; Cupchik, 1974, ). The higher-order ability to discriminate contour has been shown to underlie musical as well as spatial judgment (Cupchik, Phillips, & Hill, 2001), consistent with the central role played by the resolution of contour (i.e., outline) in discriminating fractal qualities in the style of gestural expressionist painters such as Jackson Pollock or members of the Quebec Automatiste school (Mureika, Cupchik, & Dyer, 2004; Mureika, Dyer, & Cupchik, 2005). The stimuli in the baseline blocks consisted of 16 non-representational paintings (8 fractal paintings by the American gestural expressionist artist Jackson Pollock, non-fractal paintings by the Israeli scholar Dr. Tsion Avital [Mureika et al., 2004; Mureika et al., 2005]). The non-representational paintings were also presented in four blocks, and each block consisted of four stimuli (2 Jackson Pollock, non-fractal paintings by Dr. Tsion Avital). While lacking representational content, non-representational paintings incorporate variations in colour, shape, and form. Therefore, comparing each experimental condition with the baseline condition served to isolate areas involved in viewing representational content under varying task instructions Procedure Prior to entering the scanner, subjects received thorough instructions on pragmatic and aesthetic viewing orientations. Specifically, for the pragmatic condition, they were instructed to apply an everyday informational criterion for viewing the paintings, and to approach the images in an objective and detached manner to obtain information about the content of the painting and visual narrative. In contrast, for the aesthetic condition, they were instructed to approach the paintings in a subjective and engaged manner, experiencing the mood of the work and the feelings it evokes, and to focus on its colours, tones, composition, and shapes. Note that neither condition required subjects to make any behavioural response in the scanner. Following instructions, subjects were given two practice trials under each condition to familiarize themselves with the process. The paintings used in the practice trials were not used in the experiment. Once inside the scanner, the experiment was conducted in eight blocks: Four blocks involving the representational paintings (i.e., experimental) and four blocks involving the non-representational paintings (i.e., baseline). Across pairs of subjects, the order of blocks was counterbalanced. The first block immediately following practice trials involved non-representational paintings. The four non-representational blocks were interspersed between the four representational blocks of paintings. The non-representational blocks served as baseline for the viewing of visual materials. The non-representational blocks were not accompanied by instructions to engage in any viewing orientation. Subjects were instead instructed not to engage in aesthetic or pragmatic perception while viewing the non-representational images. Images were presented using a rear projection screen inside the magnet. Viewing instructions were presented for 5 s. Each trial was initiated by a 1000 ms display of four + s at the centre of the viewing screen, and followed by a 200 ms blank screen. Each stimulus was presented for 10 s, followed by a 500 ms blank screen. Blank screens were inserted for backward masking fmri data acquisition and analysis All MRI data were acquired at a field strength of 1.5 T using a General Electric Signa scanner equipped with high-speed gradients. A high resolution 3D T1-weighted scan of each subject s brain anatomy was acquired with a matrix, field-ofview = 20 cm, covering the brain with 60 axial slices at a slice thickness of 2.2 mm. Functional images were acquired using a spiral-out sequence (Glover, 1999) with a matrix, field-ofview = 20 cm, covering the brain with 28 axial slices at a slice thickness of 4.5 mm. Each fmri scan consisted of 3 initial frames to allow the magnetization to reach equilibrium, followed by 286 frames of data acquired at a repetition time of 2.24 s (total scan time 10.5 min). Images were smoothed using a 7.6 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. We collapsed across content levels (nude, group portrait, landscape, still-life) for all analyses. Therefore, the design included three factors: Orientation (pragmatic, aesthetic), painting style (representational, non-representational), and edge (hard, soft). A regressor was created to model the BOLD response to each representational block compared to all other conditions. Thus, a two-factor ANOVA analysis using all four regressors completely models the BOLD response to representational images (modulated by orientation and edge) relative to the baseline response to nonrepresentational images. The frames corresponding to viewing instructions were excluded from the analysis. Each regressor was shifted by 6 s to account for the hemodynamic delay. The fmri data were analyzed using AFNI (Cox, 1996). The twofactor ANOVA analysis was performed on each subject s data using the AFNI program 3dDeconvolve. Contrasts of interest included viewing orientation (pragmatic, aesthetic), painting style (representational, non-representational), and edge (hard, soft). After transforming each subject s results into Talairach space, the results of each contrast of interest were averaged over subjects and a final t-test statistic map was calculated (corresponding to a random-effects analysis with subject as a random factor). Clusters of voxels

5 G.C. Cupchik et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) are reported as significant if the spatial extent of the activation exceeded a threshold of t > 2.1 and 4500 mm 3 volume, and the activation survived whole-brain family-wise correction for multiple comparisons (FWE). This corresponds to a map-wise one-sided p- value of.05 according to AFNI s AlphaSim Monte Carlo simulation. For our specific a priori hypotheses, we conducted the requisite region-of-interest (ROI) analyses. The ROI were selected based on earlier published reports, centred on peaks of maximum activation with a spherical radius of 10 mm. Specifically, we show the amplitude of the response during the relevant experimental conditions within lateral PFC ( 39, 57, 3 and 39, 54, 15; Simon et al., 2006), insula (30, 26, 12; Di Dio, Macaluso, & Rizzolatti, 2007), OFC ( 2, 36, 22; Kawabata & Zeki, 2004), superior parietal lobule ( 25, 54, 52; Goebel et al., 1998), and fusiform gyrus (38, 57, 19 and 35, 56, 21; Fairhall & Ishai, 2008). We did not use any threshold for the reported signal changes (i.e., we report raw signal changes compared to baseline) Post-scan behavioural study Recall that while in the scanner, subjects were not instructed to make behavioural responses to the stimuli. The decision not to collect behavioural responses in the scanner was made to facilitate maximal immersion in the task. To ensure that subjects engaged in the task as instructed, we relied on thorough instructions and the completion of practice trials prior to data collection. Nevertheless, immediately following the completion of the scans, each participant completed a modified version of the task outside of the scanner. Specifically, each participant viewed the same representational stimuli under the aesthetic and pragmatic viewing orientations, presented in the same block structure as in the scanner. Stimulus presentation was self-paced. After viewing each stimulus subjects were asked Did the image evoke emotion in you? to which they responded using a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). We also recorded reaction time (RT). We analyzed these data as part of our task analysis to determine whether emotion evocation is facilitated in relation to viewing soft-edge paintings in the aesthetic orientation, compared to viewing hard-edge paintings in the pragmatic orientation. 3. Results 3.1. Post-scan behavioural study First, we conducted an ANOVA with viewing orientation (pragmatic, aesthetic), edge (hard, soft), and image (nude, group portrait, landscape, still-life) as within-subjects variables, and RT to make emotion ratings (i.e., Did the image evoke emotion in you?) as the dependent variable. None of the main or interaction effects reached significance. Next, we repeated the above analysis but with the actual emotion ratings as the dependent variable. Confirming our prediction, the interaction between viewing orientation and edge was significant, F (1, 15) = 9.42, p <.01. Although neither simple main effect reached significance, in accordance with our prediction there was a trend for soft-edge images to receive higher emotion ratings in the context of the aesthetic than pragmatic viewing orientation fmri study Viewing under aesthetic and pragmatic conditions We first conducted an overall contrast between all experimental (pragmatic and aesthetic) versus baseline trials. This analysis revealed activation in a distributed network including left insula ( 35, 17, 6; t = 4.33) (BA 13), left cingulate gyrus ( 1, 11, 42; t = 4.06) (BA 32/24), left lingual gyrus ( 37, 78, 2; t = 5.94) (BA 19), and right inferior temporal gyrus (31, 72, 7; t = 5.68) (BA 37/19) (Fig. 1). Next, we conducted a set of planned contrasts, geared toward testing specific a priori hypotheses within designated ROI. We discuss our structures of interest within those ROI, but for the sake of completeness also report activations elsewhere in the brain if they exceeded a threshold of t > 2.1 and 4500 mm 3 volume and survived whole-brain FWE correction. For each ROI we illustrate the amplitude of the response (i.e., percent signal change) during the relevant experimental conditions Viewing under aesthetic or pragmatic conditions We contrasted the aesthetic and pragmatic task conditions individually versus the baseline condition. For the aesthetic-baseline contrast we predicted activations in OFC and insula. Results revealed activation in right (39, 21, 6; t = 4.93) and left ( 37, 24, 9; t = 4.15) insula (BA 13) (Fig. 2). For the pragmatic-baseline contrast we predicted activation in the fusiform gyrus, and the results revealed activation in right fusiform gyrus (27, 52, 19; t = 3.80) (BA 37) (Fig. 3). In addition, significant activations were detected in the bilateral occipital gyri in the aesthetic (33, 76, 4; t = 6.01; 38, 80, 0; t = 3.48) (BA 18/19) and pragmatic (30, 77, 6; t = 4.10; 42, 75, 10; t = 6.64) (BA 19) conditions compared to baseline Engaging in aesthetic perception For the direct comparison of aesthetic and pragmatic conditions we predicted activation in lateral PFC, and the results revealed that activation in left lateral PFC ( 44, 37, 7; t = 3.50) (BA 10) was rel- Fig. 1. Categorical contrast between the experimental (aesthetic, pragmatic) and baseline conditions revealed significant activation in a distributed network including (a) left insula ( 35, 17, 6) (BA 13), left lingual gyrus ( 37, 78, 2) (BA 19), right inferior temporal gyrus (31, 72, 7) (BA 37/19), and (b) left cingulate gyrus ( 1, 11, 42) (BA 32/ 24). Parametric maps superimposed on to axial (a) and sagittal (b) MRI. Images are presented in radiological orientation. All AFNI coordinates converted into true Talairach coordinates. The colour bar indicates T-value.

6 88 G.C. Cupchik et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) Fig. 3. The graph depicts the amplitude of the response in right fusiform gyrus (27, 52, 19) corresponding to the pragmatic and aesthetic conditions. The figure depicts the mean and standard errors of measurement (SEM). 4. Discussion Fig. 2. The graphs depict the amplitude of the response in (a) right (39, 21, 6) and (b) left ( 37, 24, 9) insula corresponding to the pragmatic and aesthetic conditions. The figures depict the mean and standard errors of measurement (SEM). atively higher in the aesthetic than pragmatic condition (Fig. 4). The reverse contrast did not reveal any significant area of activation Hard-edge versus soft-edge paintings For the direct contrast of soft versus hard-edge paintings we predicted activation in the superior parietal lobule, and the results revealed significant activation in left superior parietal lobule ( 20, 43, 47; t = 5.82) (BA 7). The reverse contrast did not reveal any significant area of activation. Importantly, to test whether softedge paintings facilitate aesthetic experience, we contrasted viewing soft-edge paintings under the aesthetic condition to viewing hard-edge paintings under the pragmatic condition. The results revealed that the left superior parietal lobule ( 34, 40, 57; t = 3.63) (BA 7) was activated relatively more when subjects viewed softedge paintings under the aesthetic condition than hard-edge paintings under the pragmatic condition (Fig. 5). The reverse contrast did not reveal any significant area of activation. The aesthetic and pragmatic conditions were accompanied by explicit instructions requiring interaction with paintings from a particular orientation. In contrast, the baseline condition was not accompanied by such instruction as subjects were asked to simply view the images. The contrast involving the pragmatic and aesthetic conditions versus the baseline condition revealed activation in a distributed network including left insula (BA 13), left cingulate gyrus (BA 32/24), left lingual gyrus (BA 19), and right inferior temporal gyrus (BA 37/19). We attribute activations in left insula and left cingulate gyrus in the experimental conditions to engagement in the task, as both structures have been shown to be activated across a wide set of paradigms whenever subjects engage in tasks relative to baseline conditions (Dosenbach et al., 2006). In addition, activations in right inferior temporal gyrus (BA 37/19) and left lingual gyrus (BA 19) can be attributed to mnemonic processing (Fletcher & Henson, 2001) while viewing representational paintings (Vartanian & Goel, 2004), respectively. To test whether the experience of emotion plays a larger role in the context of aesthetic perception, we contrasted the aesthetic and pragmatic task conditions individually versus the baseline condition. We expected to observe activations in OFC and/or insula in the aesthetic-baseline contrast. The results revealed activation in bilateral insula (BA 13) only (Fig. 2). We attribute this activation to the experience of emotion while viewing paintings, consistent with the role of the insula in the feeling of emotion (Critchley et al., 2004, 2005; Damasio et al., 2000). Interestingly, a recent fmri Fig. 4. (a) Categorical contrast between aesthetic and pragmatic conditions revealed significant activation in left lateral PFC ( 44, 37, 7) (BA 10). Parametric map superimposed on to axial MRI. Image is presented in radiological orientation. All AFNI coordinates converted into true Talairach coordinates. The colour bar indicates T-value. (b) The graph depicts the amplitude of the response in left lateral PFC corresponding to the pragmatic and aesthetic conditions. The figure depicts the mean and standard errors of measurement (SEM).

7 G.C. Cupchik et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) Fig. 5. (a) Left superior parietal lobule ( 34, 40, 57) (BA 7) was activated relatively more when soft-edge paintings were viewed under the aesthetic task orientation. Parametric map superimposed on to coronal MRI. Image is presented in radiological orientation. All AFNI coordinates converted into true Talairach coordinates. The colour bar indicates T-value. (b) The graph depicts the amplitude of the response in the left superior parietal lobule corresponding to the four experimental conditions. The figure depicts the mean and standard errors of measurement (SEM). study by Di Dio et al. (2007) showed the right anterior insula to be activated when subjects viewed objectively beautiful sculptures, compared to a condition in which the sculptures were modified to be less proportional. The authors proposed that positive feelings elicited by viewing the canonical sculptures was mediated by a cortical network involving the anterior insula. We believe that the insula may play a similar role in our study, where feelings are evoked in response to viewing paintings, although given insula s involvement in the experience of negative and positive emotions we make no claim about the valence of the emotions experienced by our subjects (see Phan, Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, 2002). In contrast, the pragmatic-baseline contrast revealed activation in right fusiform gyrus (BA 37) (Fig. 3). We attribute this activation to object recognition in representational paintings, including faces (Grill-Spector & Malach, 2004), and visual and spatial search for imposing situational models on pictorial scenes (Sieborger, Ferstl, & von Cramon, 2007). In addition, bilateral occipital gyri were activated more in the aesthetic (BA 18/19) and pragmatic (BA 19) conditions compared to baseline, which we attribute to greater load placed on the visual system when processing higher levels of visual detail in representational paintings. This study was conducted to address a specific question that we raised at the beginning of this paper: What are the processes that distinguish perception geared toward aesthetic experience from perception geared toward object identification? More specifically, does adopting the aesthetic viewing orientation require cognitive control, or is it the case that aesthetic perception is facilitated as a function of the perceptual features of the stimuli? The results suggest that aesthetic perception involves both processes. First, the direct comparison of aesthetic and pragmatic conditions resulted in activation in left lateral PFC (BA 10) (Fig. 4). We attribute this activation to top-down control in directing perception toward aesthetic orientation, consistent with the role of lateral PFC (BA 10) in top-down control of cognition (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004) and the maintenance of a main goal while performing concurrent sub-goals (Koechlin, Basso, Pietrini, Panzer, & Grafman, 1999). This interpretation is consistent with recent evidence linking lateral PFC to higher-order self-referential processing and the evaluation of internally generated information (Burgess et al., 2007; Christoff, Ream, Geddes, & Gabrieli, 2003; Northoff et al., 2006). According to this view, lateral PFC constitutes the interface between self-referential and cognitive demands of tasks, as would be expected under instructions to approach paintings from a subjective orientation. Interestingly, Jacobsen et al. (2006) reported activation in the frontomedian BA 9/10 when subjects judged the beauty of abstract graphic geometric patterns compared to a condition in which they judged the same patterns for symmetry. Although BA 10 has been linked to the introspective evaluation of internal mental states, evidence from research on prospective memory suggests that there may be a finer grained dissociation between the functions of the medial and lateral aspects of BA 10. Prospective memory tasks involve maintaining intention in memory for delayed execution. Whereas medial BA 10 may be involved in suppressing attention from external stimuli, lateral BA 10 may be involved in maintaining attention on internally generated cognitions (Burgess, Scott, & Frith, 2003; see also Christoff et al., 2003). Because the area activated in our study was lateral rather than medial, the activation may have been due to the maintenance and execution of internally generated goals to approach the artworks from an aesthetic orientation. Our results suggest that given the inherently self-referential nature of aesthetic perception, maintaining attention on internally generated cognitions may be an important component of this experience. We also addressed the extent to which aesthetic perception is preferentially engaged as a function of edge. This speaks to the bottom-up modulation of perceptual orientation as a function of stimulus features. The results revealed that left superior parietal lobule (BA 7) was activated relatively more when subjects viewed softedge paintings in the aesthetic orientation than hard-edge paintings in the pragmatic orientation (Fig. 5). Given that soft-edge paintings facilitate flexible visuospatial exploration of the stimuli (Berlyne & Ogilvie, 1974; Cupchik, 1974, ), we attribute the engagement of the left superior parietal lobule (BA 7) to viewers attempts to resolve the indeterminate forms in soft-edge paintings to construct coherent images in the aesthetic condition, consistent with the role that the parietal lobes are known to play in spatial cognition and visual imagery (Fairhall & Ishai, 2008; Marshall & Fink, 2001). 5. Conclusion Our results highlight the role of bilateral insula in aesthetic perception and the role of right fusiform gyrus in pragmatic perception. We attribute the involvement of bilateral insula in aesthetic perception to subjective experience of emotion. In turn, we attribute the involvement of the fusiform gyrus in pragmatic perception to the identification of meaningful objects and the imposition of situational models on pictorial scenes. Furthermore, our results extend the functions of left lateral PFC in top-down control of internally oriented and self-referential goals to the domain of aesthetic perception, while highlighting the involvement of left superior parietal lobule in the process of active image construction. Supporting recent theoretical formulations (Cupchik & Winston,

8 90 G.C. Cupchik et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) ; Leder et al., 2004), our results suggest that aesthetic experience is a function of the interaction between top-down orienting of attention and bottom-up perceptual input. Acknowledgments We thank Morris Moscovitch for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript, and Tonya Hendriks for help with statistical analysis. Ken Seergobin wrote the program that orchestrated stimulus presentation in the magnet as well as data collection in the separate stimulus viewing part of the study. Appendix A. Supplementary material Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi: /j.bandc References Arnheim, R. (1954/1971). Art and visual perception. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Beardsley, M. C. (1958). Aesthetics: Problems in the philosophy of criticism. New York: Harcourt. Beardsley, M. C. (1966). Aesthetics from classical Greece to the present. New York: MacMillan. Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts. Berlyne, D. E., & Ogilvie, J. C. (1974). Dimensions of perception of paintings. In D. E. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation (pp ). Washington: Hemisphere. Burgess, P. W., Dumontheil, I., & Gilbert, S. J. (2007). The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, Burgess, P. W., Scott, F. K., & Frith, A. D. (2003). The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: A lateral versus medial dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 41, Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain, 129, Christoff, K., Ream, J. M., Geddes, L. P., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2003). Evaluating selfgenerated information: Anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research, 29, Critchley, H. D., Rotshtein, P., Nagai, Y., O Doherty, J., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2005). Activity in the human brain predicting differential heart rate responses to emotional facial expressions. NeuroImage, 24, Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, Cupchik, G. C. (1974). An experimental investigation of perceptual and stylistic dimensions of paintings suggested by art history. In D. E. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation (pp ). Washington: Hemisphere. Cupchik, G. C. ( ). Perspective théorique et empirique sur la peinture impressionniste. Bulletin de Psychologie, 30, Cupchik, G. C. (1992). From perception to production: A multilevel analysis of the aesthetic process. In G. C. Cupchik & J. Laszlo (Eds.), Emerging visions of the aesthetic process: Psychology, semiology, philosophy (pp ). New York: Cambridge University Press. Cupchik, G. C., & Berlyne, D. E. (1979). The perception of collative properties in visual stimuli. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 20, Cupchik, G. C., & Gebotys, R. J. (1988). The search for meaning in art: Interpretive styles and judgments of quality. Visual Arts Research, 14, Cupchik, G. C., & Gignac, A. (2007). Finding meaning and expressing emotion in response to artworks. Visual Arts Research, 33(1), Cupchik, G. C., Phillips, K., & Hill, D. S. (2001). Shared processes in spatial rotation and musical permutation. Brain and Cognition, 46, Cupchik, G. C., & Winston, A. S. (1996). Confluence and divergence in empirical aesthetics, philosophy, and mainstream psychology. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of Perception & Cognition, Cognitive Ecology (pp ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Cupchik, G. C., Winston, A. S., & Herz, R. (1992). Judgments of similarity and difference between paintings. Visual Arts Research, 18, Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of selfgenerated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3, Di Dio, C., Macaluso, E., & Rizzolatti, G. (2007). Brain response to classical and renaissance sculptures. PloS Biology, 11, e1201. Dosenbach, N. U., Visscher, K. M., Palmer, E. D., Miezin, F. M., Wenger, K. K., Kang, H. C., et al. (2006). A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron, 50, Earl of Shaftesbury, A. (1711/1964). Characteristics of men, manners, opinions, and times (Vol. 1). Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill. Fairhall, S. L., & Ishai, A. (2008). Neural correlates of object indeterminacy in art compositions. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, Flavell, J., & Draguns, J. (1957). A microgenetic approach to perception and thought. Psychological Bulletin, 54, Fletcher, P. C., & Henson, R. N. A. (2001). Frontal lobes and human memory: Insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain, 124, Glover, G. H. (1999). Simple analytic spiral K-space algorithm. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 42, Goebel, R., Khorram-Sefat, D., Muckli, L., Hacker, H., & Singer, W. (1998). The constructive nature of vision: Direct evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of apparent motion and motion imagery. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10, Graham, N. (1981). Psychophysics of spatial-frequency channels. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (pp. 1 25). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2004). The human visual cortex. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 27, Höfel, L., & Jacobsen, T. (2007). Electrophysiological indices of processing aesthetics: Spontaneous or intentional processes? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65, Hubel, D. H., & Weisel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160, Hutcheson, F. (1725). An inquiry into the original of our ideas of beauty and virtue. London: D. Midwinter and Others. Ishai, A., Fairhall, S. L., & Pepperell, R. (2007). Perception, memory and aesthetics of indeterminate art. Brain Research Bulletin, 73, Jacobsen, T., Schubotz, R. I., Höfel, L., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. NeuroImage, 29, Julesz, B. (1981). Figure and ground perception in briefly presented isodipole textures. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kant, I. (1914). Critique of judgment (J. H. Bernard, Trans.). London: MacMillan (original work published, 1790). Kawabata, H., & Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., & Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, 339, Kringelbach, M. L., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology, 72, Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A., & Augustin, D. (2004). A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgements. British Journal of Psychology, 95, Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2001). Spatial cognition: Where we were and where we are. NeuroImage, 14, S4 S7. Millis, K., & Larson, M. (2008). Applying the construction integration framework to aesthetic responses to representational artworks. Discourse Processes, 45, Mureika, J. R., Cupchik, G. C., & Dyer, C. C. (2004). Multifractal fingerprints in the visual arts. Leonardo, 37, Mureika, J. R., Dyer, C. C., & Cupchik, G. C. (2005). Multifractal structure in nonrepresentational art. Physical Review E, 72, Nadal, M., Munar, E., Capó, M. A., Rosselló, J., & Cela-Conde, C. J. (2008). Towards a framework for the study of the neural correlates of aesthetic preference. Spatial Vision, 21, Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31, Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science. Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotional activation studies in PET and fmri. NeuroImage, 16, Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Niewenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, Sakai, K., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Prefrontal interactions reflect future task operations. Nature Neuroscience, 6, Schopenhauer, A. (1969). The world as will and representation (Vol. 1) (E. J. Payne, Trans.). New York: Dover (original published, 1818). Shklovsky, V. (1988). Art as technique. In D. Lodge (Ed.), Modern criticism and theory (pp ). New York: Longman (original published, 1917). Sieborger, F. T., Ferstl, E. C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2007). Making sense of nonsense: An fmri study of task induced inference processes during discourse comprehension. Brain Research, 1166, Simons, J. S., Schölvinck, M. L., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Differential components of prospective memory? Evidence from fmri. Neuropsychologia, 44,

9 G.C. Cupchik et al. / Brain and Cognition 70 (2009) Treisman, A. (1964). Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in selective attention. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 3, Triesman, A. (1985). Preattentive processing in vision. Computer Vision, Graphics, and Image Processing, 31, van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2006). Conflict and cognitive control in the brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2004). Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings. NeuroReport, 15, Winston, A. S., & Cupchik, G. C. (1992). The evaluation of high art and popular art by naive and experienced viewers. Visual Arts Research, 18, Wolfflin, H. (1950). Principles of art history. New York: Dover (original published, 1915).

Neuroaesthetics: a review Di Dio Cinzia 1 and Gallese Vittorio 1,2

Neuroaesthetics: a review Di Dio Cinzia 1 and Gallese Vittorio 1,2 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Neuroaesthetics: a review Di Dio Cinzia 1 and Gallese Vittorio 1,2 Neuroaesthetics is a relatively young field within cognitive neuroscience, concerned with the

More information

Empirical Aesthetics. William Seeley, Bates College

Empirical Aesthetics. William Seeley, Bates College Empirical Aesthetics William Seeley, Bates College Author's Note: This is a draft copy of the entry "Empirical Aesthetics" to appear in the forthcoming The Oxford Encyclopedia of Aesthetics, 2 nd Edition

More information

Progress in Neurobiology

Progress in Neurobiology Progress in Neurobiology 94 (2011) 39 48 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Progress in Neurobiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/pneurobio The neural foundations of aesthetic appreciation

More information

Natural Scenes Are Indeed Preferred, but Image Quality Might Have the Last Word

Natural Scenes Are Indeed Preferred, but Image Quality Might Have the Last Word Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 3, No. 1, 52 56 1931-3896/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014835 Natural Scenes Are Indeed Preferred, but

More information

23/01/51. Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the. VEP waveform. How are ERP related to gender? Event-Related Potential (ERP)

23/01/51. Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the. VEP waveform. How are ERP related to gender? Event-Related Potential (ERP) 23/01/51 EventRelated Potential (ERP) Genderselective effects of the and N400 components of the visual evoked potential measuring brain s electrical activity (EEG) responded to external stimuli EEG averaging

More information

Supplemental Material for Gamma-band Synchronization in the Macaque Hippocampus and Memory Formation

Supplemental Material for Gamma-band Synchronization in the Macaque Hippocampus and Memory Formation Supplemental Material for Gamma-band Synchronization in the Macaque Hippocampus and Memory Formation Michael J. Jutras, Pascal Fries, Elizabeth A. Buffalo * *To whom correspondence should be addressed.

More information

Discrete cortical regions associated with the musical beauty of major and minor chords

Discrete cortical regions associated with the musical beauty of major and minor chords Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2008, 8 (2), 26-3 doi: 0.3758/CABN.8.2.26 Discrete cortical regions associated with the musical beauty of major and minor chords MIHO SUZUKI, NOBUYUKI OKAMURA,

More information

Neural Signatures of the Aesthetic of Dance

Neural Signatures of the Aesthetic of Dance Neural Signatures of the Aesthetic of Dance Beatriz Calvo-Merino City University London Summary This essay explores a scientific perspective for studying the mechanism that the human mind and brain employs

More information

Leder Belke Oeberst & Augustin 2004

Leder Belke Oeberst & Augustin 2004 2016 Vol. 36 No. 2 101-106 PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPLORATION 1 2 1 1. 100084 2. 100084 B8409 A 1003-5184 2016 02-0101 - 06 1 aesthetics Alexander Gottlieb Baumgarten 2 1735 /1998 Baumgarten Fechner 1896 Kant 1790

More information

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland AWARD NUMBER: W81XWH-13-1-0491 TITLE: Default, Cognitive, and Affective Brain Networks in Human Tinnitus PRINCIPAL INVESTIGATOR: Jennifer R. Melcher, PhD CONTRACTING ORGANIZATION: Massachusetts Eye and

More information

Are there opposite pupil responses to different aspects of processing fluency?

Are there opposite pupil responses to different aspects of processing fluency? Are there opposite pupil responses to different aspects of processing fluency? Sophie G. Elschner & Ronald Hübner 60 th TeaP, Marburg, March 12 th 2018 Types of Processing Fluency Processing Fluency The

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Table S1. Peak coordinates of the regions showing repetition suppression at P- uncorrected < 0.001 MNI Number of Anatomical description coordinates T P voxels Bilateral ant. cingulum

More information

The Thinking-I and the Being-I in Psychology of the Arts

The Thinking-I and the Being-I in Psychology of the Arts 1999, Vol. 12, NO. 3, 165-173 Copyright 1999 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Thinking-I and the Being-I in Psychology of the Arts Gerald C. Cupchik University of Toronto ABSTRAC? The thinking-i

More information

With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long!

With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long! Event Related Potentials (ERPs): A window onto the timing of cognition Kim Sweeney COGS1- Introduction to Cognitive Science November 19, 2009 With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long! Overview

More information

Kant: Notes on the Critique of Judgment

Kant: Notes on the Critique of Judgment Kant: Notes on the Critique of Judgment First Moment: The Judgement of Taste is Disinterested. The Aesthetic Aspect Kant begins the first moment 1 of the Analytic of Aesthetic Judgment with the claim that

More information

Aesthetic appreciation: Convergence from experimental aesthetics and physiology

Aesthetic appreciation: Convergence from experimental aesthetics and physiology Chapter 4 Aesthetic appreciation: Convergence from experimental aesthetics and physiology 4.1 Introduction strange behaviors? Among the puzzling things that humans do which at first sight have no apparent

More information

Brain.fm Theory & Process

Brain.fm Theory & Process Brain.fm Theory & Process At Brain.fm we develop and deliver functional music, directly optimized for its effects on our behavior. Our goal is to help the listener achieve desired mental states such as

More information

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring 2009 Week 6 Class Notes Pitch Perception Introduction Pitch may be described as that attribute of auditory sensation in terms

More information

What is music as a cognitive ability?

What is music as a cognitive ability? What is music as a cognitive ability? The musical intuitions, conscious and unconscious, of a listener who is experienced in a musical idiom. Ability to organize and make coherent the surface patterns

More information

Tuning the Brain: Neuromodulation as a Possible Panacea for treating non-pulsatile tinnitus?

Tuning the Brain: Neuromodulation as a Possible Panacea for treating non-pulsatile tinnitus? Tuning the Brain: Neuromodulation as a Possible Panacea for treating non-pulsatile tinnitus? Prof. Sven Vanneste The University of Texas at Dallas School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences Lab for Clinical

More information

Individual differences in prediction: An investigation of the N400 in word-pair semantic priming

Individual differences in prediction: An investigation of the N400 in word-pair semantic priming Individual differences in prediction: An investigation of the N400 in word-pair semantic priming Xiao Yang & Lauren Covey Cognitive and Brain Sciences Brown Bag Talk October 17, 2016 Caitlin Coughlin,

More information

Is moral beauty different from facial beauty? Evidence from an fmri study

Is moral beauty different from facial beauty? Evidence from an fmri study doi:10.1093/scan/nsu123 SCAN (2015) 10, 814 ^ 823 Is moral beauty different from facial beauty? Evidence from an fmri study Tingting Wang, 1,2 Lei Mo, 1 Ce Mo, 1 Li Hai Tan, 3 Jonathan S. Cant, 4 Luojin

More information

Information Theory Applied to Perceptual Research Involving Art Stimuli

Information Theory Applied to Perceptual Research Involving Art Stimuli Marilyn Zurmuehlen Working Papers in Art Education ISSN: 2326-7070 (Print) ISSN: 2326-7062 (Online) Volume 2 Issue 1 (1983) pps. 98-102 Information Theory Applied to Perceptual Research Involving Art Stimuli

More information

Aesthetic package design: A behavioral, neural, and psychological investigation

Aesthetic package design: A behavioral, neural, and psychological investigation Journal of CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY Journal of Consumer Psychology 20 (2010) 431 441 Aesthetic package design: A behavioral, neural, and psychological investigation Martin Reimann a,, Judith Zaichkowsky b,

More information

Involved brain areas in processing of Persian classical music: an fmri study

Involved brain areas in processing of Persian classical music: an fmri study Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences 5 (2010) 1124 1128 WCPCG-2010 Involved brain areas in processing of Persian classical music: an fmri study Farzaneh, Pouladi

More information

A 5 Hz limit for the detection of temporal synchrony in vision

A 5 Hz limit for the detection of temporal synchrony in vision A 5 Hz limit for the detection of temporal synchrony in vision Michael Morgan 1 (Applied Vision Research Centre, The City University, London) Eric Castet 2 ( CRNC, CNRS, Marseille) 1 Corresponding Author

More information

Images of Mary: Effects of Style and Content on Reactions to Marian Art Abstract METHOD Participants

Images of Mary: Effects of Style and Content on Reactions to Marian Art Abstract METHOD Participants Images of Mary: Effects of Style and Content on Reactions to Marian Art Donald J. Polzella, Johann G. Roten, and Christopher W. Parker University of Dayton Abstract 105 college students rated digitized

More information

Brain and Cognition 76 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Brain and Cognition. journal homepage:

Brain and Cognition 76 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Brain and Cognition. journal homepage: Brain and Cognition 76 (2011) 172 183 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Brain and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c The Copenhagen Neuroaesthetics conference: Prospects and

More information

Color and visual complexity in abstract images

Color and visual complexity in abstract images Received: 27 June 2018 Revised and accepted: 18 July 2018 DOI: 10.1002/col.22266 RESEARCH ARTICLE Color and visual complexity in abstract images Rengin Kocaoglu Nilgün Olguntürk Department of Interior

More information

Supporting Online Material

Supporting Online Material Supporting Online Material Subjects Although there is compelling evidence that non-musicians possess mental representations of tonal structures, we reasoned that in an initial experiment we would be most

More information

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Cover Page. The handle   holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Cover Page The handle http://hdl.handle.net/1887/62348 holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation. Author: Crucq, A.K.C. Title: Abstract patterns and representation: the re-cognition of

More information

Music training and mental imagery

Music training and mental imagery Music training and mental imagery Summary Neuroimaging studies have suggested that the auditory cortex is involved in music processing as well as in auditory imagery. We hypothesized that music training

More information

NEUROSCIENCE AND VISUAL ART; MOVING THROUGH EMPATHY TO THE INEFFABLE

NEUROSCIENCE AND VISUAL ART; MOVING THROUGH EMPATHY TO THE INEFFABLE Medicinska naklada - Zagreb, Croatia Conference paper NEUROSCIENCE AND VISUAL ART; MOVING THROUGH EMPATHY TO THE INEFFABLE Mark Agius Clare College Cambridge, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge,

More information

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY Eugene Mikyung Kim Department of Music Technology, Korea National University of Arts eugene@u.northwestern.edu ABSTRACT

More information

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion Adam Tierney, *1 Aniruddh Patel #2, Mara Breen^3 * Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, United Kingdom # Department

More information

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions International Symposium on Performance Science ISBN 978-94-90306-02-1 The Author 2011, Published by the AEC All rights reserved Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

More information

INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC

INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC Michal Zagrodzki Interdepartmental Chair of Music Psychology, Fryderyk Chopin University of Music, Warsaw, Poland mzagrodzki@chopin.edu.pl

More information

Memory and learning: experiment on Sonata KV 331, in A Major by W. A. Mozart

Memory and learning: experiment on Sonata KV 331, in A Major by W. A. Mozart Bulletin of the Transilvania University of Braşov Series VIII: Performing Arts Vol. 10 (59) No. 1-2017 Memory and learning: experiment on Sonata KV 331, in A Major by W. A. Mozart Stela DRĂGULIN 1, Claudia

More information

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY 1 Psychology PSY 120 Introduction to Psychology 3 cr A survey of the basic theories, concepts, principles, and research findings in the field of Psychology. Core

More information

Event-related potential P2 correlates of implicit aesthetic experience Xiaoyi Wang a,b, Yujing Huang a,b, Qingguo Ma a,b and Nan Li c

Event-related potential P2 correlates of implicit aesthetic experience Xiaoyi Wang a,b, Yujing Huang a,b, Qingguo Ma a,b and Nan Li c CE: Jishana ED: Maitreyee Op: Sampath WNR 55: LWW_WNR_55 Brain imaging; Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology 1 Event-related potential P correlates of implicit aesthetic experience Xiaoyi Wang a,b,

More information

Population codes representing musical timbre for high-level fmri categorization of music genres

Population codes representing musical timbre for high-level fmri categorization of music genres Population codes representing musical timbre for high-level fmri categorization of music genres Michael Casey 1, Jessica Thompson 1, Olivia Kang 2, Rajeev Raizada 3, and Thalia Wheatley 2 1 Bregman Music

More information

The Healing Power of Music. Scientific American Mind William Forde Thompson and Gottfried Schlaug

The Healing Power of Music. Scientific American Mind William Forde Thompson and Gottfried Schlaug The Healing Power of Music Scientific American Mind William Forde Thompson and Gottfried Schlaug Music as Medicine Across cultures and throughout history, music listening and music making have played a

More information

Klee or Kid? The subjective experience of drawings from children and Paul Klee Pronk, T.

Klee or Kid? The subjective experience of drawings from children and Paul Klee Pronk, T. UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Klee or Kid? The subjective experience of drawings from children and Paul Klee Pronk, T. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Pronk, T. (Author).

More information

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension Bahriye Selin Gokcesu (bgokcesu@hsc.edu) Department of Psychology, 1 College Rd. Hampden Sydney, VA, 23948 Abstract One of the prevailing questions

More information

The Effects of Web Site Aesthetics and Shopping Task on Consumer Online Purchasing Behavior

The Effects of Web Site Aesthetics and Shopping Task on Consumer Online Purchasing Behavior The Effects of Web Site Aesthetics and Shopping Task on Consumer Online Purchasing Behavior Cai, Shun The Logistics Institute - Asia Pacific E3A, Level 3, 7 Engineering Drive 1, Singapore 117574 tlics@nus.edu.sg

More information

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Emergence of dmpfc and BLA 4-Hz oscillations during freezing behavior.

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Emergence of dmpfc and BLA 4-Hz oscillations during freezing behavior. Supplementary Figure 1 Emergence of dmpfc and BLA 4-Hz oscillations during freezing behavior. (a) Representative power spectrum of dmpfc LFPs recorded during Retrieval for freezing and no freezing periods.

More information

REVIEW ARTICLE IDEAL EMBODIMENT: KANT S THEORY OF SENSIBILITY

REVIEW ARTICLE IDEAL EMBODIMENT: KANT S THEORY OF SENSIBILITY Cosmos and History: The Journal of Natural and Social Philosophy, vol. 7, no. 2, 2011 REVIEW ARTICLE IDEAL EMBODIMENT: KANT S THEORY OF SENSIBILITY Karin de Boer Angelica Nuzzo, Ideal Embodiment: Kant

More information

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. BACKGROUND AND AIMS [Leah Latterner]. Introduction Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin Yale University, Cognition of Musical

More information

Permutations of the Octagon: An Aesthetic-Mathematical Dialectic

Permutations of the Octagon: An Aesthetic-Mathematical Dialectic Proceedings of Bridges 2015: Mathematics, Music, Art, Architecture, Culture Permutations of the Octagon: An Aesthetic-Mathematical Dialectic James Mai School of Art / Campus Box 5620 Illinois State University

More information

Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription

Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription 2.2.1 Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription Continued research and development of a broadcast news speech transcription system has been promoted. Universities and researchers

More information

Structural and functional neuroplasticity of tinnitus-related distress and duration

Structural and functional neuroplasticity of tinnitus-related distress and duration Structural and functional neuroplasticity of tinnitus-related distress and duration Martin Meyer, Patrick Neff, Martin Schecklmann, Tobias Kleinjung, Steffi Weidt, Berthold Langguth University of Zurich,

More information

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Covert Painting Simulations Influence Aesthetic Appreciation of Artworks

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Covert Painting Simulations Influence Aesthetic Appreciation of Artworks Manuscript under review for Psychological Science Covert Painting Simulations Influence Aesthetic Appreciation of Artworks Journal: Psychological Science Manuscript ID: PSCI--0.R Manuscript Type: Short

More information

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A.

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A. Psychology MAJOR, MINOR PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. (chair), George W. ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A. The core program in psychology emphasizes the learning of representative

More information

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC Fabio Morreale, Raul Masu, Antonella De Angeli, Patrizio Fava Department of Information Engineering and Computer Science, University Of Trento, Italy

More information

Emotion in aesthetics: Reactive and reflective models. Gerald C. Cupchik

Emotion in aesthetics: Reactive and reflective models. Gerald C. Cupchik POETICS ELSEVIER Poetics 23 (1994) 177-188 Emotion in aesthetics: Reactive and reflective models Gerald C. Cupchik Scarborough College, Life Science Division, University of Toronto, Scarborough, Ontario,

More information

COMPLEXITY AND AESTHETIC PREFERENCE FOR DIVERSE VISUAL STIMULI

COMPLEXITY AND AESTHETIC PREFERENCE FOR DIVERSE VISUAL STIMULI COMPLEXITY AND AESTHETIC PREFERENCE FOR DIVERSE VISUAL STIMULI DOCTORAL THESIS AUTHOR: Marcos Nadal Roberts DIRECTOR: Camilo José Cela Conde and Gisèle Marty Departament de Psicologia Universitat de les

More information

I. INTRODUCTION. Electronic mail:

I. INTRODUCTION. Electronic mail: Neural activity associated with distinguishing concurrent auditory objects Claude Alain, a) Benjamin M. Schuler, and Kelly L. McDonald Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, 3560

More information

A Hybrid Model of Painting: Pictorial Representation of Visuospatial Attention through an Eye Tracking Research

A Hybrid Model of Painting: Pictorial Representation of Visuospatial Attention through an Eye Tracking Research A Hybrid Model of Painting: Pictorial Representation of Visuospatial Attention through an Eye Tracking Research S.A. Al-Maqtari, R.O. Basaree, and R. Legino Abstract A hybrid pictorial representation of

More information

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS Areti Andreopoulou Music and Audio Research Laboratory New York University, New York, USA aa1510@nyu.edu Morwaread Farbood

More information

Stewart, Lauren and Walsh, Vincent (2001) Neuropsychology: music of the hemispheres Dispatch, Current Biology Vol.11 No.

Stewart, Lauren and Walsh, Vincent (2001) Neuropsychology: music of the hemispheres Dispatch, Current Biology Vol.11 No. Originally published: Stewart, Lauren and Walsh, Vincent (2001) Neuropsychology: music of the hemispheres Dispatch, Current Biology Vol.11 No.4, 2001, R125-7 This version: http://eprints.goldsmiths.ac.uk/204/

More information

Rethinking the Aesthetic Experience: Kant s Subjective Universality

Rethinking the Aesthetic Experience: Kant s Subjective Universality Spring Magazine on English Literature, (E-ISSN: 2455-4715), Vol. II, No. 1, 2016. Edited by Dr. KBS Krishna URL of the Issue: www.springmagazine.net/v2n1 URL of the article: http://springmagazine.net/v2/n1/02_kant_subjective_universality.pdf

More information

High School Photography 1 Curriculum Essentials Document

High School Photography 1 Curriculum Essentials Document High School Photography 1 Curriculum Essentials Document Boulder Valley School District Department of Curriculum and Instruction February 2012 Introduction The Boulder Valley Elementary Visual Arts Curriculum

More information

J 0 rgen Weber The Judgement of the Eye

J 0 rgen Weber The Judgement of the Eye J 0 rgen Weber The Judgement of the Eye Jiirgen Weber The J udgement of the Eye The Metamorphoses of Geometry - One of the Sources of Visual Perception and Consciousness (A Further Development of Gestalt

More information

Trauma & Treatment: Neurologic Music Therapy and Functional Brain Changes. Suzanne Oliver, MT-BC, NMT Fellow Ezequiel Bautista, MT-BC, NMT

Trauma & Treatment: Neurologic Music Therapy and Functional Brain Changes. Suzanne Oliver, MT-BC, NMT Fellow Ezequiel Bautista, MT-BC, NMT Trauma & Treatment: Neurologic Music Therapy and Functional Brain Changes Suzanne Oliver, MT-BC, NMT Fellow Ezequiel Bautista, MT-BC, NMT Music Therapy MT-BC Music Therapist - Board Certified Certification

More information

Embodied music cognition and mediation technology

Embodied music cognition and mediation technology Embodied music cognition and mediation technology Briefly, what it is all about: Embodied music cognition = Experiencing music in relation to our bodies, specifically in relation to body movements, both

More information

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction Music-Perception Winter 1990, Vol. 8, No. 2, 203-214 I990 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds ROY D. PATTERSON MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge,

More information

I like those glasses on you, but not in the mirror: Fluency, preference, and virtual mirrors

I like those glasses on you, but not in the mirror: Fluency, preference, and virtual mirrors Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY Journal of Consumer Psychology 20 (2010) 471 475 I like those glasses on you, but not in the mirror: Fluency, preference, and virtual

More information

Blending in action: Diagrams reveal conceptual integration in routine activity

Blending in action: Diagrams reveal conceptual integration in routine activity Cognitive Science Online, Vol.1, pp.34 45, 2003 http://cogsci-online.ucsd.edu Blending in action: Diagrams reveal conceptual integration in routine activity Beate Schwichtenberg Department of Cognitive

More information

Surprise & emotion. Theoretical paper Key conference theme: Interest, surprise and delight

Surprise & emotion. Theoretical paper Key conference theme: Interest, surprise and delight Surprise & emotion Geke D.S. Ludden, Paul Hekkert & Hendrik N.J. Schifferstein, Department of Industrial Design, Delft University of Technology, Landbergstraat 15, 2628 CE Delft, The Netherlands, phone:

More information

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education Spring, 2003, No. 156 Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance Zebulon Highben Ohio State University Caroline

More information

Modeling memory for melodies

Modeling memory for melodies Modeling memory for melodies Daniel Müllensiefen 1 and Christian Hennig 2 1 Musikwissenschaftliches Institut, Universität Hamburg, 20354 Hamburg, Germany 2 Department of Statistical Science, University

More information

Penultimate Draft- Final version forthcoming in Philosophical Psychology

Penultimate Draft- Final version forthcoming in Philosophical Psychology Penultimate Draft- Final version forthcoming in Philosophical Psychology The Phenomenological Mind: An Introduction to Philosophy of Mind and Cognitive Science Shaun Gallagher and Dan Zahavi New York:

More information

The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and Attitude According to Congruity-Incongruity

The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and Attitude According to Congruity-Incongruity Volume 118 No. 19 2018, 2435-2449 ISSN: 1311-8080 (printed version); ISSN: 1314-3395 (on-line version) url: http://www.ijpam.eu ijpam.eu The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and

More information

VivoSense. User Manual Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) Analysis Module. VivoSense, Inc. Newport Beach, CA, USA Tel. (858) , Fax.

VivoSense. User Manual Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) Analysis Module. VivoSense, Inc. Newport Beach, CA, USA Tel. (858) , Fax. VivoSense User Manual Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) Analysis VivoSense Version 3.1 VivoSense, Inc. Newport Beach, CA, USA Tel. (858) 876-8486, Fax. (248) 692-0980 Email: info@vivosense.com; Web: www.vivosense.com

More information

Regional homogeneity on resting state fmri in patients with tinnitus

Regional homogeneity on resting state fmri in patients with tinnitus HOSTED BY Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Journal of Otology 9 (2014) 173e178 www.journals.elsevier.com/journal-of-otology/ Regional homogeneity on resting state fmri in patients

More information

Years 9 and 10 standard elaborations Australian Curriculum: Drama

Years 9 and 10 standard elaborations Australian Curriculum: Drama Purpose Structure The standard elaborations (SEs) provide additional clarity when using the Australian Curriculum achievement standard to make judgments on a five-point scale. These can be used as a tool

More information

Acta Psychologica 130 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage:

Acta Psychologica 130 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage: Acta Psychologica 130 (2009) 241 250 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/actpsy Just how stable are stable aesthetic features? Symmetry,

More information

Motion Video Compression

Motion Video Compression 7 Motion Video Compression 7.1 Motion video Motion video contains massive amounts of redundant information. This is because each image has redundant information and also because there are very few changes

More information

Shared liking and association valence for representational art but not abstract art

Shared liking and association valence for representational art but not abstract art Journal of Vision (2015) 15(5):11, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/15/5/11 1 Shared liking and association valence for representational art but not abstract art Department of Psychology, University

More information

Computational Parsing of Melody (CPM): Interface Enhancing the Creative Process during the Production of Music

Computational Parsing of Melody (CPM): Interface Enhancing the Creative Process during the Production of Music Computational Parsing of Melody (CPM): Interface Enhancing the Creative Process during the Production of Music Andrew Blake and Cathy Grundy University of Westminster Cavendish School of Computer Science

More information

Research Methodology for the Internal Observation of Design Thinking through the Creative Self-formation Process

Research Methodology for the Internal Observation of Design Thinking through the Creative Self-formation Process Research Methodology for the Internal Observation of Design Thinking through the Creative Self-formation Process Yukari Nagai 1, Toshiharu Taura 2 and Koutaro Sano 1 1 Japan Advanced Institute of Science

More information

Urban Space and Architectural Scale - Two Examples of Empirical Research in Architectural Aesthetics

Urban Space and Architectural Scale - Two Examples of Empirical Research in Architectural Aesthetics Urban Space and Architectural Scale - Two Examples of Empirical Research in Architectural Aesthetics Weber, Ralf and Wolter, Birgit*; Jacobsen, Thomas*; Vosskoetter, Silke** * Collaborators in Project

More information

Intimacy and Embodiment: Implications for Art and Technology

Intimacy and Embodiment: Implications for Art and Technology Intimacy and Embodiment: Implications for Art and Technology Sidney Fels Dept. of Electrical and Computer Engineering University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada ssfels@ece.ubc.ca ABSTRACT People

More information

Second Grade: National Visual Arts Core Standards

Second Grade: National Visual Arts Core Standards Second Grade: National Visual Arts Core Standards Connecting #VA:Cn10.1 Process Component: Interpret Anchor Standard: Synthesize and relate knowledge and personal experiences to make art. Enduring Understanding:

More information

Key Term: Anti-Kantian Aesthetics. Peter Blouw. Innovative, influential, and always somewhat controversial, Immanuel Kant s

Key Term: Anti-Kantian Aesthetics. Peter Blouw. Innovative, influential, and always somewhat controversial, Immanuel Kant s Blouw 1 Key Term: Anti-Kantian Aesthetics. Peter Blouw Innovative, influential, and always somewhat controversial, Immanuel Kant s Critique of Judgment provided the prevailing account of aesthetic judgment

More information

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical and schemas Stella Paraskeva (,) Stephen McAdams (,) () Institut de Recherche et de Coordination

More information

Reconstruction of Ca 2+ dynamics from low frame rate Ca 2+ imaging data CS229 final project. Submitted by: Limor Bursztyn

Reconstruction of Ca 2+ dynamics from low frame rate Ca 2+ imaging data CS229 final project. Submitted by: Limor Bursztyn Reconstruction of Ca 2+ dynamics from low frame rate Ca 2+ imaging data CS229 final project. Submitted by: Limor Bursztyn Introduction Active neurons communicate by action potential firing (spikes), accompanied

More information

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore Issue: 17, 2010 Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore ABSTRACT Rational Consumers strive to make optimal

More information

Edward Winters. Aesthetics and Architecture. London: Continuum, 2007, 179 pp. ISBN

Edward Winters. Aesthetics and Architecture. London: Continuum, 2007, 179 pp. ISBN zlom 7.5.2009 8:12 Stránka 111 Edward Winters. Aesthetics and Architecture. London: Continuum, 2007, 179 pp. ISBN 0826486320 Aesthetics and Architecture, by Edward Winters, a British aesthetician, painter,

More information

The Influence of Art Expertise and Training on Emotion and Preference Ratings for Representational and Abstract Artworks

The Influence of Art Expertise and Training on Emotion and Preference Ratings for Representational and Abstract Artworks RESEARCH ARTICLE The Influence of Art Expertise and Training on Emotion and Preference Ratings for Representational and Abstract Artworks Jorien van Paasschen 1, Francesca Bacci 1,2, David P. Melcher 1

More information

The e ect of musicianship on pitch memory in performance matched groups

The e ect of musicianship on pitch memory in performance matched groups AUDITORYAND VESTIBULAR SYSTEMS The e ect of musicianship on pitch memory in performance matched groups Nadine Gaab and Gottfried Schlaug CA Department of Neurology, Music and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Beth

More information

Instructions to Authors

Instructions to Authors Instructions to Authors European Journal of Psychological Assessment Hogrefe Publishing GmbH Merkelstr. 3 37085 Göttingen Germany Tel. +49 551 999 50 0 Fax +49 551 999 50 111 publishing@hogrefe.com www.hogrefe.com

More information

ITU-T Y.4552/Y.2078 (02/2016) Application support models of the Internet of things

ITU-T Y.4552/Y.2078 (02/2016) Application support models of the Internet of things I n t e r n a t i o n a l T e l e c o m m u n i c a t i o n U n i o n ITU-T TELECOMMUNICATION STANDARDIZATION SECTOR OF ITU Y.4552/Y.2078 (02/2016) SERIES Y: GLOBAL INFORMATION INFRASTRUCTURE, INTERNET

More information

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION Michael Epstein 1,2, Mary Florentine 1,3, and Søren Buus 1,2 1Institute for Hearing, Speech, and Language 2Communications and Digital

More information

that would join theoretical philosophy (metaphysics) and practical philosophy (ethics)?

that would join theoretical philosophy (metaphysics) and practical philosophy (ethics)? Kant s Critique of Judgment 1 Critique of judgment Kant s Critique of Judgment (1790) generally regarded as foundational treatise in modern philosophical aesthetics no integration of aesthetic theory into

More information

GLOSSARY for National Core Arts: Visual Arts STANDARDS

GLOSSARY for National Core Arts: Visual Arts STANDARDS GLOSSARY for National Core Arts: Visual Arts STANDARDS Visual Arts, as defined by the National Art Education Association, include the traditional fine arts, such as, drawing, painting, printmaking, photography,

More information

Effects of Musical Training on Key and Harmony Perception

Effects of Musical Training on Key and Harmony Perception THE NEUROSCIENCES AND MUSIC III DISORDERS AND PLASTICITY Effects of Musical Training on Key and Harmony Perception Kathleen A. Corrigall a and Laurel J. Trainor a,b a Department of Psychology, Neuroscience,

More information

National Standards for Visual Art The National Standards for Arts Education

National Standards for Visual Art The National Standards for Arts Education National Standards for Visual Art The National Standards for Arts Education Developed by the Consortium of National Arts Education Associations (under the guidance of the National Committee for Standards

More information

J. Anat. (2010) 216, pp doi: /j x

J. Anat. (2010) 216, pp doi: /j x Journal of Anatomy J. Anat. (2010) 216, pp184 191 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01164.x REVIEW Beauty and the brain: culture, history and individual differences in aesthetic appreciation Thomas Jacobsen

More information

iafor The International Academic Forum

iafor The International Academic Forum A Study on the Core Concepts of Environmental Aesthetics Curriculum Ya-Ting Lee, National Pingtung University, Taiwan The Asian Conference on Arts and Humanities 2017 Official Conference Proceedings Abstract

More information