Contextual modulation of N400 amplitude to lexically ambiguous words

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Contextual modulation of N400 amplitude to lexically ambiguous words"

Transcription

1 Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) Contextual modulation of N400 amplitude to lexically ambiguous words Debra A. Titone a, * and Dean F. Salisbury b a Department of Psychology, McGill University, Stewart Biological Sciences Building, 1205 Dr. Penfield Ave., Montreal, Que., Canada H3A 1B1 b Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, McLean Hospital, USA Accepted 12 February 2004 Available online 12 April 2004 Abstract Though much is known about the N400 component, an event-related EEG potential that is sensitive to semantic manipulations, it is unclear whether modulations of N400 amplitude reflect automatic processing, controlled processing, or both. We examined this issue using a semantic judgment task that manipulated local and global contextual cues. Word triplets (prime noun target, e.g., finance bank money) were sequentially presented on a computer screen (500 ms duration, 1000 ms stimulus onset asynchrony), in which the second word was a homograph. The first word (prime) created a neutral-, dominant-meaning- or subordinate-meaningbiased global context, and the third word (target) created a dominant- or subordinate-biased local context that was either congruent or incongruent with the global context established by the first prime word. Participants were instructed to read all three words but to decide only whether the second and third words were semantically related. Event-related potentials (ERPs), specifically the N400, were recorded to the third terminal word. N400 amplitudes evoked by dominant meaning-related third words incongruent with the globally biased subordinate context (e.g., river bridge money) were significantly more negative than dominant endings in neutral contexts (e.g., taxi bank money), but not different from unrelated filler triplets. In addition, there was some indication that left hemisphere, temporal parietal electrode sites were associated with greater N400 negativity for dominant targets in conflicting subordinate global contexts than homologous right hemisphere electrode sites, the latter of which showed significant activation to subordinate meanings in cooperating contexts. Thus, N400 amplitude was more affected by global than local context suggesting that controlled processes may take priority over automatic processes in modulating N400 amplitude, especially for left hemisphere electrode sites. Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction The N400 is a negative-going potential that occurs approximately 400 ms following a semantically based stimulus event (reviewed in Kutas & Federmeier, 2000). Its amplitude is inversely related to the degree of semantic relatedness of a stimulus event (e.g., The man liked his coffee with dog elicits a larger (i.e., more negative) N400 amplitude than The man liked his coffee with sugar). Consequently, unrelated word pairs, incongruous sentence endings, and congruous but unexpected sentence endings all elicit larger N400 responses than, respectively, related word pairs, congruous sentence endings, and * Corresponding author. Fax: address: dtitone@psych.mcgill.ca (D.A. Titone). congruous but expected sentence endings. Modulations of N400 amplitude have also been found for pictures and faces (e.g., Debruille, Pineda, & Renault, 1996; Nigam, Hoffman, & Simons, 1992), which are semantically meaningful, or at the very least, verbally classifiable. Though much is known about the many linguistic variables that modulate N400 amplitude, its functional significance from a cognitive and psycholinguistic standpoint remains a matter of debate. Given the higher-order linguistic variables that affect N400, some researchers claim that modulations of N400 amplitude reflect sentence integration exclusively (e.g., van den Brink, Brown, & Hagoort, 2001). According to this view, experimental manipulations that modulate N400 amplitude may be classed as instances of post-lexical or controlled semantic processing. For example, using a /$ - see front matter Ó 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: /j.bandc

2 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) semantic priming paradigm, Holcomb (1993) found that changes in N400 amplitude were not coupled with changes in semantic priming RT effects for perceptually degraded targets, which would have been predicted by automatic accounts of N400, given that stimulus degradation is thought to affect lexical selection. Thus, N400 amplitude was argued to reflect contextual integration after lexical selection had taken place. In contrast, other researchers suggest that automatic semantic processing, in addition to controlled semantic processing, modulates N400 amplitude. For example, Deacon, Hewitt, Yang, and Nagata (2000) found smaller N400 amplitudes to semantically related word pairs when the first word was not consciously perceived, and presumably, not subject to controlled semantic processing. In an earlier study, Deacon, Uhm, Ritter, Hewitt, and Dynowska (1999) found that automatic and expectancy-based priming effects on N400 amplitude corresponded to the pattern of behavioral results from the classic Neely (1977) experiment. As part of this experiment, participants are instructed to expect members of one category (e.g., types of furniture) when they encounter members of another category (e.g., body parts). Thus, automatic and expectancy-based cues can be put into conflict on low frequency switch trials where an unexpected semantic associate follows the prime (e.g., arm leg). Automatic priming effects alone were obtained at a short stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA 250 ms), whereas both automatic and expectancy-based priming effects on N400 amplitude were obtained at a longer SOA (2000 ms). Thus, a conservative view of the N400, consistent with the conclusions of Kutas and Federmeier (2000), is that automatic and controlled semantic processes jointly affect N400 amplitude, although they may follow different time courses or be differentially weighted as a function of the specific comprehension problem to be solved. It may be argued that the automatic and expectancybased cues employed in Deacon et al. (1999) and in the original Neely (1977) experiment can be recast as local and global contextual cues, respectively. Thus, the local context corresponds to the actual word that is encountered, whereas the global context corresponds to the task instruction whereby participants come to expect exemplars of one category to follow exemplars of another. Although these specific local and global contextual cues are somewhat artificial, language users routinely encounter more naturally occurring local and global contextual cues during comprehension that may affect N400 amplitude, and thus shed light on its functional significance. In particular, several studies indicate that global context reduces (i.e., makes more positive) the N400 potential that would otherwise be evoked by particular sentences. In a discourse processing experiment discussed by Kutas and Federmeier (2000), for example, van Berkum, Hagoort, and Brown (1999) found a smaller N400 response to words in local sentence contexts as a function of the global discourse context in which a sentence occurred. In that study, the words quickly and slowly elicit identical N400 responses in the sentence frame The mouse quickly/slowly returned to its hole. However, quickly elicits a more positive-going N400 when the sentence is preceded by the sentence The cat entered the room suddenly startling a mouse which had found a bit of cheese in the corner, suggesting that it had been primed by the global context. Thus, global context appears to reduce (i.e., make more positive) N400 amplitude over and above that of a wordõs local contextual environment. In another experiment that examined healthy participants (and schizophrenia patients), Sitnikova, Salisbury, Kuperberg, and Holcomb (2002) found effects of local vs. global context on N400 amplitude for ambiguous words (e.g., bridge) in sentences for healthy controls (but interestingly, not for schizophrenia patients; see also Titone, Levy, & Holzman, 2000, for a similar pattern of behavioral data). Here, sentence constraint (the global context) was put into conflict with the inherent semantic relationship between two words in the sentence (the local context, e.g., The guest played bridge because the river had rocks in it.) The results showed that the N400 evoked by the word river was more negative when the sentence context biased bridgeõs subordinate meaning (i.e., card game) than when it biased its dominant meaning (i.e., structure spanning water) (see also, Van Petten & Kutas, 1987). However, because this study did not include a neutral baseline condition where the global context neither cooperated nor conflicted with the local word-level relationship, it is unclear whether contextual modulation of N400 amplitude was facilitory or inhibitory in nature. This issue is important because it addresses the potential roles of automatic and controlled processing in modulating N400 amplitude. The purpose of the present study is to further examine the simultaneous effects of local and global context on N400 amplitude to determine whether the N400 potential evoked by semantically related words can be enhanced or inhibited by a cooperating or conflicting global context. Similar to Sitnikova et al. (2002), the present study examined ambiguous words in dominant or subordinate biased contexts. Unlike Sitnikova et al. (2002), we examined the effects of cooperating and conflicting contexts for isolated words with respect to a neutral baseline condition to determine whether global context would increase or reduce N400 amplitude. Specifically, ambiguous homographs such as bank were embedded in word triplets where the first word was either neutral with respect to the homographsõ dominant and subordinate meanings, biased toward dominant meanings, or biased toward subordinate meanings. The homographs comprised the second word of each triplet,

3 472 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) and the final target word was related to either the dominant or subordinate meaning (e.g., finance bank money). Thus, we will examine facilitation of the N400 effect to a global cooperating context (i.e., a more positive component than that found for a neutral context), and/or inhibition to a global conflicting context (i.e., a more negative component than that found for a neutral context) for the same two semantically related words, which themselves comprise the local context. We assume that automatic processing will be manifested by an effect of local context (i.e., the relationship between the second and third words) that resists any global contextual influence, whereas controlled processing will be manifested by an effect of global context that directly affects processing of the semantic relationship between the second and third words, and the N400 response to those two words. Further, the pattern of data for conflicting global contexts (e.g., river bank money) is likely to be most important for evaluating the role of automatic and controlled processes in modulating N400 amplitude, given that automatic and controlled processing accounts do not differ substantially in their predictions for cooperating contexts (e.g., river bank muddy). That is, both accounts would be consistent with a facilitory effect of the cooperating context, or no difference between the cooperating and neutral contexts (assuming that a particular meaning was maximally activated in the neutral context). Thus, the absence of decreased N400 amplitude to the third word in a conflicting global context, and the presence of increased N400 amplitude to the same word relative to the unrelated condition, would suggest that the local semantic relationship between the final two words alone modulates N400 amplitude. This pattern of data (i.e., cooperating context P neutral context ¼ conflicting context > unrelated condition) would attribute priority to automatic processes in modulating N400 amplitude. In contrast, the presence of decreased N400 amplitude to the third word in a conflicting global context relative to a neutral context, but no difference in N400 amplitude to the same third word relative to an unrelated condition, would suggest that the global context provided by the first word modulates N400 amplitude over and above the automatic processing of the local context (i.e., the inherent semantic relationship between the final two words). This pattern (i.e., cooperating context P neutral context > conflicting context ¼ unrelated condition) would attribute priority to controlled processes in modulating N400 amplitude. Finally, decreased N400 amplitude to the third word in a conflicting global context relative to a neutral context, but increased N400 amplitude to the same third word relative to the unrelated condition, would suggest that both the local semantic relationship between the final two words and the global context provided by the first word modulates N400 amplitude. This pattern (i.e., cooperating context P neutral context > conflicting context > unrelated condition) would support the view that both automatic and controlled processes modulate N400 amplitude. 2. Methods 2.1. Participants Twenty-four right-handed men and women were recruited through newspaper advertisements from the local community. All participants were native English speakers. None of the participants had a history of drug dependence, neurological disease or trauma, dementia, delirium, psychopathology, and any immediate family history of psychopathology (by self-report). All subjects gave their informed consent Stimuli Word triplets requiring a yes response consisted of 126 homographs preceded by neutral, dominant- or subordinate-biased context words, and followed by dominant- or subordinate-biased words (respectively, taxi bank money, finance bank money, river bank money, taxi bank muddy, finance bank muddy, river bank muddy). All homographs had clear dominant and subordinate meanings as determined by a greater than 1.5 ratio of dominant to subordinate responses from a large corpus of free association norms (Nelson, McEvoy, Walling, & Wheeler, 1980). Semantically related context and target words were selected also using these norms. Thus, two factors were crossed in constructing each yes triplet; target type (i.e., the third word which was either dominant, subordinate) and context type (i.e., the first word which was either cooperating, neutral, conflicting with respect to the semantic relationship emphasized by the final two words). The 126 homographs were counterbalanced across six stimulus lists so that by the end of the experiment, every homograph had an equal chance of participating in each condition, and all participants received an equal number of items per condition ðn ¼ 21Þ, without encountering repetitions of any one homograph. Word triplets requiring a no response were also presented, and these triplets were matched in their relational structure to the yes triplets with the exception of the final two words on which the semantic judgment decision was based. Although we collapsed across all no triplets in the final analysis, it was important to create these different categories of no triplets so that participants would not develop any response strategies (e.g., say yes whenever the first two words are related). Consider first the relational structure for the

4 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) cooperating context triplets (e.g., river bank muddy). Here, the first word is related to the second and third words, and the second word is related to the third word. Thus, in the corresponding no item, we created triplets where all the same relationships existed except the relationship between the second and third word (e.g., shed hair roof i.e., shed is a homograph that is related to both hair and roof, but hair and roof are not themselves related). Consider next the relational structure for the conflicting context triplets (e.g., river bank money). Here, the first word is related to the second word, and the second word is related to the third word, but the first and third words are unrelated. Thus, in the corresponding no item, we created triplets where the same relationship existed between the first and second words, but where the relationship between the second and third words was eliminated (e.g., salt pepper book). Finally, consider the relational structure for the neutral triplets in which only the final two words of each triplet were related (e.g., taxi bank money). Thus, in the corresponding no item, none of the three words were semantically related (e.g., dog table radio). There were 21 distinct instances of each no triplet condition Experimental procedure and analytic technique Words from each triplet were presented sequentially on a computer screen. Each trial sequence was as follows: first word duration 500 ms, 500 ms delay, second word duration 500 ms, 500 ms delay, third word duration 2500 ms (during which time a response occurred). The inter-trial interval was 500 ms. Participants were instructed to indicate as quickly and accurately as possible whether the final two words of the triplet were related by making a right thumb press for a yes response or a left thumb press for a no response. Participants sat approximately 1 m from the computer screen. Individual letters comprising the words were presented in lowercase font and 5 cm high. The entire list of words was between 6 and 12 cm wide. EEG activity was recorded from the scalp through 28 tin electrodes arranged in pre-configured caps (Electro- Cap International). Linked-earlobe electrodes served as the reference, and a forehead electrode served as the ground. Bipolar electrodes positioned medial to the right eye, one above and one below, were used to monitor vertical eye movements and blinks. Bipolar electrodes placed at the outer canthi of the eyes were used to monitor horizontal eye movements. All electrode impedances were below 3 kx, and the ears were matched within 1 kx. The EEG amplifier bandpass was 0.15 (6 db/octave rolloff) to 40 Hz (36 db/octave rolloff). Single trial epochs were digitized at 3.9 ms/sample (256 Hz). Each epoch was of 1100 ms duration, including a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline. Averaging and artifact rejection were performed off-line. ERP responses were digitally low-pass filtered at 8.5 Hz with a 24 db/ octave rolloff to remove ambient electrical noise, muscle artifact, and alpha contamination. Epochs from each electrode site were baseline corrected by subtraction of the average pre-stimulus voltage, and corrected for eye movement artifact using regression-based weighting coefficients. Trials were baseline corrected after eyecorrection. Epochs that contained voltage levels exceeding 75 lv at F7, F8, Fp1, or Fp2 were rejected. Averages were computed for the final word of each yes triplet and for the final word of all unrelated noõ triplets. N400 amplitude was measured over a 50 ms window centered about 400 ms post-stimulus onset (i.e., ms), which reflected the peak N400 latency for all conditions at Cz. Since electric fields arise simultaneously across the scalp, using the static interval centered about N400 peak latency assures that the same underlying cortical phenomena is measured at each site. Repeated measures ANOVAs on amplitudes of the N400 were computed separately for the midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, and Pz) and temporal parietal electrode sites (TCP1 and TCP2, corresponding to WernickeÕs left and right), with target type (dominant, subordinate; i.e., the final word) and context type (cooperating, neutral, conflicting, unrelated; i.e., the first word) as within subject factors Results All behavioral and N400 amplitude data are presented in Table 1. Grand average waveforms for the tripletsõ final words are presented in Figs. 1A and B. Note that three subjectsõ data were discarded due to equipment failure Semantic judgment responses Semantic judgment accuracy and response latencies of correct responses were analyzed using two separate 2 (Target type: dominant, subordinate) 4 (Context type: cooperating, neutral, conflicting, unrelated) repeated measure ANOVAs. Accuracy of semantic judgment responses varied as a function of both target type and context. Cooperating contexts failed to significantly improve semantic judgment accuracy for dominant targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼ 3:1, p ¼ :08) and subordinate targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼ 2:4, p ¼ :13). However, conflicting contexts significantly impaired semantic judgment accuracy for dominant targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼7:0, p <:01) and subordinate targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼25:6, p <:01) relative to the neutral context condition. Further, a significant interaction between target type and context indicated that conflicting contexts were more detrimental for subordinate targets than for dominant targets (F ð3; 60Þ ¼9:1, p <:01).

5 474 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) Table 1 Summary of behavioral and electrophysiological data DOM targets SUB targets UNREL targets Mean SEM Mean SEM Mean SEM Semantic judgment responses to final two words Latency (ms) Cooperating context Neutral context Conflicting context Facilitation (Neut-Coop) )1 57 Inhibition (Neut-Confl) )48 )85 Percent correct Cooperating context Neutral context Conflicting context Facilitation (Neut-Coop) )3.63 )3.18 Inhibition (Neut-Confl) N400 amplitude (microvolts) to final word Midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz) ) Cooperating context Neutral context ) Conflicting context ) ) Facilitation (Neut-Coop) )0.47 )1.19 Inhibition (Neut-Confl) 1.72 )0.65 TCP1 (LH) Cooperating context Neutral context Conflicting context ) Facilitation (Neut-Coop) )0.57 )0.71 Inhibition (Neut-Confl) 1.49 )0.04 TCP2 (RH) Cooperating context Neutral context Conflicting context ) Facilitation (Neut-Coop) )0.44 )0.88 Inhibition (Neut-Confl) * p <:05. ** p <:01. Latency of correct semantic judgment responses also varied as a function of target type and context. Cooperating contexts failed to significantly speed correct judgment responses for dominant targets relative to the neutral context condition (F ð1; 20Þ < 1). However, cooperating contexts did significantly speed responses for subordinate targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼8:9, p <:01). Conflicting contexts slowed correct responses relative to the neutral context condition for both dominant and subordinate targets (respectively, F ð1; 20Þ ¼6:5, p <:05; F ð1; 20Þ ¼20:3, p <:01). As in the accuracy data, there was a significant interaction between target type and context indicating that subordinate targets were differentially more affected by conflicting contexts than dominant targets relative to the neutral context condition (F ð3; 60Þ ¼17:1, p <:01) N400 amplitude Midline electrode sites (Fz, Cz, Pz) N400 amplitudes to correct responses were analyzed using a 2 (Target type: dominant, subordinate) 4 (Context type: cooperating, neutral, conflicting, unrelated) 3 (Electrode site: Fz, Cz, Pz) repeated measures ANOVA. N400 amplitude varied as a function of all three independent variables. A main effect of target type indicated that dominant targets had significantly more positive N400 amplitude than subordinate targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼6:7, p <:05). A main effect of electrode site indicated that N400 amplitude was more positive-going for all stimuli at posterior electrode sites than at frontal electrode sites (Fz < Cz < Pz) (F ð2; 40Þ ¼9:3, p <:01). A

6 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) Fig. 1. (A) Waveforms for event-related potentials evoked by dominant-related targets as a function of each context type (i.e., cooperating, neutral, and conflicting) and the final word of unrelated control triplets at different electrode sites. (B) Waveforms for event-related potentials evoked by subordinate-related targets as a function of each context type (i.e., cooperating, neutral, and conflicting) and the final word of unrelated control triplets at different electrode sites. main effect of context type indicated that the global context created by the first word of each triplet influenced the N400 amplitude evoked by the third word. However, this main effect was qualified by a marginal interaction between target type and context (F ð3; 60Þ ¼2:5, p ¼ :07). Cooperating context relative to the neutral context condition did not alter N400 amplitude for dominant targets (F ð1; 20Þ < 1) or subordinate targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼ 2:4, p ¼ :12). In contrast, conflicting context relative to the neutral context condition were associated with more negative-going N400 amplitude for dominant targets (F ð1; 20Þ ¼5:1, p <:05) but not for subordinate targets (F ð1; 20Þ < 1) (i.e., the first word inducing the dominant meaning when the target is related to the subordinate

7 476 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) meaning). This differential effect of conflicting contexts for the two target types was also evident in a marginal interaction between target type and context (F ð3; 60Þ ¼ 2:5, p ¼ :07). Conflicting contexts did not differ from the unrelated condition for either target type (dominant and subordinate targets, respectively, F ð1; 20Þ ¼1:1, p ¼ :30; F ð1; 20Þ ¼1:0, p ¼ :31) Temporal parietal electrode sites (TCP1, TCP2) N400 amplitude to correct responses was analyzed using a 2 (Target type: dominant, subordinate) 4 (Context type: cooperating, neutral, conflicting, unrelated) 2 (Electrode site: TCP1, TCP2) repeated measures ANOVA. In this analysis, we were particularly interested in whether hemispheric differences in electrode site emerged as a function of context. The only significant effect in the omnibus ANOVA was a main effect of context (F ð3; 60Þ ¼3:4, p <:05) indicating that N400 amplitude was more negative-going for the conflicting context and unrelated condition than for the cooperating context condition. However, planned comparisons between the context and target conditions as a function of electrode site (i.e., left vs. right hemisphere) revealed that contextual facilitation and inhibition differed as a function target type. Cooperating contexts for dominant targets did not affect N400 responses relative to the neutral context condition at either electrode site (TCP1, F ð1; 20Þ ¼1:6, p ¼ :22; TCP2, F ð1; 20Þ < 1). In contrast, cooperating contexts for subordinate targets were associated with more positive-going N400 amplitude relative to the neutral context condition, especially at the right hemisphere electrode site (TCP1, F ð1; 20Þ ¼ 3:0, p ¼ :09; TCP2, F ð1; 20Þ ¼4:6, p <:05) In contrast with cooperating contexts, conflicting contexts for dominant meanings were associated with more negative-going N400 amplitude relative to the neutral context condition for both the left hemisphere and right hemisphere electrode site (TCP1, F ð1; 20Þ ¼10:8, p <:01; TCP2, F ð1; 20Þ ¼5:7, p <:05). Interestingly, a direct comparison of the conflicting context conditions for left and right hemisphere electrode sites revealed more negative-going N400 amplitude for the left hemisphere than for the right hemisphere (F ð1; 20Þ ¼4:5, p <:05). Conflicting contexts failed to affect N400 amplitude relative to the neutral condition for subordinate targets ðf < 1Þ. Note that these hemispheric effects were only found in the planned comparisons, thus their interpretation should be viewed with caution. 3. Discussion The purpose of the study was to examine whether N400 amplitude is jointly modulated by global and local contextual cues in an overt semantic judgment task where participants were instructed to attend to local contextual cues. The results confirmed these predictions in revealing a complex interaction of local and global contextual effects on N400 amplitude. First, modulations of N400 amplitude were only found for triplets containing dominant targets. None of the triplets containing subordinate targets produced N400 amplitudes that were more positive than the unrelated triplet condition, with the one exception of subordinate targets in cooperating contexts at right hemisphere, temporal parietal electrode sites. This suggests that dominant meanings have a clear priority over subordinate meanings with respect to N400 modulation, even for contexts that globally bias subordinate meanings (see also Salisbury, Shenton, Nestor, & McCarley, 2002). The association of right hemisphere temporal parietal electrode sites with more positive-going N400 amplitudes for cooperating contexts is consistent with other work suggesting a specific right hemisphere involvement in generating and maintaining activation of peripheral word meanings (e.g., Atchley, Burgess, & Keeney, 1999; Beeman, 1993; Brownell, 1988; Chiarello, 1991; Federmeier & Kutas, 1999; Titone, 1998), although given that this effect was only found in planned comparisons, further work will be necessary to substantiate this interpretation of the data. Second, with respect to triplets containing dominant targets, only neutral and cooperating contexts produced significantly smaller N400 amplitudes than the unrelated condition. Specifically, conflicting context triplets containing dominant targets (e.g., river bank money) evoked more negative-going N400 amplitudes than neutral context triplets sharing the same local dominant context (e.g., taxi bank money.) Moreover, N400 amplitude for these conflicting triplets did not differ from that found for no triplets where the second and third words were unrelated. Further, N400 amplitude for conflicting triplets appeared to be more negative at left hemisphere temporal parietal electrode sites (TCP1) than at right hemisphere sites (TCP2), consistent with previous work suggesting a specific left hemisphere involvement in lexical selection or inhibition, and semantic integration (e.g., Atchley et al., 1999; Beeman, 1993; Brownell, 1988; Chiarello, 1991; Federmeier & Kutas, 1999; Titone, 1998). Note that this hemispheric effect is also preliminary in that it was only statistically substantiated by planned comparisons and not the overall ANOVA. In contrast to the conflicting context triplets, the cooperating context triplets (e.g., finance bank money) did not differ in N400 amplitude from the neutral context triplets (e.g., taxi bank money). Taken together, these data suggest that global context trumps N400 priming normally evoked by a local context consisting of two semantically related words. These data also suggest that the dominant meaning was maximally activated in the neutral context condition given that

8 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) there was no increase in N400 amplitude in the cooperating context condition. The N400 results for dominant targets are consistent with the semantic judgment accuracy and latency data, although the behavioral results for subordinate targets show clear effects of context that are not found in the N400 data. In particular, conflicting contexts were associated with reduced accuracy and longer latencies of semantic judgment responses compared to the neutral context for triplets containing both dominant and subordinate targets. Cooperating contexts, on the other hand, produced a different pattern of results for dominant and subordinate targets. For dominant targets, cooperating contexts were associated with increased accuracy but no difference in latency compared to neutral contexts. For subordinate targets, cooperating contexts were associated with shorter latencies but no difference in accuracy compared to neutral contexts. In addition, semantic judgment responses to dominant targets were more accurate and faster than responses to subordinate targets, consistent with the behavioral literature on lexical ambiguity resolution (for comprehensive reviews, see Klepousniotou, 2002; Titone, 1998; Van Petten, 2002). This is exemplified by the finding of comparable accuracy for dominant meanings in conflicting contexts and subordinate meanings in cooperating contexts (see Table 1). N400 amplitude was also significantly smaller for dominant targets in neutral contexts than for subordinate targets in neutral contexts. The overall pattern of results is consistent with the view that N400 amplitude is more affected by global than by local semantic context, lending support to the notion that controlled semantic processes exert a greater influence on N400 amplitude than automatic semantic processes. We arrive at this conclusion for three main reasons. First, N400 amplitude to dominant targets in conflicting contexts was significantly larger (i.e., more negative) than N400 amplitude to dominant targets in neutral contexts. Second, N400 amplitude to dominant targets in conflicting contexts was no different from N400 amplitude of the unrelated no triplets. Finally, the task was inherently biased to produce an effect of local context because participants were instructed to focus their attention on the local semantic relationship between the tripletsõ final two words. If automatic semantic processes partially drove N400 amplitude, the conflicting context triplets would have shown smaller (i.e., more positive) N400 amplitudes relative to the unrelated condition, especially given that these triplets contained two semantically related words upon which explicit attention was directed for a semantic judgment response. It is possible that modifications of the experimental paradigm would evoke smaller N400 amplitudes for dominant targets in conflicting contexts than the unrelated condition, and thus an N400 effect for local contexts. First, the use of shorter SOAs than what was employed here (1000 ms) might be more conducive to observing an effect of local context, and thus, automatic processing on N400 amplitudes. However, we are confident that the 1000 ms SOA used here could have detected an effect of automatic processing on N400 amplitude given that Deacon et al. (1999) found automatic processing effects in a slightly different paradigm using an SOA of 2000 ms. Second, the semantic judgment task may have caused participants to encode the context word of each triplet more deeply than what would normally occur during passive reading, thus inducing an overall controlled processing strategy. Although given that participants were explicitly instructed to attend to the local context (i.e., the final two words), this feature of the task might also account for the data that were observed. Third, the use of single words rather than sentence contexts may increase the likelihood that these effects are occurring intra-lexically rather than semantically (e.g., Seidenberg, Tanenhaus, Leiman, & Bienkowski, 1982). For these reasons, future work will need to determine which of these and other potential boundary conditions are important factors in the modulation of N400 amplitude, or perhaps different subcomponents of the N400 potential (Kutas & Federmeier, 2000). Acknowledgments This study was supported in part by grants from NSERC, CRC, CFI, NIMH (R03 MH60272), and a NARSAD Young Investigator Award to D. Titone, and grants NIMH (RO158704), and a NARSAD Young Investigator Award to D. Salisbury. The authors are grateful to the participants, and also to Tali Ditman, Nicola Sumorok, and Margaret Fagan for assistance with participant testing and data analysis. References Atchley, R. A., Burgess, C., & Keeney, M. (1999). The effect of time course and context on the facilitation of semantic features in the cerebral hemispheres. Neuropsychology, 13, Beeman, M. (1993). Semantic processing in the right hemisphere may contribute to drawing inferences from discourse. Brain and Language, 44, Brownell, H. H. (1988). Appreciation of metaphoric and connotative word meaning in brain damaged patients. In C. Chiarello (Ed.), Right hemisphere contributions to lexical semantics. New York: Springer-Verlag. Chiarello, C. (1991). Interpretation of word meanings by the cerebral hemispheres: One is not enough. In P. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings. LEA Publishers. Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., Yang, C., & Nagata, M. (2000). Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: Evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. Cognitive Brain Research, 9,

9 478 D.A. Titone, D.F. Salisbury / Brain and Cognition 55 (2004) Deacon, D., Uhm, T. J., Ritter, W., Hewitt, S., & Dynowska, A. (1999). The lifetime of automatic semantic priming effects may exceed two seconds. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, Debruille, J. B., Pineda, J., & Renault, B. (1996). N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition. Cognitive Brain Research, 4, Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). Right words and left words: Electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 8, Holcomb, P. (1993). Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: Implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. Psychophysiology, 30, Klepousniotou, E. (2002). The processing of lexical ambiguity: Homonymy and polysemy in the mental lexicon. Brain and Language, 81, Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limitedcapacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., Walling, J. R., & Wheeler, J. W. (1980). The University of South Florida homograph norms. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 12, Nigam, A., Hoffman, J. E., & Simons, R. F. (1992). N400 to semantically anomalous pictures and words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, Salisbury, D., Shenton, M. E., Nestor, P. G., & McCarley, R. W. (2002). Semantic bias, homograph comprehension, and eventrelated potentials in schizophrenia. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, Seidenberg, M. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Leiman, J. M., & Bienkowski, M. (1982). Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations of knowledge based processing. Cognitive Psychology, 14, Sitnikova, T., Salisbury, D. F., Kuperberg, G., & Holcomb, P. I. (2002). Electrophysiological insights into language processing in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology, 39, Titone, D. (1998). Hemispheric differences in context sensitivity during lexical ambiguity resolution. Brain and Language, 65, Titone, D., Levy, D., & Holzman, P. S. (2000). Contextual insensitivity in schizophrenic language processing: Evidence from lexical ambiguity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, van Berkum, J. J., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C. M. (1999). Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: Evidence from the N400. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, van den Brink, D., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (2001). Electrophysiological evidence for early contextual influences during spokenword recognition: N200 versus N400 effects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, Van Petten, C. (2002). Lexical ambiguity resolution. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science (pp ). London: Macmillan. Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1987). Ambiguous words in context: An event-related potential analysis of the time course of meaning activation. Journal of Memory and Language, 26,

Non-native Homonym Processing: an ERP Measurement

Non-native Homonym Processing: an ERP Measurement Non-native Homonym Processing: an ERP Measurement Jiehui Hu ab, Wenpeng Zhang a, Chen Zhao a, Weiyi Ma ab, Yongxiu Lai b, Dezhong Yao b a School of Foreign Languages, University of Electronic Science &

More information

23/01/51. Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the. VEP waveform. How are ERP related to gender? Event-Related Potential (ERP)

23/01/51. Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the. VEP waveform. How are ERP related to gender? Event-Related Potential (ERP) 23/01/51 EventRelated Potential (ERP) Genderselective effects of the and N400 components of the visual evoked potential measuring brain s electrical activity (EEG) responded to external stimuli EEG averaging

More information

Individual differences in prediction: An investigation of the N400 in word-pair semantic priming

Individual differences in prediction: An investigation of the N400 in word-pair semantic priming Individual differences in prediction: An investigation of the N400 in word-pair semantic priming Xiao Yang & Lauren Covey Cognitive and Brain Sciences Brown Bag Talk October 17, 2016 Caitlin Coughlin,

More information

The Influence of Explicit Markers on Slow Cortical Potentials During Figurative Language Processing

The Influence of Explicit Markers on Slow Cortical Potentials During Figurative Language Processing The Influence of Explicit Markers on Slow Cortical Potentials During Figurative Language Processing Christopher A. Schwint (schw6620@wlu.ca) Department of Psychology, Wilfrid Laurier University 75 University

More information

Grand Rounds 5/15/2012

Grand Rounds 5/15/2012 Grand Rounds 5/15/2012 Department of Neurology P Dr. John Shelley-Tremblay, USA Psychology P I have no financial disclosures P I discuss no medications nore off-label uses of medications An Introduction

More information

Semantic bias, homograph comprehension, and event-related potentials in schizophrenia

Semantic bias, homograph comprehension, and event-related potentials in schizophrenia Clinical Neurophysiology 113 (2002) 383 395 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Semantic bias, homograph comprehension, and event-related potentials in schizophrenia Dean F. Salisbury a,b, *, Martha E. Shenton

More information

Information processing in high- and low-risk parents: What can we learn from EEG?

Information processing in high- and low-risk parents: What can we learn from EEG? Information processing in high- and low-risk parents: What can we learn from EEG? Social Information Processing What differentiates parents who abuse their children from parents who don t? Mandy M. Rabenhorst

More information

The N400 and Late Positive Complex (LPC) Effects Reflect Controlled Rather than Automatic Mechanisms of Sentence Processing

The N400 and Late Positive Complex (LPC) Effects Reflect Controlled Rather than Automatic Mechanisms of Sentence Processing Brain Sci. 2012, 2, 267-297; doi:10.3390/brainsci2030267 Article OPEN ACCESS brain sciences ISSN 2076-3425 www.mdpi.com/journal/brainsci/ The N400 and Late Positive Complex (LPC) Effects Reflect Controlled

More information

MEANING RELATEDNESS IN POLYSEMOUS AND HOMONYMOUS WORDS: AN ERP STUDY IN RUSSIAN

MEANING RELATEDNESS IN POLYSEMOUS AND HOMONYMOUS WORDS: AN ERP STUDY IN RUSSIAN Anna Yurchenko, Anastasiya Lopukhina, Olga Dragoy MEANING RELATEDNESS IN POLYSEMOUS AND HOMONYMOUS WORDS: AN ERP STUDY IN RUSSIAN BASIC RESEARCH PROGRAM WORKING PAPERS SERIES: LINGUISTICS WP BRP 67/LNG/2018

More information

How Order of Label Presentation Impacts Semantic Processing: an ERP Study

How Order of Label Presentation Impacts Semantic Processing: an ERP Study How Order of Label Presentation Impacts Semantic Processing: an ERP Study Jelena Batinić (jelenabatinic1@gmail.com) Laboratory for Neurocognition and Applied Cognition, Department of Psychology, Faculty

More information

Semantic integration in videos of real-world events: An electrophysiological investigation

Semantic integration in videos of real-world events: An electrophysiological investigation Semantic integration in videos of real-world events: An electrophysiological investigation TATIANA SITNIKOVA a, GINA KUPERBERG bc, and PHILLIP J. HOLCOMB a a Department of Psychology, Tufts University,

More information

Right Hemisphere Sensitivity to Word and Sentence Level Context: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials. Seana Coulson, UCSD

Right Hemisphere Sensitivity to Word and Sentence Level Context: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials. Seana Coulson, UCSD Right Hemisphere Sensitivity to Word and Sentence Level Context: Evidence from Event-Related Brain Potentials Seana Coulson, UCSD Kara D. Federmeier, University of Illinois Cyma Van Petten, University

More information

Event-Related Brain Potentials Reflect Semantic Priming in an Object Decision Task

Event-Related Brain Potentials Reflect Semantic Priming in an Object Decision Task BRAIN AND COGNITION 24, 259-276 (1994) Event-Related Brain Potentials Reflect Semantic Priming in an Object Decision Task PHILLIP.1. HOLCOMB AND WARREN B. MCPHERSON Tufts University Subjects made speeded

More information

Cross-modal Semantic Priming: A Timecourse Analysis Using Event-related Brain Potentials

Cross-modal Semantic Priming: A Timecourse Analysis Using Event-related Brain Potentials LANGUAGE AND COGNITIVE PROCESSES, 1993, 8 (4) 379-411 Cross-modal Semantic Priming: A Timecourse Analysis Using Event-related Brain Potentials Phillip J. Holcomb and Jane E. Anderson Department of Psychology,

More information

Electrophysiological Evidence for Early Contextual Influences during Spoken-Word Recognition: N200 Versus N400 Effects

Electrophysiological Evidence for Early Contextual Influences during Spoken-Word Recognition: N200 Versus N400 Effects Electrophysiological Evidence for Early Contextual Influences during Spoken-Word Recognition: N200 Versus N400 Effects Daniëlle van den Brink, Colin M. Brown, and Peter Hagoort Abstract & An event-related

More information

Running head: RESOLUTION OF AMBIGUOUS CATEGORICAL ANAPHORS. The Contributions of Lexico-Semantic and Discourse Information to the Resolution of

Running head: RESOLUTION OF AMBIGUOUS CATEGORICAL ANAPHORS. The Contributions of Lexico-Semantic and Discourse Information to the Resolution of Anaphor Resolution and ERPs 1 Running head: RESOLUTION OF AMBIGUOUS CATEGORICAL ANAPHORS The Contributions of Lexico-Semantic and Discourse Information to the Resolution of Ambiguous Categorical Anaphors

More information

Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading

Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading Research Report Frequency and predictability effects on event-related potentials during reading Michael Dambacher a,, Reinhold Kliegl a, Markus Hofmann b, Arthur M. Jacobs b a Helmholtz Center for the

More information

Ellen F. Lau 1,2,3. Phillip J. Holcomb 2. Gina R. Kuperberg 1,2

Ellen F. Lau 1,2,3. Phillip J. Holcomb 2. Gina R. Kuperberg 1,2 DISSOCIATING N400 EFFECTS OF PREDICTION FROM ASSOCIATION IN SINGLE WORD CONTEXTS Ellen F. Lau 1,2,3 Phillip J. Holcomb 2 Gina R. Kuperberg 1,2 1 Athinoula C. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts

More information

Understanding words in sentence contexts: The time course of ambiguity resolution

Understanding words in sentence contexts: The time course of ambiguity resolution Brain and Language 86 (2003) 326 343 www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l Understanding words in sentence contexts: The time course of ambiguity resolution Tamara Swaab, a, * Colin Brown, b and Peter Hagoort b,c

More information

With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long!

With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long! Event Related Potentials (ERPs): A window onto the timing of cognition Kim Sweeney COGS1- Introduction to Cognitive Science November 19, 2009 With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long! Overview

More information

Event-Related Brain Potentials (ERPs) Elicited by Novel Stimuli during Sentence Processing

Event-Related Brain Potentials (ERPs) Elicited by Novel Stimuli during Sentence Processing Event-Related Brain Potentials (ERPs) Elicited by Novel Stimuli during Sentence Processing MARTA KUTAS AND STEVEN A. HILLYARD Department of Neurosciences School of Medicine University of California at

More information

Neural evidence for a single lexicogrammatical processing system. Jennifer Hughes

Neural evidence for a single lexicogrammatical processing system. Jennifer Hughes Neural evidence for a single lexicogrammatical processing system Jennifer Hughes j.j.hughes@lancaster.ac.uk Background Approaches to collocation Background Association measures Background EEG, ERPs, and

More information

Brain & Language 123 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Brain & Language. journal homepage:

Brain & Language 123 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Brain & Language. journal homepage: Brain & Language 123 (2012) 11 21 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Brain & Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l Not all ambiguous words are created equal: An EEG investigation

More information

Semantic priming modulates the N400, N300, and N400RP

Semantic priming modulates the N400, N300, and N400RP Clinical Neurophysiology 118 (2007) 1053 1068 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph Semantic priming modulates the N400, N300, and N400RP Michael S. Franklin a,b, *, Joseph Dien a,c, James H. Neely d, Elizabeth

More information

Processing new and repeated names: Effects of coreference on repetition priming with speech and fast RSVP

Processing new and repeated names: Effects of coreference on repetition priming with speech and fast RSVP BRES-35877; No. of pages: 13; 4C: 11 available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Processing new and repeated names: Effects of coreference on repetition priming

More information

Connecting sound to meaning. /kæt/

Connecting sound to meaning. /kæt/ Connecting sound to meaning /kæt/ Questions Where are lexical representations stored in the brain? How many lexicons? Lexical access Activation Competition Selection/Recognition TURN level of activation

More information

NeuroImage 61 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 61 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 61 (2012) 206 215 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg From N400 to N300: Variations in the timing of semantic processing

More information

Dissociating N400 Effects of Prediction from Association in Single-word Contexts

Dissociating N400 Effects of Prediction from Association in Single-word Contexts Dissociating N400 Effects of Prediction from Association in Single-word Contexts Ellen F. Lau 1,2,3, Phillip J. Holcomb 2, and Gina R. Kuperberg 1,2 Abstract When a word is preceded by a supportive context

More information

On the locus of the semantic satiation effect: Evidence from event-related brain potentials

On the locus of the semantic satiation effect: Evidence from event-related brain potentials Memory & Cognition 2000, 28 (8), 1366-1377 On the locus of the semantic satiation effect: Evidence from event-related brain potentials JOHN KOUNIOS University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

Two Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Semantic Integration during the Comprehension of Visual Real-world Events

Two Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Semantic Integration during the Comprehension of Visual Real-world Events Two Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Semantic Integration during the Comprehension of Visual Real-world Events Tatiana Sitnikova 1, Phillip J. Holcomb 2, Kristi A. Kiyonaga 3, and Gina R. Kuperberg 1,2 Abstract

More information

ARTICLE IN PRESS BRESC-40606; No. of pages: 18; 4C:

ARTICLE IN PRESS BRESC-40606; No. of pages: 18; 4C: BRESC-40606; No. of pages: 18; 4C: DTD 5 Cognitive Brain Research xx (2005) xxx xxx Research report The effects of prime visibility on ERP measures of masked priming Phillip J. Holcomb a, T, Lindsay Reder

More information

Dual-Coding, Context-Availability, and Concreteness Effects in Sentence Comprehension: An Electrophysiological Investigation

Dual-Coding, Context-Availability, and Concreteness Effects in Sentence Comprehension: An Electrophysiological Investigation Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1999, Vol. 25, No. 3,721-742 Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0278-7393/99/S3.00 Dual-Coding, Context-Availability,

More information

The Time Course of Orthographic and Phonological Code Activation Jonathan Grainger, 1 Kristi Kiyonaga, 2 and Phillip J. Holcomb 2

The Time Course of Orthographic and Phonological Code Activation Jonathan Grainger, 1 Kristi Kiyonaga, 2 and Phillip J. Holcomb 2 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Report The Time Course of Orthographic and Phonological Code Activation Jonathan Grainger, 1 Kristi Kiyonaga, 2 and Phillip J. Holcomb 2 1 CNRS and University of Provence,

More information

SITTING ON THE FENCE: AN EEG INVESTIGATION OF THE ROLE OF THE RIGHT HEMISPHERE IN METAPHOR PROCESSING

SITTING ON THE FENCE: AN EEG INVESTIGATION OF THE ROLE OF THE RIGHT HEMISPHERE IN METAPHOR PROCESSING SITTING ON THE FENCE: AN EEG INVESTIGATION OF THE ROLE OF THE RIGHT HEMISPHERE IN METAPHOR PROCESSING Jennifer A. E. Lines, Lucy J. MacGregor, Ekaterini Klepousniotou "I understand words, but I'm missing

More information

Pre-Processing of ERP Data. Peter J. Molfese, Ph.D. Yale University

Pre-Processing of ERP Data. Peter J. Molfese, Ph.D. Yale University Pre-Processing of ERP Data Peter J. Molfese, Ph.D. Yale University Before Statistical Analyses, Pre-Process the ERP data Planning Analyses Waveform Tools Types of Tools Filter Segmentation Visual Review

More information

This is a repository copy of Sustained meaning activation for polysemous but not homonymous words: Evidence from EEG.

This is a repository copy of Sustained meaning activation for polysemous but not homonymous words: Evidence from EEG. This is a repository copy of Sustained meaning activation for polysemous but not homonymous words: Evidence from EEG. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/83189/

More information

I like my coffee with cream and sugar. I like my coffee with cream and socks. I shaved off my mustache and beard. I shaved off my mustache and BEARD

I like my coffee with cream and sugar. I like my coffee with cream and socks. I shaved off my mustache and beard. I shaved off my mustache and BEARD I like my coffee with cream and sugar. I like my coffee with cream and socks I shaved off my mustache and beard. I shaved off my mustache and BEARD All turtles have four legs All turtles have four leg

More information

Semantic combinatorial processing of non-anomalous expressions

Semantic combinatorial processing of non-anomalous expressions *7. Manuscript Click here to view linked References Semantic combinatorial processing of non-anomalous expressions Nicola Molinaro 1, Manuel Carreiras 1,2,3 and Jon Andoni Duñabeitia 1! "#"$%&"'()*+&,+-.+/&0-&#01-2.20-%&"/'2-&'-3&$'-1*'1+%&40-0(.2'%&56'2-&

More information

ERP Assessment of Visual and Auditory Language Processing in Schizophrenia

ERP Assessment of Visual and Auditory Language Processing in Schizophrenia Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1997, Vol. 106, No. 1, 85-94 In the public domain ERP Assessment of Visual and Auditory Language Processing in Schizophrenia M. A. Niznikiewicz, B. F. O'Donnell, P. G. Nestor,

More information

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring 2009 Week 6 Class Notes Pitch Perception Introduction Pitch may be described as that attribute of auditory sensation in terms

More information

DATA! NOW WHAT? Preparing your ERP data for analysis

DATA! NOW WHAT? Preparing your ERP data for analysis DATA! NOW WHAT? Preparing your ERP data for analysis Dennis L. Molfese, Ph.D. Caitlin M. Hudac, B.A. Developmental Brain Lab University of Nebraska-Lincoln 1 Agenda Pre-processing Preparing for analysis

More information

Auditory semantic networks for words and natural sounds

Auditory semantic networks for words and natural sounds available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Auditory semantic networks for words and natural sounds A. Cummings a,b,c,,r.čeponienė a, A. Koyama a, A.P. Saygin c,f,

More information

Neuroscience Letters

Neuroscience Letters Neuroscience Letters 530 (2012) 138 143 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuroscience Letters j our nal ho me p ag e: www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet Event-related brain potentials of

More information

Neuroscience Letters

Neuroscience Letters Neuroscience Letters 469 (2010) 370 374 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuroscience Letters journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet The influence on cognitive processing from the switches

More information

Aberrant Semantic Activation in Schizophrenia: A Neurophysiological Study

Aberrant Semantic Activation in Schizophrenia: A Neurophysiological Study SEMANTIC NESTOR, Am J Psychiatry KIMBLE, ACTIVATION 154:5, O DONNELL, May IN SCHIZOPHRENIA 1997ET AL. Aberrant Semantic Activation in Schizophrenia: A Neurophysiological Study Paul G. Nestor, Ph.D., Matthew

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Psychophysiology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 April 23.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Psychophysiology. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 April 23. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: Psychophysiology. 2014 February ; 51(2): 136 141. doi:10.1111/psyp.12164. Masked priming and ERPs dissociate maturation of orthographic

More information

Event-related potentials in word-pair processing

Event-related potentials in word-pair processing University of Wollongong Research Online University of Wollongong Thesis Collection University of Wollongong Thesis Collections 2002 Event-related potentials in word-pair processing Joseph Graffi University

More information

[In Press, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience] Right Hemisphere Activation of Joke-Related Information: An Event-Related Brain Potential Study

[In Press, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience] Right Hemisphere Activation of Joke-Related Information: An Event-Related Brain Potential Study [In Press, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience] Right Hemisphere Activation of Joke-Related Information: An Event-Related Brain Potential Study Seana Coulson Ying Choon Wu Cognitive Science, University of

More information

Attentional modulation of unconscious automatic processes: Evidence from event-related potentials in a masked priming paradigm

Attentional modulation of unconscious automatic processes: Evidence from event-related potentials in a masked priming paradigm Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience in press Attentional modulation of unconscious automatic processes: Evidence from event-related potentials in a masked priming paradigm Markus Kiefer 1 and Doreen Brendel

More information

Michael Dambacher, Reinhold Kliegl. first published in: Brain Research. - ISSN (2007), S

Michael Dambacher, Reinhold Kliegl. first published in: Brain Research. - ISSN (2007), S Universität Potsdam Michael Dambacher, Reinhold Kliegl Synchronizing timelines: Relations between fixation durations and N400 amplitudes during sentence reading first published in: Brain Research. - ISSN

More information

I. INTRODUCTION. Electronic mail:

I. INTRODUCTION. Electronic mail: Neural activity associated with distinguishing concurrent auditory objects Claude Alain, a) Benjamin M. Schuler, and Kelly L. McDonald Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, 3560

More information

The N400 as a function of the level of processing

The N400 as a function of the level of processing Psychophysiology, 32 (1995), 274-285. Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. Copyright 1995 Society for Psychophysiological Research The N400 as a function of the level of processing DOROTHEE

More information

Semantic bias, homograph comprehension, and event-related potentials in schizophrenia

Semantic bias, homograph comprehension, and event-related potentials in schizophrenia Semantic bias, homograph comprehension, and event-related potentials in schizophrenia The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story

More information

Comprehenders Rationally Adapt Semantic Predictions to the Statistics of the Local Environment: a Bayesian Model of Trial-by-Trial N400 Amplitudes

Comprehenders Rationally Adapt Semantic Predictions to the Statistics of the Local Environment: a Bayesian Model of Trial-by-Trial N400 Amplitudes Comprehenders Rationally Adapt Semantic Predictions to the Statistics of the Local Environment: a Bayesian Model of Trial-by-Trial N400 Amplitudes Nathaniel Delaney-Busch (ndelan02@tufts.edu) 1, Emily

More information

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen The following full text is a publisher's version. For additional information about this publication click this link. http://hdl.handle.net/2066/15973

More information

Running head: INTERHEMISPHERIC & GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SYNCHRONICITY 1

Running head: INTERHEMISPHERIC & GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SYNCHRONICITY 1 Running head: INTERHEMISPHERIC & GENDER DIFFERENCE IN SYNCHRONICITY 1 Interhemispheric and gender difference in ERP synchronicity of processing humor Calvin College Running head: INTERHEMISPHERIC & GENDER

More information

The Time-Course of Metaphor Comprehension: An Event-Related Potential Study

The Time-Course of Metaphor Comprehension: An Event-Related Potential Study BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 55, 293 316 (1996) ARTICLE NO. 0107 The Time-Course of Metaphor Comprehension: An Event-Related Potential Study JOËL PYNTE,* MIREILLE BESSON, FABRICE-HENRI ROBICHON, AND JÉZABEL POLI*

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Connectionist Language Processing. Lecture 12: Modeling the Electrophysiology of Language II

Connectionist Language Processing. Lecture 12: Modeling the Electrophysiology of Language II Connectionist Language Processing Lecture 12: Modeling the Electrophysiology of Language II Matthew W. Crocker crocker@coli.uni-sb.de Harm Brouwer brouwer@coli.uni-sb.de Event-Related Potentials (ERPs)

More information

Syntactic expectancy: an event-related potentials study

Syntactic expectancy: an event-related potentials study Neuroscience Letters 378 (2005) 34 39 Syntactic expectancy: an event-related potentials study José A. Hinojosa a,, Eva M. Moreno a, Pilar Casado b, Francisco Muñoz b, Miguel A. Pozo a a Human Brain Mapping

More information

INTEGRATIVE AND PREDICTIVE PROCESSES IN TEXT READING: THE N400 ACROSS A SENTENCE BOUNDARY. Regina Calloway

INTEGRATIVE AND PREDICTIVE PROCESSES IN TEXT READING: THE N400 ACROSS A SENTENCE BOUNDARY. Regina Calloway INTEGRATIVE AND PREDICTIVE PROCESSES IN TEXT READING: THE N400 ACROSS A SENTENCE BOUNDARY by Regina Calloway B.S. in Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, 2013 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty

More information

Semantic transparency and masked morphological priming: An ERP investigation

Semantic transparency and masked morphological priming: An ERP investigation Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), 506 521. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00538.x Semantic transparency

More information

Communicating hands: ERPs elicited by meaningful symbolic hand postures

Communicating hands: ERPs elicited by meaningful symbolic hand postures Neuroscience Letters 372 (2004) 52 56 Communicating hands: ERPs elicited by meaningful symbolic hand postures Thomas C. Gunter a,, Patric Bach b a Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences,

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

NeuroImage 44 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 44 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 44 (2009) 520 530 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Event-related brain potentials during the monitoring of speech errors Niels

More information

Association and not semantic relationships elicit the N400 effect: Electrophysiological evidence from an explicit language comprehension task

Association and not semantic relationships elicit the N400 effect: Electrophysiological evidence from an explicit language comprehension task Psychophysiology, 44 (2007), ** **. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2007 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00598.x Association and not semantic

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report Research Report SINGING IN THE BRAIN: Independence of Lyrics and Tunes M. Besson, 1 F. Faïta, 2 I. Peretz, 3 A.-M. Bonnel, 1 and J. Requin 1 1 Center for Research in Cognitive Neuroscience, C.N.R.S., Marseille,

More information

VivoSense. User Manual Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) Analysis Module. VivoSense, Inc. Newport Beach, CA, USA Tel. (858) , Fax.

VivoSense. User Manual Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) Analysis Module. VivoSense, Inc. Newport Beach, CA, USA Tel. (858) , Fax. VivoSense User Manual Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) Analysis VivoSense Version 3.1 VivoSense, Inc. Newport Beach, CA, USA Tel. (858) 876-8486, Fax. (248) 692-0980 Email: info@vivosense.com; Web: www.vivosense.com

More information

Brain & Language. A lexical basis for N400 context effects: Evidence from MEG. Ellen Lau a, *, Diogo Almeida a, Paul C. Hines a, David Poeppel a,b,c,d

Brain & Language. A lexical basis for N400 context effects: Evidence from MEG. Ellen Lau a, *, Diogo Almeida a, Paul C. Hines a, David Poeppel a,b,c,d Brain & Language 111 (2009) 161 172 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Brain & Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l A lexical basis for N400 context effects: Evidence from MEG

More information

THE N400 IS NOT A SEMANTIC ANOMALY RESPONSE: MORE EVIDENCE FROM ADJECTIVE-NOUN COMBINATION. Ellen F. Lau 1. Anna Namyst 1.

THE N400 IS NOT A SEMANTIC ANOMALY RESPONSE: MORE EVIDENCE FROM ADJECTIVE-NOUN COMBINATION. Ellen F. Lau 1. Anna Namyst 1. THE N400 IS NOT A SEMANTIC ANOMALY RESPONSE: MORE EVIDENCE FROM ADJECTIVE-NOUN COMBINATION Ellen F. Lau 1 Anna Namyst 1 Allison Fogel 1,2 Tania Delgado 1 1 University of Maryland, Department of Linguistics,

More information

N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition

N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition Ž. Cognitive Brain Research 4 1996 133 144 Research report N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition Jacques B. Debruille a,), Jaime Pineda b, Bernard Renault c a Centre de Recherche

More information

Neuroscience Letters

Neuroscience Letters Neuroscience Letters 468 (2010) 220 224 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuroscience Letters journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet Event-related potentials findings differ between

More information

An ERP study of low and high relevance semantic features

An ERP study of low and high relevance semantic features Brain Research Bulletin 69 (2006) 182 186 An ERP study of low and high relevance semantic features Giuseppe Sartori a,, Francesca Mameli a, David Polezzi a, Luigi Lombardi b a Department of General Psychology,

More information

Differential integration efforts of mandatory and optional sentence constituents

Differential integration efforts of mandatory and optional sentence constituents Psychophysiology, 43 (2006), 440 449. Blackwell Publishing Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2006 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00426.x Differential integration

More information

Predictability and novelty in literal language comprehension: An ERP study

Predictability and novelty in literal language comprehension: An ERP study BRES-41659; No. of pages: 13; 4C: BRAIN RESEARCH XX (2011) XXX XXX available at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report Predictability and novelty in literal language comprehension:

More information

Keywords: aphasia; lexical-semantic processing; right hemisphere semantics; event-related brain potentials; N400

Keywords: aphasia; lexical-semantic processing; right hemisphere semantics; event-related brain potentials; N400 Brain (1996), 119, 627-649 Lexical-semantic event-related potential effects in patients with left hemisphere lesions and aphasia, and patients with right hemisphere lesions without aphasia Peter Hagoort,

More information

It s all in your head: Effects of expertise on real-time access to knowledge during written sentence processing

It s all in your head: Effects of expertise on real-time access to knowledge during written sentence processing It s all in your head: Effects of expertise on real-time access to knowledge during written sentence processing Melissa Troyer 1 (mtroyer@ucsd.edu) & Marta Kutas 1,2 (mkutas@ucsd.edu) Department of Cognitive

More information

Is Semantic Processing During Sentence Reading Autonomous or Controlled? Evidence from the N400 Component in a Dual Task Paradigm

Is Semantic Processing During Sentence Reading Autonomous or Controlled? Evidence from the N400 Component in a Dual Task Paradigm Is Semantic Processing During Sentence Reading Autonomous or Controlled? Evidence from the N400 Component in a Dual Task Paradigm Annette Hohlfeld 1, Manuel Martín-Loeches 1,2 and Werner Sommer 3 1 Center

More information

Watching the Word Go by: On the Time-course of Component Processes in Visual Word Recognition

Watching the Word Go by: On the Time-course of Component Processes in Visual Word Recognition Language and Linguistics Compass 3/1 (2009): 128 156, 10.1111/j.1749-818x.2008.00121.x Watching the Word Go by: On the Time-course of Component Processes in Visual Word Recognition Jonathan Grainger 1

More information

International Journal of Psychophysiology

International Journal of Psychophysiology International Journal of Psychophysiology 84 (2012) 102 112 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect International Journal of Psychophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho

More information

Hemispheric asymmetry and pun comprehension: When cowboys have sore calves

Hemispheric asymmetry and pun comprehension: When cowboys have sore calves Brain and Language xxx (2005) xxx xxx www.elsevier.com/locate/b&l Hemispheric asymmetry and pun comprehension: When cowboys have sore calves Seana Coulson a,, Els Severens b a Cognitive Science Department,

More information

Event-related potentials during discourse-level semantic integration of complex pictures

Event-related potentials during discourse-level semantic integration of complex pictures Cognitive Brain Research 13 (2002) 363 375 www.elsevier.com/ locate/ bres Research report Event-related potentials during discourse-level semantic integration of complex pictures a, b W. Caroline West

More information

Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia Neuropsychologia 50 (2012) 1271 1285 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Neuropsychologia jo u rn al hom epa ge : www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia ERP correlates of spatially incongruent

More information

Individual Differences in the Generation of Language-Related ERPs

Individual Differences in the Generation of Language-Related ERPs University of Colorado, Boulder CU Scholar Psychology and Neuroscience Graduate Theses & Dissertations Psychology and Neuroscience Spring 1-1-2012 Individual Differences in the Generation of Language-Related

More information

Interplay between Syntax and Semantics during Sentence Comprehension: ERP Effects of Combining Syntactic and Semantic Violations

Interplay between Syntax and Semantics during Sentence Comprehension: ERP Effects of Combining Syntactic and Semantic Violations Interplay between Syntax and Semantics during Sentence Comprehension: ERP Effects of Combining Syntactic and Semantic Violations Peter Hagoort Abstract & This study investigated the effects of combined

More information

HBI Database. Version 2 (User Manual)

HBI Database. Version 2 (User Manual) HBI Database Version 2 (User Manual) St-Petersburg, Russia 2007 2 1. INTRODUCTION...3 2. RECORDING CONDITIONS...6 2.1. EYE OPENED AND EYE CLOSED CONDITION....6 2.2. VISUAL CONTINUOUS PERFORMANCE TASK...6

More information

What is music as a cognitive ability?

What is music as a cognitive ability? What is music as a cognitive ability? The musical intuitions, conscious and unconscious, of a listener who is experienced in a musical idiom. Ability to organize and make coherent the surface patterns

More information

The role of character-based knowledge in online narrative comprehension: Evidence from eye movements and ERPs

The role of character-based knowledge in online narrative comprehension: Evidence from eye movements and ERPs brain research 1506 (2013) 94 104 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres Research Report The role of character-based knowledge in online narrative comprehension: Evidence

More information

for a Lexical Integration Deficit

for a Lexical Integration Deficit Spoken Sentence Comprehension in Aphasia: Eventrelated Potential Evidence for a Lexical Integration Deficit Tamara Swab Center for Neuroscience, University of California, Davis Colin Brown and Peter Hagoort

More information

Affective Priming. Music 451A Final Project

Affective Priming. Music 451A Final Project Affective Priming Music 451A Final Project The Question Music often makes us feel a certain way. Does this feeling have semantic meaning like the words happy or sad do? Does music convey semantic emotional

More information

When Do Vehicles of Similes Become Figurative? Gaze Patterns Show that Similes and Metaphors are Initially Processed Differently

When Do Vehicles of Similes Become Figurative? Gaze Patterns Show that Similes and Metaphors are Initially Processed Differently When Do Vehicles of Similes Become Figurative? Gaze Patterns Show that Similes and Metaphors are Initially Processed Differently Frank H. Durgin (fdurgin1@swarthmore.edu) Swarthmore College, Department

More information

Acoustic Prosodic Features In Sarcastic Utterances

Acoustic Prosodic Features In Sarcastic Utterances Acoustic Prosodic Features In Sarcastic Utterances Introduction: The main goal of this study is to determine if sarcasm can be detected through the analysis of prosodic cues or acoustic features automatically.

More information

Back and forth: real-time computation of linguistic dependencies. Wing-Yee Chow (University College London)

Back and forth: real-time computation of linguistic dependencies. Wing-Yee Chow (University College London) Back and forth: real-time computation of linguistic dependencies Wing-Yee Chow (University College London) Collaborators Suiping Wang (SCNU) Ellen Lau (Maryland) Colin Phillips (Maryland) Shota Momma (UCSD)

More information

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. BACKGROUND AND AIMS [Leah Latterner]. Introduction Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin Yale University, Cognition of Musical

More information

DO STRATEGIC PRIMING PROCESSES DIFFER FOR CATEGORY VS. ASSOCIATIVE PRIMING? AN EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS STUDY OF PROACTIVE EXPECTANCY STRATEGIES.

DO STRATEGIC PRIMING PROCESSES DIFFER FOR CATEGORY VS. ASSOCIATIVE PRIMING? AN EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS STUDY OF PROACTIVE EXPECTANCY STRATEGIES. DO STRATEGIC PRIMING PROCESSES DIFFER FOR CATEGORY VS. ASSOCIATIVE PRIMING? AN EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS STUDY OF PROACTIVE EXPECTANCY STRATEGIES. By Linzi Gibson Submitted to the graduate degree program

More information

Modulation of Language Processing in Schizophrenia: Effects of Context and Haloperidol on the Event-Related Potential

Modulation of Language Processing in Schizophrenia: Effects of Context and Haloperidol on the Event-Related Potential Modulation of Language Processing in Schizophrenia: Effects of Context and Haloperidol on the Event-Related Potential Ruth Condray, Stuart R. Steinhauer, Jonathan D. Cohen, Daniel P. van Kammen, and Annette

More information

Journal of Memory and Language

Journal of Memory and Language Journal of Memory and Language xxx (2012) xxx xxx Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Generalized event knowledge

More information

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing THE NEUROSCIENCES AND MUSIC III: DISORDERS AND PLASTICITY Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing Sean Hutchins a and David Campbell b a Department of Psychology, McGill University,

More information

The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and Attitude According to Congruity-Incongruity

The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and Attitude According to Congruity-Incongruity Volume 118 No. 19 2018, 2435-2449 ISSN: 1311-8080 (printed version); ISSN: 1314-3395 (on-line version) url: http://www.ijpam.eu ijpam.eu The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and

More information