Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords

Similar documents
DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Consonance, 2: Psychoacoustic factors: Grove Music Online Article for print

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

Room acoustics computer modelling: Study of the effect of source directivity on auralizations

SPATIAL UTILIZATION OF SENSORY DISSONANCE AND THE CREATION OF SONIC SCULPTURE

Auditory-nerve responses predict pitch attributes related to musical consonance-dissonance for normal and impaired hearing a)

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

MEASURING SENSORY CONSONANCE BY AUDITORY MODELLING. Dept. of Computer Science, University of Aarhus

AN INTRODUCTION TO MUSIC THEORY Revision A. By Tom Irvine July 4, 2002

Harmonic Generation based on Harmonicity Weightings

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England

Dimensions of Music *

Compact multichannel MEMS based spectrometer for FBG sensing

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Author Index. Absolu, Brandt 165. Montecchio, Nicola 187 Mukherjee, Bhaswati 285 Müllensiefen, Daniel 365. Bay, Mert 93

Consonance and Pitch

Report. Individual Differences Reveal the Basis of Consonance

Musical Signal Processing with LabVIEW Introduction to Audio and Musical Signals. By: Ed Doering

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

The Pythagorean Scale and Just Intonation

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Pitch is one of the most common terms used to describe sound.

Asynchronous Preparation of Tonally Fused Intervals in Polyphonic Music

Physics and Neurophysiology of Hearing

Experiments on tone adjustments

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

Do Zwicker Tones Evoke a Musical Pitch?

Identification of Harmonic Musical Intervals: The Effect of Pitch Register and Tone Duration

Reduced complexity MPEG2 video post-processing for HD display

Psychoacoustics and cognition for musicians

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Psychoacoustic Approaches for Harmonic Music Mixing

Brugg, Switzerland, September R. F.

Beethoven s Fifth Sine -phony: the science of harmony and discord

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

On the strike note of bells

CONSONANCE AND DISSONANCE 4.2. Simple integer ratios Why is it that two notes an octave apart sound consonant, while two notes a little more or

Music 175: Pitch II. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015

CHALLENGING EQUAL TEMPERAMENT: PERCEIVED DIFFERENCES BETWEEN TWELVE-TONE EQUAL TEMPERAMENT AND TWELVE FIFTH-TONES TUNING

Determination of Sound Quality of Refrigerant Compressors

Lecture 7: Music

Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant

Proceedings of the 7th WSEAS International Conference on Acoustics & Music: Theory & Applications, Cavtat, Croatia, June 13-15, 2006 (pp54-59)

Smooth Rhythms as Probes of Entrainment. Music Perception 10 (1993): ABSTRACT

The role of the auditory brainstem in processing musically relevant pitch

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

The Pines of the Appian Way from Respighi s Pines of Rome. Ottorino Respighi was an Italian composer from the early 20 th century who wrote

Welcome to Vibrationdata

Informational Masking and Trained Listening. Undergraduate Honors Thesis

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

CHARACTERIZING NOISE AND HARMONICITY: THE STRUCTURAL FUNCTION OF CONTRASTING SONIC COMPONENTS IN ELECTRONIC COMPOSITION

Calibration of auralisation presentations through loudspeakers

Lecture 1: What we hear when we hear music

E314: Conjecture sur la raison de quelques dissonances generalement recues dans la musique

The interaction between room and musical instruments studied by multi-channel auralization

Music Theory: A Very Brief Introduction

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Implementing sharpness using specific loudness calculated from the Procedure for the Computation of Loudness of Steady Sounds

Creative Computing II

Error resilient H.264/AVC Video over Satellite for low Packet Loss Rates

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 (3), March /2000/107(3)/1589/9/$ Acoustical Society of America 1589

Chapter Two: Long-Term Memory for Timbre

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

ADVANCED PROCEDURES FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC NOISE EVALUATION

2005 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. The Influence of Pitch Interval on the Perception of Polyrhythms

CTP 431 Music and Audio Computing. Basic Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology (GSCT) Juhan Nam

Loudness and Sharpness Calculation

Beltone True TM with Tinnitus Breaker Pro

MASTER'S THESIS. Listener Envelopment

BIBB 060: Music and the Brain Tuesday, 1:30-4:30 Room 117 Lynch Lead vocals: Mike Kaplan

Auditory Illusions. Diana Deutsch. The sounds we perceive do not always correspond to those that are

Lab P-6: Synthesis of Sinusoidal Signals A Music Illusion. A k cos.! k t C k / (1)

Simple Harmonic Motion: What is a Sound Spectrum?

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Report. Individual Differences Reveal the Basis of Consonance

A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL STUDY OF LOW AMPLITUDE SUPERSONIC AIRCRAFT NOISE AND OTHER TRANSIENT SOUNDS

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Exploring the Rules in Species Counterpoint

TIMBRE SPACE MODEL OF CLASSICAL INDIAN MUSIC

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

Musical Acoustics Lecture 16 Interval, Scales, Tuning and Temperament - I

Aalborg Universitet. Composition: 3 Piano Pieces. Bergstrøm-Nielsen, Carl. Creative Commons License CC BY-NC 4.0. Publication date: 2017

How to Obtain a Good Stereo Sound Stage in Cars

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

Colour-influences on loudness judgements

AP Music Theory 2010 Scoring Guidelines

POST-PROCESSING FIDDLE : A REAL-TIME MULTI-PITCH TRACKING TECHNIQUE USING HARMONIC PARTIAL SUBTRACTION FOR USE WITHIN LIVE PERFORMANCE SYSTEMS

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Intonation preferences for major thirds with non-beating ensemble sounds

Perceiving patterns of ratios when they are converted from relative durations to melody and from cross rhythms to harmony

AP Music Theory 2013 Scoring Guidelines

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Music Theory. Fine Arts Curriculum Framework. Revised 2008

Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs

Pitch correction on the human voice

Music Representations

Transcription:

Downloaded from orbit.dtu.dk on: Nov 24, 28 Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords Rasmussen, Marc; Santurette, Sébastien; MacDonald, Ewen Published in: Proceedings of Forum Acusticum Publication date: 24 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link back to DTU Orbit Citation (APA): Rasmussen, M., Santurette, S., & MacDonald, E. (24). Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords. In Proceedings of Forum Acusticum General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords Marc Rasmussen, Sébastien Santurette, Ewen N. MacDonald Centre for Applied Hearing Research, Department of Electrical Engineering, Technical University of Denmark, Kgs. Lyngby, Denmark. Summary Sensory consonance and dissonance are perceptual attributes of musical intervals conveying pleasantness, tension, and harmony in musical phrases. For complex-tone dyads, corresponding to two musical notes played simultaneously, consonance is known to vary with the ratio in fundamental frequency (F) between the two tones in the dyad. While such a relationship is well established for dyads, the subjective consonance of chords containing three or more simultaneous notes, that form the basis of most musical pieces, remains to be explored. The present study aimed at comparing consonance judgments for dyads and 3-note chords as a function of the F ratio between their element tones. Dyads and chords were generated by adding two or three complex tones containing 6 harmonics with equal amplitude and random phase. The base F of the first tone was randomly selected from an interval spanning ±3/4 of a semitone centered at 44 Hz. The second tone F varied between 2 semitones above the base F. For chords, the third tone F was fixed either at 5 (Perfect 4th, P4) or at 7 (Perfect 5th, P5) semitones above the base F. Ten normal-hearing listeners were presented with all possible dyad/dyad, dyad/chord, and chord/chord combinations in random order and were asked to judge which interval was most consonant in each paired comparison. The results for dyad/dyad comparisons were consistent with earlier findings, with the unison, octave, P5, and P4 intervals being perceived as the most consonant. For dyad/chord comparisons, dyads were more consonant in the intervals around the fixed third tone. Overall, chords were not found to be more dissonant than dyads. This suggests that the hypothesis according to which consonance decreases with the amount of interaction between present harmonics, arguing for a potential role of frequency selectivity for consonance perception of dyads, might not hold for chords. PACS no. 43.66.Lj, 43.75.Cd. Introduction In Western music, the sound produced as a result of the interaction between two musical tones played simultaneously can be perceived by a listener as consonant (pleasant/harmonious) or dissonant (unpleasant/inharmonious). While the perception of consonance has received much study, and several theories have been proposed (e.g., [, 2]), it remains a current topic of research (e.g, [3]). Moreover, while the consonance of pure-tone and complex-tone dyads, corresponding to two simultaneous musical notes, has been widely studied and is known to vary with the ratio in fundamental frequency (F) between the two tones in the dyad, there is less knowledge about the subjective consonance of chords, i.e., groups of three or more notes. Specifically, the relationship between the con- (c) European Acoustics Association sonance of chords and dyads has rarely been studied directly (e.g., [4]), and the design of a unifying modeling framework that can account for the consonance and dissonance of musical sounds containing different numbers of simultaneous notes remains a challenge. Hermann von Helmholtz suggested that consonance is related to beats, i.e., fluctuations in sound amplitude resulting from the frequency difference between the interacting tones, and roughness, an attribute of sound quality occurring for small frequency differences of simultaneous tones that listeners usually describe as unpleasant []. This explanation was further refined and related to the critical bandwidth of auditory filters by Plomp and Levelt [2], who suggested that the consonance between pure tones decreases up to a frequency spacing of about /4 of the critical bandwidth, and increases progressively for larger spacings. Such a relationship between auditory-filter bandwidth and dissonance judgments by normalhearing and hearing-impaired listeners was also observed for complex-tone dyads in a study by Tufts et

FORUM ACUSTICUM 24 7-2 September, Krakow al. [4], whose observations suggested a role of peripheral frequency selectivity for dissonance perception. Independently from [2], Kameoka and Kuriyagawa developed a quantitative framework to predict dissonance of tones and harmonic complexes [5]. Underlying this framework is the key assumption that the dissonance of a dyad or chord created from harmonic complexes is the sum of the dissonances resulting from all the possible combinations of harmonic pairs. Thus, this framework predicts that chords should always be more dissonant than dyads, as adding the third tone or harmonic complex to a dyad would add dissonances from the interaction of the added harmonics. Other explanations have been given for the perception of consonance and dissonance. Indeed, a recent study of McDermott et al. suggests that harmonicity in the complex-tone spectra plays a key role in the perception of consonance [3] and can more strongly influence consonance perception than beating. Thus, these results might predict that chords created by adding a perfect fourth or perfect fifth note to a dyad would be judged as more consonant than the dyad, as the spectra would include more harmonicity, particularly if the dyad was dissonant. In the present study, listeners directly compared the consonance of dyads and chords against each other using a paradigm inspired by [4]. The aim was to clarify whether dissonance is directly related to the amount of harmonic interactions within peripheral auditory filters, in which case chords should overall be more dissonant than dyads, or whether other factors such as harmonicity or pitch strength should be considered in models of consonance perception. 2. Method A total of listeners with varying degrees of musical training (ranging from none to seven years of training) participated. None of the listeners reported any history of hearing loss and all had normal audiometric thresholds ( 25 db HL, 5 8 Hz). The procedure and stimuli were inspired by that used in [4]. Participants sat in a sound-insulated booth and listened to pairs of dyads and/or chords. For each pair, listeners judged which was more consonant. As in [4], participants were instructed that a consonant sound was "pleasant, smooth, pure, and harmonious". Stimuli were presented monaurally to the right ear using Senheiser HD58 headphones. The set of intervals used to construct the dyads and chords are presented in Table I. Each dyad consisted of two harmonic complex tones. Each harmonic complex contained six components (i.e., a fundamental and 5 harmonics). All twelve components were were of equal amplitude and random starting phase. Similarly, the chords consisted of three harmonic complexes, each with six components. For the perfect fourth (P4) chords, one of the harmonic complexes Table I. Semitone separation, name, and corresponding frequency ratio of the intervals used in this study. *Note that an incorrect ratio for the perfect fifth was used. The correct ratio should have been.498. Semitone Interval Frequency Separation Name Ratio Unison. Minor 2nd.59 2 Major 2nd.22 3 Minor 3rd.89 4 Major 3rd.26 5 Perfect 4th.335 6 Tritone.44 7 Perfect 5th.489* 8 Minor 6th.587 9 Major 6th.682 Minor 7th.782 Major 7th.888 2 Octave 2. corresponded with the five semitone interval. Similarly, for the perfect fifth (P5) chord, one of the harmonic complexes corresponded with the seven semitone interval. Each dyad and/or chord was 6 ms in duraction and each pair was separated by a silent interval of 25 ms. Over the course of the experiment, each participant listened to a total of 482 pairs. All possible dyad/chord combinations were presented twice (i.e., in both orders, A vs. B and B vs. A). For each pair, the F of the root was randomly chosen. The values were uniformly distributed between 42.35 Hz and 459.48 Hz, which corresponded to 3/4 of a semitone below and above 44 Hz, respectively. Due to a typing error, the frequency ratio of the presented perfect 5th was offset relative to the one originally intended. Thus, the presented perfect 5th was not a true perfect 5th. The frequency ratio used in the experiment was.489 rather than.498. This introduced an error of approximately %. While small, the difference in terms of consonance is still audible. 3. Results For each participant, a running score was kept for each of the dyad and chords. When a listener judged a dyad (or chord) to be more consonant, the score for that dyad (or chord) was incremented. The total score was then normalized by the total number of presentations of that dyad (or chord). Thus, a normalized score of corresponded to the case where that dyad (or chord) was always judged as more consonant. Conversely, a normalized score of corresponded to the opposite case, where the dyad (or chord) was always judged as less consonant (i.e., more dissonant). The normalized results, averaged across listeners, are plotted in Figure. The results in the left panel

FORUM ACUSTICUM 24 7-2 September, Krakow. Total score 2 4 6 8 2. Vs. same kind 2 4 6 8 2 Figure. Normalized mean total consonance scores (left) and mean consonance scores vs. same type (right). Points that lie outside the grey region are significantly different from chance (p <.5, Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons). Errorbars indicate one standard error. Dyad P4 P5. Dyad over Chord 2 4 6 8 2 Dyad vs. P4 Dyad vs. P5. P4 over P5 2 4 6 8 2 Figure 2. Normalized mean consonance scores of dyads vs. chords of the same interval (left) and P4 vs. P5 chords of the same interval (right). Errorbars indicate one standard error. are from judgements of all combinations, while the results in the right panel are from judgements of the same type (i.e., dyad vs. dyad, P4 vs. P4, or P5 vs. P5). For dyads, the most consonant intervals were the unison, perfect 4th, perfect 5th, and octave (i.e., intervals of, 5, 7, and 2 semitones, respectively). Conversely, the minor 2nd and major 7th (i.e., intervals of and semitones, respectively) were the most dissonant. For the P4 chords, the most consonant intervals were the unison, perfect 4th, major 6th, and octave (i.e., intervals of, 5, 9, and 2 semitones, respectively). Conversely, the minor 2nd, major 3rd, tritone, and major 7th (i.e., intervals of, 4, 6, and semitones, respectively) were the most dissonant. For the P5 chords, the most consonant intervals were the unison, major 3rd, perfect 5th, and octave (i.e., intervals of, 4, 7, and 2 semitones, respectively). Conversely, the minor 2nd, tritone, minor 6th, and major 7th (i.e., intervals of, 6, 8, and semitones, respectively) were the most dissonant. While the results in the left and right panels of Figure are quite similar, a direct comparison of dyads and chords with the same interval is plotted in Figure 2. In the left panel, the normalized results indicate the relative frequency of the dyad being judged as more consonant than the chord. Thus, scores near indicate that the dyad and chord were judged to be

FORUM ACUSTICUM 24 7-2 September, Krakow similar in consonance. In the right panel, the normalized results indicate the relative frequency of the P4 chord being judged as more consonant than the P5 chord. 4. Discussion In the present study, participants listened to pairs of dyads and/or chords and, for each pair, judged which was more consonant than the other. Overall, the pattern of results for the dyads is in agreement with previous studies (e.g., [2, 5, 4]). In comparing the normalized dyad consonance scores, the results for the total score as a function of interval (left panel of Figure ) are quite similar to the normalized score when the comparisons are limited to only other dyads (right panel of Figure ). A similar pattern is observed between the total score and the vs. same kind scores for P4 and P5 chords. Taken together, these results do not suggest that dyads are systematically more or less consonant than chords. A more complex picture emerges when dyads and chords with the same interval are compared (Figure 2, left panel). When dyads and P4 chords of the same interval are compared directly, they are judged to be similar in consonance (i.e., the normalized consonance score is approximately ) for all intervals except 3, 4, 6, and 7 semitones (blue curve). A similar pattern is observed for dyads and P5 chords, except that the intervals where dyads are judged to be more consonant are 5, 6, 8, and 9 semitones (red curve). Thus, a dyad is perceived as more consonant than the corresponding P4 or P5 chord if the interval between the second and third tone of the chord is within two semitones. For second chord notes more than 2 semitones away from the third P4 and P5 notes (i.e., intervals of,, 2,,, and 2 semitones), the consonance of dyads and either chord type do not differ (scores of approximately in the left panel of Figure 2). No large dyad vs chord difference is observed either when the second note is equal to the third (5 semitones for P4, 7 semitones for P5), i.e., conditions in which the frequencies of all harmonics are identical for the dyad and the corresponding chords and only harmonic amplitudes differ. These observations also indicate that, when the second and third note in the chord are sufficiently far apart in terms of F, chords are neither more consonant nor more dissonant than corresponding dyads. When P4 and P5 chords are directly compared (Figure 2, right panel), similar consonance ratings are obtained except when the interval between the second and third note is greater than semitone. Overall, the behavioural measures suggest that dyads are not more consonant than chords, except in the intervals close to the fixed third tone of the particular chord. This is in contrast to the framework developed by Kameoka and Kuriyagawa [5] that suggests that dissonance of a set of sinusoids is equal to the sum of the dissonance of all combinations of harmonic pairs. While the introduction of a - or 2- semitone interval within a chord is detrimental to consonance, suggesting a contribution of a close spacing between harmonics to dissonance, such a contribution cannot be generalized in an additive fashion to all tested semitone intervals. Thus, other aspects of auditory perception must play a role in the subjective consonance of chords and dyads. 5. Conclusions In general, dyads are not systematically more consonant or dissonant than chords unless the interval between the second and third tone of a three-tone chord is within two semitones (in which case dyads are perceived as more consonant). This implies that assumptions regarding the general additivity of dissonance (e.g., [5]) are likely incorrect. The present findings suggest that a model of consonance perception based solely on the amount of peripheral interaction between harmonic components within the critical bandwidth of auditory filters would fail at correctly predicting consonance judgments of both complextone dyads and chords. A comparison of consonance predictions from such a model with those of modeling approaches based on harmonicity (e.g., [3]) or pitch strength (e.g., [6]) may reveal whether one or a combination of these different attributes can best account for the subjective consonance and dissonance of any combination of complex tones. References [] H. von Helmholtz: Die Lehre von der Tonempfindungen als physiologische Grundlage für die Theorie der Musik. F. Vieweg & Sohn, Braunschweig, 863. [2] R. Plomp, W. J. M. Levelt: Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 38 (965) 548-56. [3] J. H. McDermott, A. J. Lehr, A. J. Oxenham: Individual differences reveal the basis of consonance. Current Biology 2 (2) 35-4. [4] J. B. Tufts, M. R. Molis, M. R. Leek: Perception of dissonance by people with normal hearing and sensorineural hearing loss. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 8 (25) 955-967. [5] A. Kameoka, M. Kuriyagawa: Consonance theory part II: Consonance of complex tones and its calculation method. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 45 (969) 46-469. [6] G. M. Bidelman, M. G. Heinz: Auditory-nerve responses predict pitch attributes related to musical consonance-dissonance for normal and impaired hearing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 3 (2) 488-52.