Developing a Semantic Fieldwork Project November 5, 2013

Similar documents
Semantic Research Methodology

Semantics and Generative Grammar. Conversational Implicature: The Basics of the Gricean Theory 1

INTERNATIONAL INDIAN SCHOOL BURAIDAH ENGLISH GRAMMAR WORKSHEET 06 GRADE- 3

The Adventures of Ali Baba Bernstein

LEVEL PRE-A1 LAAS LANGUAGE ATTAINMENT ASSESSMENT SYSTEM. English English Language Language Examinations Examinations. December 2005 December 2007

Where are we? Lecture 37: Modelling Conversations. Gap. Conversations

Sample Chapter. Unit 5. Refusing in Japanese. 100 Unit 5

ntre.ir ENGLISH FILE 2 End-of-course Test Grammar, Vocabulary, and Pronunciation

Introduction to Semantics. Semantic Fieldwork Project: The Indefinite Ren Bat in Basque

Linking semantic and pragmatic factors in the Japanese Internally Headed Relative Clause

MODAL VERBS ABILITY. We can t meet them tomorrow. Can you hear that noise?

1 Family and friends. 1 Play the game with a partner. Throw a dice. Say. How to play

able, alone, animal, become, call, catch, country, monkey, thin, word; baby, clean, eat, enjoy, family, fruit, jump, kind, man, parent

Exploring nominal reference in the field: Diagnostics plus results from Bulu

Reading On The Move. Reasoning and Logic

Grammar. Name: 1 Underline the correct words.

Presentations- Correct the Errors

Lecture 24: Motivating Modal Logic, Translating into It

Sentence Processing. BCS 152 October

Hello. I m Q-rex. Target Language. Phone Number :

Part A Instructions and examples

Topics in Linguistic Theory: Propositional Attitudes

English in Mind. Level 2. Module 1. Guided Dialogues RESOURCES MODULE 1 GUIDED DIALOGUES

Meaning 1. Semantics is concerned with the literal meaning of sentences of a language.

Write your answers on the question paper. You will have six minutes at the end of the test to copy your answers onto the answer sheet.

Page 1. Tomorrow, we could go for a walk play games indoors. Tick one. when. because. We had to hurry because the bus was about to leave.

Homework Monday. The Shortcut

Dilworth Elementary Character Education: Honesty (November)

Mental Spaces, Conceptual Distance, and Simulation: Looks/Seems/Sounds Like Constructions in English

General Revision on Module 1& 1 and (These are This is You are) two red apples in the basket.

Sometimes, at night, the dirt outside turns into a beautiful

Rubric: Cambridge English, Preliminary English Test for Schools - Listening.

HAVE GOT WAS WERE CAN. Koalatext.com TO BE GRAMMAR CONDITIONAL 0

ENGLISH FILE. Progress Test Files Complete the sentences with the correct form of the. 3 Underline the correct word or phrase.

Unit 6. of Anna s family members in the correct spaces in the family tree. Look at the box with

English quiz Quiz1 / September 2016 Class : Grade 9(a,b,c,d) Duration : 50min Obj: Maintain info/tenses

Workbook 3 Audio script

21 강. Lesson12 Dead Poets Society-➀

Introduction to Natural Language Processing This week & next week: Classification Sentiment Lexicons

DRAMA SCRIPTS - 3 x 5 minute plays Target audience: 7-11 year olds

LESSON 35. Objectives

Influence of lexical markers on the production of contextual factors inducing irony

7- Read the dialogue and answer. Did anyone help you with your homework? No. I did it by myself.

1 Read the text. Then complete the sentences. (6 x 2 = 12 points)

Terminology down down down down down down down

Section 2: Known and Unknown

Six. Unit. What does he do? Target Language. What does he do?

1. As you study the list, vary the order of the words.

Tanuló neve és osztálya: Tanára: Elért eredménye: Írásbeli: / 60 Szóbeli: /40 Összes: /100

Intro to Pragmatics (Fox/Menéndez-Benito) 10/12/06. Questions 1

Lexie World (The Three Lost Kids, #1) Chapter 1- Where My Socks Disappear

Talking about the Future in English. Rules Stories Exercises SAMPLE CHAPTER. By Really Learn English

UNIT 3 Comparatives and superlatives

G. D. GOENKA PUBLIC SCHOOL SECTOR - 48, GURGAON MOCK TEST-I ( ) CLASS - V SUBJECT - ENGLISH Name : Roll No. : Class : Section : Date :

Name Period Date. Grade 7, Unit 1 Pre-assessment. Read this selection from Fast Sam, Cool Clyde, and Stuff by Walter Dean Myers

1 Family and friends. 1 A: Is Susan your grandmother? B: Yes,. 2 A: Is Tom your dad? B: No,. He s my uncle.

ДЕМОВЕРСИЯ РАБОТЫ ПО АНГЛИЙСКОМУ ЯЗЫКУ ДЛЯ ПОСТУПЛЕНИЯ В 8 КЛАСС. VOCABULARY

Prepared by Dahlia Yousri. New Headway Plus: Pre-Intermediate UNIT 8 GRAMMAR

Skill-Builders. Grades 4 5. Grammar & Usage. Writer Sarah Guare. Editorial Director Susan A. Blair. Project Manager Erica L.

CRONOGRAMA DE RECUPERAÇÃO ATIVIDADE DE RECUPERAÇÃO

THE GREATEST GRANDMOTHER Hal Ames

TEST ONE. Singing Star Showing this week. !The Wild Wheel Ride! Indoor tennis centre. RACING CAR TRACK To drive, children must be 1 metre or more

ENGLISH FILE. End-of-course Test. 1 Complete the sentences. Use the correct form of the. 3 Underline the correct word(s) in each sentence.

Curtis & Kevin. before gerunds

South Avenue Primary School. Name: New Document 1. Class: Date: 44 minutes. Time: 44 marks. Marks: Comments: Page 1

SURVEYS FOR REFLECTIVE PRACTICE

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

Brooklyn Says OY! Brooklyn Responds YO! Deborah Kass at The Brooklyn Museum

1. There are some bananas on the table, but there aren t any apples.

STYLE. Sample Test. School Tests for Young Learners of English. Form A. Level 1

DEPARTMENT OF AFRICAN AND GENERAL STUDIES UDS WA GEN 101 LECTURE 3

AULAS 11 e 12 MODAL VERBS SUMMARY

Inglês CHAPTERS 11 and 12

Bismarck, North Dakota is known for several things. First of all, you probably already know that Bismarck is the state capitol. You might even know

Sidestepping the holes of holism

Conversation 1. Conversation 2. Conversation 3. Conversation 4. Conversation 5

Complete the Sentences with Modal Verbs

PAM&PAUL, track 1. Pam and Paul, Story 1

Take a Look! DVD Fichas Fotocopiáveis Episode 1 Happy Birthday! (scene 1)

Main and Helping Verbs

On Recanati s Mental Files

2018 English Entrance Exam for Returnees

Commonly Misspelled Words

8 HERE AND THERE _OUT_BEG_SB.indb 68 13/09/ :41

Anglia Examinations Preliminary Level Four Skills

All you ever wanted to know about literary terms and MORE!!!

Denise and Lisa are chatting over the phone. Listen to their conversation and answer the question that follows about the main idea.

9 Guests are allowed to wear casual dress. 11 There's a possibility that the show will be cancelled think that Andrew will collect the money.

Chapter. Adverb Clauses CHAPTER SUMMARY. CHART Introduction. Page 365 Time: minutes. Adverb Clauses 119

UNIT 3 Past simple OJ Circle the right words in each sentence.

Here s a question for you: What happens if we try to go the other way? For instance:

A. Write a or an before each of these words. (1 x 1mark = 10 marks) St. Thomas More College Half Yearly Examinations February 2009

Types of perceptual content

ESL Podcast 415 Being Clean and Dirty

Lesson 1 Vocabulary. 1 Write the words and phrases in the puzzle. 2 Read and complete the definitions. 3 Read and remember the grammar in the lesson.

CS 5014: Research Methods in Computer Science

Hello! & Welcome to A Twisted Plays/Junior Drama Sample Script! On the following pages you will find a sample of the script that is available for

TOUR OF A UNIT. Step 1: Grammar in Context

Readers Theater Adaptation of Edgar Allan s Official Crime Investigation Notebook by Mary Amato. Characters

THE TWENTY MOST COMMON LANGUAGE USAGE ERRORS

Transcription:

Developing a Semantic Fieldwork Project November 5, 2013 I. Background reading: Lisa Matthewson. On the methodology of semantic fieldwork. http://faculty.arts.ubc.ca/lmatthewson/pdf/fieldwork.pdf II. Classes: Tuesday, November 5: Introduction to elicitation methods in semantics; how do semanticists conduct research? Thursday, November 7: Illustrating and practicing elicitation methods, hypothesis formation, and conclusion drawing with Navajo data. Tuesday, November 12: 1. Develop a testable hypothesis about a construction in Italian 2. Establish the predictions made by your hypothesis There will be additional times for fieldwork sessions when you are revising your hypotheses for the second JYW assignment. III. Assignments based on fieldwork: A: Lab report, to be turned in as Homework 6 In the lab report, you will work in small groups on a data set from a language other than English. Your task, as a group, will be to: (i) Come up with a hypothesis regarding the semantic interpretation of this particular data set (ii) Formulate the predictions that your hypothesis makes (iii) Construct contexts/target sentences to test your predictions during the elicitation sessions. You will receive feedback on your homework lab report which will help you revise for B: Second Junior Year Writing Paper (unless you ve made other arrangements) Your second JYW assignment will be a short paper based on our fieldwork session. The final paper, based on your revised hypothesis after the elicitation sessions, is due on the last day of classes. 1

1. Translations Summary of Semantic Research Methodologies Based on Matthewson (2004) and Powerpoint Slides Ask for translations of complete sentences and open-lexical words only - never ask for the translation of functional words. Try to make the source string a grammatical sentence. Assume that the result string is a grammatical sentence. Do not expect your consultant to conduct analysis. This includes not asking him/her to compare an English construction (or a construction of your meta-language) to one in his/her language and produce something parallel. 2. Judgments A judgment is something a native speaker is qualified to give, by virtue of knowing the language (Matthewson 2004: 399) Comment A comment is anything else the speaker might want to say about the structure or meaning of a string (Matthewson 2004; 399) Types of judgment: A. Grammaticality judgments Commonly used in syntax; test whether a sentence is acceptable at all in a language or not B. Truth value judgments Test whether a sentence is True or False in a particular scenario C. Felicity judgments Test the acceptability of an utterance in the discourse context Truth value judgments have to do with truth conditions (or, asserted content). Felicity judgments relate to presupposed content (or, sometimes, implicatures). Record (when possible) and take note of every single comment made by the consultant during an elicitation session. This includes statements the consultant makes about context, alternative ways of saying things, fine-grained grammaticality judgments, meanings of parts of words, comments about formality, alternative word order possibilities, etc. (Matthewson 2004) You may quote the comments of your consultant in your paper. See section 4.5. (page 408) in Matthewson (2004) s paper. 3. A mock interview session using a verbal context First, present the context first, then present the target sentence. For every new scenario you test, repeat the sentence without modification (F = fieldworker, C = consultant). 2

F: Say that Mary was dancing yesterday and right now she s resting. Could I say Mary danced? C: No, that s wrong. F: Say that Mary is dancing right now. Could I say Mary danced? C: Yes, that s good. F: Say that Mary is resting right now, but she s going to be dancing in an hour. Could I say Mary danced? C: No, that s not right. Next step, part 1: Try a minimally different sentence in the same contexts so that you can compare your target construction with other data from the language. F: Okay, say that Mary was dancing yesterday and right now she s resting. Could I say, Mary is dancing? Next step, part 2: Try to figure out whether the bad sentences were false or infelicitous in the context. Imagine that you got the following results: (1) Context: You are riding the bus with your friend Bill talking to you about his family. He says to you, out of the blue, a. My cousin looks like Elvis. b. # It s my cousin who looks like Elvis. (2) Context: Your friend Bill tells you that someone in his family looks like Elvis, but he can t remember who. You are looking through photos of his family together. Bill sees one of his cousin and says, a. My cousin looks like Elvis. b. It s my cousin who looks like Elvis. Is (1b) false or infelicitous in the context? What is the difference between the two contexts? In (2) but not (1) included in the context is the information that someone in Bill s family looks like Elvis. o Intuition: The it-cleft construction it s my cousin who looks like Elvis presupposes that there is someone who looks like Elvis. Try to hey wait a minute test to see if you can target this presupposition to the exclusion of other (asserted) content. (3) A: It s my cousin who looks like Elvis. B1: Hey, wait a minute! There s someone who looks like Elvis in your family?! B2: #Hey, wait a minute! I didn t know that your cousin looks like Elvis! It seems like the badness of (1b) is due to it being infelicitous in the context, rather than it being false. 3

Illustration of Fieldwork Methodology with Navajo Modals 1. Background on English modality In all languages, you can talk about events possibly or necessarily happening. Modals can be possibility (or, weak ) modals or necessity (or, strong ) modals. Might is an example of a possibility modal in English, while must is an example of a necessity modal. In English, sentences modals are typically ambiguous (they have at least two different sets of truth conditions) between belief, law, and ability meanings (or some subset of these meanings). (1) a. The dog must be outside. 1.) What you know: you haven t seen the dog inside anywhere; you searched high and low for it and haven t seen it anywhere. 2.) What the rules are: the dog is required to be outside; it would be against the rules for the dog to come inside. b. They dog may be outside. 1.) What you know: you haven t seen the dog inside, but you re not totally sure if she s inside or outside. It s possible that she s outside. 2.) What the rules are: The dog is allowed to be outside; it wouldn t be against the rules for the dog to be outside. c. The dog can go outside. 1.) What the rules are: The dog is allowed to be outside. 2.) What the dog is physically able to do: It s legs work, it s not in a crate. Modals are ambiguous between a number of different readings: o Belief: What is possible or necessary given what you know about the world. o Laws: What is possible or necessary given what the rules or laws are. o Ability: What is possible or necessary given someone s abilities (e.g. physical abilities). 2. Introducing the Navajo Data There are two particles in Navajo that have been described as being modal : daats í and shį į. Intuition from the previous literature: Willie (1996) writes that daats í is somehow weaker than shį į. 4

Hypothesis 1: shį į is completely equivalent to English must. Daats í is completely equivalent to English might. Prediction of this hypothesis: We will be able to use shį į in any context where must is possible in English. We will be able to use daats í in any context where a belief use of might is possible in English. Testable question: Do we find shį į being used in all the environments where we find English must? Crucially, must can get both belief and law readings. Do we find daats í being used with both belief and law readings? In order to examine belief vs. law readings of modals, we used two storyboards. Storyboards are series of pictures that form a narrative designed to elicit particular sorts of constructions. (2) Storyboard designed to elicit law modals Context: Mary s friends are asking if she can come out and play. Her mother gave her a number of chores to do, however. How would you narrate this slide in English? 5

How is it narrated in Navajo? Do we see daats í in the first sentence and shį į in the second? No. We find people using constructions like the following: (3) Mééwii ání, Doo bíighah da. łeets aa tábi niigiz. Mary says not PRT not dishes I.will.wash.them Free translation: Mary says, I can t do it, I m going to wash the dishes. I asked consultants if (4) was possible. They said it was not. (4) * Mééwii ání, Doo daats í da. łeets aa shį į tábi niigiz. Mary says not PRT not dishes PRT I.will.wash.them Methodological question: Why should I be careful about the judgment that (4) is bad? What are some of the reasons why the sentence might have been rejected? Having found that daats í and shį į cannot be used in contexts where we find law modals in English, I looked at belief modals in Navajo with a second storyboard. (5) Storyboard designed to elicit belief modals: Context: George has not arrived to his birthday party. His friends are wondering why he is not there. How would you narrate this slide in English? 6

How was this slide narrated in Navajo? Did we see people using daats í or shį į? Yes. We found people using both. (6) a. K ad hooghandi shį į t óó ajiłhosh. now at.home PRT still 4Subj.sleeps b. Jóoj daats i t ahdii ałhosh. George PRT still 3Subj.sleeps Hypothesis 2: Shį į is a strong belief modal. Daats í is a weak belief modal. Prediction of this hypothesis: We will be able to use shį į in any context where a belief use of must is possible in English. We will be able to use daats í in any context where a belief use of might is possible in English. Hypothesis 3: Daats í and shį į interchangeable since both were volunteered in (6). Both are equivalent to must and might. Prediction of this hypothesis: We can use daats í and shį į in exactly the same contexts. They are totally interchangeable. In order to test these hypotheses, we have to think about the differences between possibility and necessity belief modals in English. 2.1 Having too much direct evidence rules out the use of must Is the same true for Navajo daats í and shį į? (7) Context: You see pieces of a plate on your kitchen floor. You say: łeets aa shį į sits il (sentence constructed by me) plate PRT it.broke Comment: You wouldn t use shį į here at all. If you see the pieces, it s funny. If you just heard the sound, then it s okay. (8) Context:You drive past Ellen s house and see her walking past the window. a. You say: Ellen shį į sidá. (sentence constructed by me) Ellen PRT she.sits Comment: You see her, and say Ellen shį į sidá? That doesn t sound right. 7

b. You say: Ellen daats í sidá. (sentence constructed by me) Ellen PRT she.sits Comment: Why would you voice possibilities if the evidence is there? What do these data suggest about our hypotheses 2 and 3? 2.2 Must, but not might, can be used where the speaker have a lot of evidence for a particular conclusion (9) Context: You look out the window. In the distance, you can see dark clouds gathering. You also can smell the damp pavement smell that typically precedes rain, and feel a cool breeze blowing. You tell me what you re thinking about the weather. In English, I might say It must be going to rain. What would you say in Navajo? Tł odi shį į nahooniiłtah (free response by consultant) outside PRT rain (10) Context: You see very fresh elk tracks in the mud while hiking. You say, a. Dzééh shį į kǫ ǫ naaghá (sentence constructed by me) elk PRT here it.goes Comment: You can say shį į in this context. If you don t have shį į, then you need to see it close by. The elk is pretty close. b. Dzééh daats í kǫ ǫ naaghá (sentence constructed by me) elk PRT here it.goes Comment 1: Shį į sounds better here. When you say daats í, you re not really sure. With shį į you re more leaning to the positive that it happened. With daats í you re not really sure there s an elk around here Comment 2: This sentence is also okay with daats í - I wonder if it is still nearby. (11) Context: You work indoors in an office building. There aren t any windows near your desk. You see people coming in with wet clothing and dripping umbrellas. In English, I might say It must be raining. 8

Tł oodi shį į nahałtin. outside PRT rains (free response by consultant) Comment: If you say daats i then it sounds like you re not too sure, you re questioning yourself. (12) Context: You are playing a game with three cups. Mary asks you to figure out which cup is hiding the pebble. You have already asked her to turn over Cup 1 and Cup 2: the pebble wasn t under either one of them. You say to Mary, Díí shįí tsé biyaa si ą (sentence constructed by me) this PRT rock it.under it.sits Comment: You could say this. It s the last option. Shį į is more definite than daats í, so it s good where it s the only option left. What do these data suggest about our hypotheses 2 and 3? Does this picture change at all when we look at (13)? (13) Context: You looked at the weather forecast for the upcoming week and saw the prediction for tomorrow s weather said that there is a 30% chance of rain. Nahoodoołtį į ł shį į. will.rain PRT Comment 1: There s a possibility that it s going to rain tomorrow. Comment 2: With 30%, shį į sounds best. What sorts of interim conclusions might you draw here? What further data would you want to see? 2.3 How does daats í compare with might? In the following examples, how would it have sounded if you had used might? (14) Context: You don t know if it is raining or not. Your coworker has been in your windowless office with you all day so you know she doesn t know. You say to her, Nahałtin daats í it.is.raining PRT Free translation by speaker: I wonder if it s raining, Maybe it s raining. 9

(15) Context: You went to school before your sister Mary. She was still in bed when you left. You know she had a stomachache this morning. a. Mary bibid diniih. Ólta góó daats í doogááł. Mary her.stomach it.hurts school-to DAATS Í she.will.go Mary has a stomachache. I wonder if she ll go to school maybe she will go to school, or not. b. # Mary bibid diniih. Ólta góó shį į doogááł. Mary her.stomach it.hurts school-to MODAL she.will.go Mary has a stomachache. She ll probably go to school. Comment: Sounds more positive that Mary will go to school. Sounds weird unless you have áko ndi ( but ) before the second sentence. Do these data suggest any differences between daats í and might? How do these data change our working hypothesis? 2.4 The problem of translations of daats í sentences Recall that one of the problems that Matthewson (2004) discusses for the use of translations as anything more than clues is that morphemes or constructions in two languages may not have perfect correspondences. When speakers offered free translations of sentences with daats í in them, there was a lot of variation. What does this variation mean? (16) Nahałtin daats í it.is.raining DAATS Í a. Is it raining? b. I wonder if it s raining. Maybe it s raining, or maybe not. I don t know if it s raining or not. It might be raining. Daats í hypothesis 1: Sentences with daats í are ambiguous between questions and might sentences (or something like them, viz. 2.3). Prediction: We will find sentences with daats í used in the same contexts as questions in Navajo and English (assume that Navajo and English questions work the same way). Methodological Point: The following materials show a variation on the judgment in a context method. Here, the context includes material following the target sentence. This helps you to see what kinds of discourse situations a particular sentence is felicitous and/or true in. 10

Difference 1: While questions (in both Navajo and English) require a response from the Addressee and are not appropriate as answers to previous questions, daats í sentences can stand alone as answers and do not require replies. (17) Context: You (Speaker) and a friend (Addressee) are discussing who ll come to a party. Speaker says: Ted=ísh yiighah Ted-Q he.will.come Will Ted come? Addressee says: a. Ted daats í yiighah Ted PRT he.will.come b. Aoo, Ted yiighah Yes Ted he.will.come c. Ted shį į yiighah Ted PRT he.will.come d.# Ted=ísh yiighah Ted=Q he.will.come Difference 2: Sentences with daats í are somehow sufficiently informative such that action can be taken on their basis. Questions are not. (18) Context: You (Speaker) and a friend (Addressee) are discussing what food to make for the party on the basis of which guests might come. A: Ted daats í yiighah Ted PRT he.will.come B: Akóshį į, dah diniilghazh ła adeeshłiish. okay.then frybread some I.will.make.it Okay then, I will make some frybread ( because Ted likes frybread). (19) Context: You (Speaker) and a friend (Addressee) are discussing what food to make for the party on the basis of which guests might come. A: Ted=ísh yiighah Ted=Q he.will.come Will Ted come? B: # Akóshį į, dah diniilghazh ła adeeshłiish. okay.then frybread some I.will.make.it Okay then, I will make some frybread ( because Ted likes frybread). On the basis of these data, do we want to claim that one meaning of daats í sentences is a question meaning? Do they function like questions? 11