MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Similar documents
TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

Experiments on tone adjustments

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 (3), March /2000/107(3)/1589/9/$ Acoustical Society of America 1589

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England

Chapter 2 Measurement of Loudness, Part I: Methods, Problems, and Pitfalls

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

The mid-difference hump in forward-masked intensity discrimination a)

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

A comparison of the temporal weighting of annoyance and loudness

Do Zwicker Tones Evoke a Musical Pitch?

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

THE PERSISTENCE OF LOUDNESS IN SPEECH FREQUENCIES INFLUENCE OF ECOLOGICAL CONTEXT.

I. INTRODUCTION. Electronic mail:

Dynamic Characteristics of Hearing and Its Applications

Pitch perception for mixtures of spectrally overlapping harmonic complex tones

Binaural summation of loudness: Noise and two-tone complexes

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

Beltone True TM with Tinnitus Breaker Pro

Individual Differences in Loudness Processing and Loudness Scales

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

An overview of Bertram Scharf s research in France on loudness adaptation

Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant

Informational Masking and Trained Listening. Undergraduate Honors Thesis

Loudness of pink noise and stationary technical sounds

Perceptual thresholds for detecting modifications applied to the acoustical properties of a violin

Navigating on Handheld Displays: Dynamic versus Static Peephole Navigation

Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch

The Relationship Between Auditory Imagery and Musical Synchronization Abilities in Musicians

Intensity discrimination and loudness for tones in notched noise

Effects of headphone transfer function scattering on sound perception

Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Violin timbre and the picket fence

EFFECTS OF REVERBERATION TIME AND SOUND SOURCE CHARACTERISTIC TO AUDITORY LOCALIZATION IN AN INDOOR SOUND FIELD. Chiung Yao Chen

Tinnitus Assessment Appointment

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

Detection and demodulation of non-cooperative burst signal Feng Yue 1, Wu Guangzhi 1, Tao Min 1

Effects of Remaining Hair Cells on Cochlear Implant Function

Rhona Hellman and the Munich School of Psychoacoustics

The presence of multiple sound sources is a routine occurrence

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

IP Telephony and Some Factors that Influence Speech Quality

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

Perceptual and physical evaluation of differences among a large panel of loudspeakers

Spatial-frequency masking with briefly pulsed patterns

INTENSITY DYNAMICS AND LOUDNESS CHANGE: A REVIEW OF METHODS AND PERCEPTUAL PROCESSES

The importance of recording and playback technique for assessment of annoyance

A 5 Hz limit for the detection of temporal synchrony in vision

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Voice segregation by difference in fundamental frequency: Effect of masker type

Colour-influences on loudness judgements

Why are natural sounds detected faster than pips?

Music Perception with Combined Stimulation

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Psychoacoustics. lecturer:

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Pitch is one of the most common terms used to describe sound.

Symmetric interactions and interference between pitch and timbre

Client centred sound therapy selection: Tinnitus assessment into practice. G D Searchfield

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

Dial A440 for absolute pitch: Absolute pitch memory by non-absolute pitch possessors

2. Measurements of the sound levels of CMs as well as those of the programs

Overview of ITU-R BS.1534 (The MUSHRA Method)

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

Predicting annoyance judgments from psychoacoustic metrics: Identifiable versus neutralized sounds

Tinnitus Quick Guide

Reliability of Tinnitus Loudness Matches under Procedural Variation

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Electrical Stimulation of the Cochlea to Reduce Tinnitus. Richard S. Tyler, Ph.D. Overview

I. INTRODUCTION. 1 place Stravinsky, Paris, France; electronic mail:

Semi-automated extraction of expressive performance information from acoustic recordings of piano music. Andrew Earis

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

A Real Word Case Study E- Trap by Bag End Ovasen Studios, New York City

A Real Word Case Study E- Trap by Bag End Ovasen Studios, New York City

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Residual Inhibition Functions in Relation to Tinnitus Spectra and Auditory Threshold Shift

Processes for the Intersection

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Behavioral and neural identification of birdsong under several masking conditions

Residual inhibition functions in relation to tinnitus spectra and auditory threshold shift

COGNITIVE INTERFERENCE IN THE PERCEPTION OF PITCH AND LOUDNESS IN A FIVE-NOTE MUSICAL PATTERN DISSERTATION. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Music 175: Pitch II. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015

Activation of learned action sequences by auditory feedback

Precedence-based speech segregation in a virtual auditory environment

Overview of All Pixel Circuits for Active Matrix Organic Light Emitting Diode (AMOLED)

Metrical Accents Do Not Create Illusory Dynamic Accents

Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Musicians and nonmusicians sensitivity to differences in music performance

Absolute Pitch and Its Frequency Range

Transcription:

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION Michael Epstein 1,2, Mary Florentine 1,3, and Søren Buus 1,2 1Institute for Hearing, Speech, and Language 2Communications and Digital Signal Processing Center, ECE Dept. (440 DA) 3Dept. of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology (133 FR) Northeastern University, 360 Huntington Ave., Boston, MA 02115 U.S.A. Email: mepstein@ece.neu.edu, florentin@neu.edu, buus@neu.edu Abstract McFadden (1975) questioned the accuracy and reliability of the method of magnitude estimation for the measurement of loudness of tones that vary both in duration and level. He suggested that it produced unreliable results and should not be used to assess how loudness depends on stimulus duration. To examine this issue further, the present study obtained loudness functions for 5- and 200-ms tones in nine listeners using magnitude estimation. Loudness matches between the short and long tones were also obtained using an adaptive 2IFC procedure. The average amounts of temporal integration (defined as the level difference between equally loud short and long tones) obtained with the two procedures show good agreement. However, this may not apply to individual listeners. Some listeners show poor agreement, whereas others show good agreement. These results indicate that magnitude estimation provides a rapid and accurate means for assessing group-average loudness functions for tones of different durations. Nonetheless, it appears that magnitude estimation often is too susceptible to judgment biases and variability to reveal detailed features of individual listeners loudness functions for tones of various durations, even if it does reveal their general form. Magnitude estimation has frequently been used to measure the growth of loudness. As a procedure, it has received its share of criticism (e.g., McFadden, 1975; Poulton, 1989) as well as praise (e.g., Stevens, 1975; Hellman and Zwislocki, 1964). Previous experiments using magnitude estimation to assess temporal integration have had mixed success. Stevens and Hall (1966) attempted to obtain information about the growth of loudness for bursts of white noise with different durations using magnitude estimation. They measured loudness across a relatively wide range of levels (36 to 109 db SPL) and assumed a simple power-function representation. They found that the average slope of the loudness functions were nearly the same for noise bursts ranging in duration from 5 to 500 ms. They did not report on the variability, but the reported data appear to be internally consistent. On the other hand, McFadden (1975) found magnitude estimation to be unsuitable for obtaining loudness functions for pure tones that varied both in duration and level. His data showed very

large variability and his results were inconsistent with other measures of loudness as a function of duration. It is unclear why the results of these two studies differ so dramatically. The purpose of the present study is to evaluate the suitability of magnitude estimation as a method for measuring loudness functions for pure tones of different durations. In order to assess this, magnitude estimation data are compared with loudness matches to examine their internal consistency. Procedure All stimuli were presented monaurally via headphones. Listeners were seated in a double-wall sound-attenuating booth. 1. Absolute Thresholds Absolute thresholds were measured monaurally for 1-kHz tones with 5- and 200-ms durations. Measurements were performed using a three-down, one-up adaptive staircase method and a two-interval, two-alternative, forced-choice paradigm with feedback. 2. Absolute Magnitude Estimation Each listener was asked to rate the loudness of individual tones by assigning a number whose magnitude matched the tone s loudness. The number was typed on a computer-microterminal keypad. No reference or range was given as a basis for this judgment. The 5- and 200-ms tones were presented in mixed order at levels ranging from 5 db SL to 100 db SPL for the 200- ms tones and 110 db SPL for the 5-ms tones. Ten magnitude estimates were obtained for each level and duration. The trials were chosen by selecting each new stimulus level and duration randomly from the set of possibilities that met the following criteria: The SL needed to be within 30 db of the level in the previous trial for tones of the same duration and within 25 db for the other duration. The final estimates were obtained as the geometric mean of the ten estimates completed for each duration and level. 3. Loudness Matching The final part of the experiment consisted of loudness matches between 5- and 200-ms stimuli. This was performed using a roving-level two-alternative forced-choice adaptive procedure similar to that used by Buus et al. (1999). This procedure obtains ten concurrent loudness matches by randomly interleaving ten adaptive tracks. Five of these tracks varied the 5-ms tone and five varied the 200-ms tone. The fixed stimulus for each of the five tracks was set to different SLs between 5 and 85 db in 20-dB steps. On each trial, the listener heard two tones separated by a 600-ms interstimulus interval. The fixed-level tone followed the variable tone or the reverse with equal a priori probability. The listener s task was to indicate which sound was louder by pressing a key on the response terminal. The level of the variable tone was adjusted according to a simple up-down method. If the listener indicated that the variable tone was the louder one, its level was reduced; otherwise it was increased. The step size was 5 db until the second reversal, after which it was 2 db. Each track ended after nine reversals and the average level of the last four reversals was used as an estimate of the point of subjective equality. This procedure made the variable tone

converge towards a level at which it was judged louder than the fixed tone in 50% of the trials (Levitt, 1971). Results and Discussion Figure 1 shows the geometric mean of nine listeners loudness functions obtained using magnitude estimation for long and short tones. The loudness functions have a mid-to-high-level slope of about 0.18. This is lower than the frequently reported slope of 0.3 (Hellman, 1991), but it is within the range of previously reported slopes (cf. Viemeister and Bacon, 1988). 30 200 ms 10 5 ms 1 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 db SPL Figure 1. Geometric mean of nine listeners loudness functions for long and short tones obtained with magnitude estimation. The average amount of temporal integration defined as the level difference between equally loud short and long tones is shown in Figure 2. The open circles show the mean loudnessmatching results and the line shows the amount of temporal integration derived by averaging level differences between points yielding equal magnitude estimates for each listener. In other words, the line shows the average horizontal distance between the long and short loudness functions. The temporal-integration functions obtained from loudness matching and magnitude estimation (Fig. 2) agree with one another and are consistent with other similar studies (Florentine et al., 1996; Florentine et al., 1998; Buus et al., 1999) in both magnitude and form. The amount of temporal integration varies non-monotonically with level and is largest at moderate levels.

45 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 Magnitude Estimation Loudness Matches 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 110 Level of 5-ms Tone [db SPL] Figure 2. Temporal integration of loudness derived from loudness matches and magnitude estimation; Error bars indicate the standard deviation of the means of 9 subjects. However, individual data (not shown here due to space constraints) are highly variable. Some listeners exhibit good agreement between loudness matches and magnitude estimation, but others do not. The general form of the loudness functions for all listeners was clear, but detailed information was obscured by variability in most cases. It is noteworthy that some listeners produce results with enough variability to prevent precise details of loudness functions of different durations to be obtained using magnitude estimation, although the average data appear accurate. Whereas variability of magnitude estimation appears to obscure details of loudness functions in individual listeners, other procedures, such as cross-modality matching using string length (Florentine et al., this volume) seem able to reveal such details. Cross-modality matching appeared to be more successful with the same listeners and comparable instruction and training. Conclusion Magnitude estimation of loudness for tones with different durations produces fairly high variability in individual listeners. Some listeners were able to provide reliable results that were consistent with direct loudness matches, but others were not. Therefore, experimenters should use caution when using magnitude estimation to determine individuals loudness functions for

pure tones of various durations. Mean data, however, appear to be useful in assessing the shape of the loudness function for tones of different durations. Acknowledgment This research was supported by NIH/NIDCD grant R01DC02241. References Buus, S., Florentine, M. and Poulsen, T. (1999). Temporal integration of loudness in listeners with hearing losses of primarily cochlear origin. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 105, 3464-3480. Florentine, M., Buus, S. and Poulsen, T. (1996). Temporal integration of loudness as a function of level. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 99, 1633-1644. Florentine, M., Buus, S. and Robinson, M. (1998). Temporal integration of loudness under partial masking. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 104, 999-1007. Florentine, M., Epstein, M., and Buus, S. This volume. Hellman, R. P. (1991). Loudness scaling by magnitude scaling: Implications for intensity coding. In: G. A. Gescheider and S. J. Bolanowski (Eds.), Ratio Scaling of Psychological Magnitude: In Honor of the Memory of S. S. Stevens, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hellman, R. P. and Zwislocki, J. J. (1964). Loudness function of a 1000-cps tone in the presence of a masking noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 36, 1618-1627. Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 49, 467-477. McFadden, D. (1975). Duration-intensity reciprocity for equal loudness. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 57, 702-704. Poulton, E. C. (1989). Bias in Quantifying Judgments. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Stevens, J. C. and Hall, J. W. (1966). Brightness and loudness as a function of stimulus duration. Perc. Psychophys., 1, 319-327. Stevens, S.S. (1975). Psychophysics. New York: Wiley. Viemeister, N. F. and Bacon, S. P. (1988). Intensity discrimination, increment detection, and magnitude estimation for 1-kHz tones. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 84, 172-178.