SWING, SWING ONCE MORE: RELATING TIMING AND TEMPO IN EXPERT JAZZ DRUMMING

Similar documents
The effect of exposure and expertise on timing judgments in music: Preliminary results*

On time: the influence of tempo, structure and style on the timing of grace notes in skilled musical performance

Swing Ratios and Ensemble Timing in Jazz Performance: Evidence for a Common Rhythmic Pattern

A cross-cultural comparison study of the production of simple rhythmic patterns

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm and Timing 1

Modeling the Effect of Meter in Rhythmic Categorization: Preliminary Results

Music Performance Panel: NICI / MMM Position Statement

On the contextual appropriateness of performance rules

About Giovanni De Poli. What is Model. Introduction. di Poli: Methodologies for Expressive Modeling of/for Music Performance

Finger motion in piano performance: Touch and tempo

MUCH OF THE WORLD S MUSIC involves

Effects of Tempo on the Timing of Simple Musical Rhythms

The Formation of Rhythmic Categories and Metric Priming

However, in studies of expressive timing, the aim is to investigate production rather than perception of timing, that is, independently of the listene

A STATISTICAL VIEW ON THE EXPRESSIVE TIMING OF PIANO ROLLED CHORDS

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

MPATC-GE 2042: Psychology of Music. Citation and Reference Style Rhythm and Meter

Smooth Rhythms as Probes of Entrainment. Music Perception 10 (1993): ABSTRACT

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

2005 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. The Influence of Pitch Interval on the Perception of Polyrhythms

On music performance, theories, measurement and diversity 1

Matthew W. Butterfield

The influence of musical context on tempo rubato. Renee Timmers, Richard Ashley, Peter Desain, Hank Heijink

y POWER USER MUSIC PRODUCTION and PERFORMANCE With the MOTIF ES Mastering the Sample SLICE function

Human Preferences for Tempo Smoothness

Construction of a harmonic phrase

Tapping to Uneven Beats

LESSON 1 PITCH NOTATION AND INTERVALS

THERE IS A QUALITY OF MUSIC that makes people EXPERIENCING GROOVE INDUCED BY MUSIC: CONSISTENCY AND PHENOMENOLOGY

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

Differences in Metrical Structure Confound Tempo Judgments Justin London, August 2009

The Relationship Between Auditory Imagery and Musical Synchronization Abilities in Musicians

FREE music lessons from Berklee College of Music

Polyrhythms Lawrence Ward Cogs 401

Timing variations in music performance: Musical communication, perceptual compensation, and/or motor control?

EMBODIED EFFECTS ON MUSICIANS MEMORY OF HIGHLY POLISHED PERFORMANCES

Automatic meter extraction from MIDI files (Extraction automatique de mètres à partir de fichiers MIDI)

Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

Rhythmic Dissonance: Introduction

The Beat Alignment Test (BAT): Surveying beat processing abilities in the general population

Francesco Villa. Playing Rhythm. Advanced rhythmics for all instruments

ATOMIC NOTATION AND MELODIC SIMILARITY

Interacting with a Virtual Conductor

Perceiving temporal regularity in music

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

A structurally guided method for the decomposition of expression in music performance

Effects of articulation styles on perception of modulated tempos in violin excerpts

Trevor de Clercq. Music Informatics Interest Group Meeting Society for Music Theory November 3, 2018 San Antonio, TX

Temporal control mechanism of repetitive tapping with simple rhythmic patterns

Zooming into saxophone performance: Tongue and finger coordination

Lab #10 Perception of Rhythm and Timing

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

Correlation between Groovy Singing and Words in Popular Music

This project builds on a series of studies about shared understanding in collaborative music making. Download the PDF to find out more.

Chapter 2: Beat, Meter and Rhythm: Simple Meters

Rhythm together with melody is one of the basic elements in music. According to Longuet-Higgins

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Experiment on adjustment of piano performance to room acoustics: Analysis of performance coded into MIDI data.

Measuring Musical Rhythm Similarity: Further Experiments with the Many-to-Many Minimum-Weight Matching Distance

Playing the Accent - Comparing Striking Velocity and Timing in an Ostinato Rhythm Performed by Four Drummers

8 th Grade Concert Band Learning Log Quarter 1

drumlearn ebooks Fast Groove Builder by Karl Price

The Concepts and Acoustical Characteristics of Groove in. Japan

hprints , version 1-1 Oct 2008

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

Teaching Total Percussion Through Fundamental Concepts

Musical Entrainment Subsumes Bodily Gestures Its Definition Needs a Spatiotemporal Dimension

Variability oftiming in expressive piano performance increases with interval duration

Measuring & Modeling Musical Expression

Chords not required: Incorporating horizontal and vertical aspects independently in a computer improvisation algorithm

Bach-Prop: Modeling Bach s Harmonization Style with a Back- Propagation Network

A Beat Tracking System for Audio Signals

Controlling Musical Tempo from Dance Movement in Real-Time: A Possible Approach

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Matching the rule parameters of PHRASE ARCH to performances of Träumerei : a preliminary study

Rhythm. Pattern Generator

Hip Hop Robot. Semester Project. Cheng Zu. Distributed Computing Group Computer Engineering and Networks Laboratory ETH Zürich

BEAT AND METER EXTRACTION USING GAUSSIFIED ONSETS

Polymetric Rhythmic Feel for a Cognitive Drum Computer

An Empirical Comparison of Tempo Trackers

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

TEMPO AND BEAT are well-defined concepts in the PERCEPTUAL SMOOTHNESS OF TEMPO IN EXPRESSIVELY PERFORMED MUSIC

The Human, the Mechanical, and the Spaces in between: Explorations in Human-Robotic Musical Improvisation

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

Detecting Audio-Video Tempo Discrepancies between Conductor and Orchestra

Tempo Fluctuation in Two Versions of Body and Soul by John Coltrane and Dexter Gordon

Activation of learned action sequences by auditory feedback

Sensorimotor synchronization with chords containing tone-onset asynchronies

COLLEGE OF PIPING AND DRUMMING SNARE DRUMMING LEVEL ONE / PRELIMINARY. Syllabus and Resources. The Royal New Zealand Pipe Bands Association

CLASSIFICATION OF MUSICAL METRE WITH AUTOCORRELATION AND DISCRIMINANT FUNCTIONS

Skip Length and Inter-Starvation Distance as a Combined Metric to Assess the Quality of Transmitted Video

COLLEGE OF PIPING AND DRUMMING BASS AND TENOR DRUMMING LEVEL ONE / PRELIMINARY. Syllabus and Resources. The Royal New Zealand Pipe Bands Association

Music Theory 4 Rhythm Counting Second Chances Music Program

2013 Assessment Report. Music Level 1

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

ATSSB Bb clarinet (revised February 2016) Artistic Studies Book I from the French School David Hite/Southern Music

Rhythmic Studies for All Instruments Volume 1. by Tony Moreno. Muse Eek Publishing Company New York, NY

Durham Research Online

ESTIMATING THE ERROR DISTRIBUTION OF A TAP SEQUENCE WITHOUT GROUND TRUTH 1

Transcription:

Swing Once More 471 SWING ONCE MORE: RELATING TIMING AND TEMPO IN EXPERT JAZZ DRUMMING HENKJAN HONING & W. BAS DE HAAS Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands SWING REFERS TO A CHARACTERISTIC LONG-SHORT subdivision of the beat that is generally considered a crucial aspect that contributes to the quality of a jazz or pop performance. The current study measures this pattern (referred to as the swing ratio ) at different tempi in jazz drumming. The experimental setup differs from earlier studies in a number of ways. First, swing ratios were systematically measured at different beat durations in a musically realistic range. Second, repeated performances were collected to check for consistency. Third, drummers were asked to perform on a full MIDI drum kit. The results show that professional jazz drummers have enormous control over their timing. Nevertheless, the swing ratio is not kept constant, but it is systematically adapted to a global tempo. As such, this study provides further support for the hypothesis that expressive timing generally does not scale with tempo. Received July 26, 2007, accepted January 27, 2008. Key words: music performance, drumming, jazz, timing, tempo GROOVE, DRIVE, AND SHUFFLE are just a few of the many terms that are used SWING, by musicians to describe the timing patterns that are common to jazz and pop music. Groove and swing are generally considered crucial aspects that contribute to the quality of a jazz performance, as well as listeners appreciation of it (Iyer, 2002; Madison, 2001). Swing, the topic of this study, refers to the structural property that the beat (usually the quarter note pulse) is subdivided into unequal units, a characteristic longshort pattern that effectively changes the ratio between pairs of notes from 1:1 to 2:1 or even higher ratios (see Figure 1a) to give a sense of groove or swing. Swing rhythm has a rich cultural background (Gridley, 1985) and forms the basis of many grooves in modern music, ranging from rumba and jazz to funk and hip hop (Butler, 2006; Butterfield, 2006). Swing also denotes a jazz style, originating in the 1930s, in which this subdivision is a prominent feature (Schuller, 1989). Jazz swing has been a topic of several empirical studies (for a review see Friberg & Sundström, 2002). We will review those that focus on jazz drumming, swing, and the role of tempo, the topic of this research report. Collier and Collier (1996) presented analyses of drummers swing ratio as a function of tempo. They asked three drummers to perform a swing pattern (see cymbal pattern in Figure 1b) on a single MIDI drum pad with a cymbal sound in two conditions: one with a natural swing feel, the other in strict triple time. The swing ratios were measured at a wide range of different tempi, from extremely slow (a beat duration of 2400 ms) to relatively fast (a beat duration of 214 ms). In the swing condition one drummer performed with a clear increase in swing ratio with decreasing tempo (see AKC, the raising line in Figure 2a in color plate section). The other drummers showed quite a different pattern, both of them with a slight peak in the swing ratio in the intermediate tempo range (EWP and RAP in Figure 2a in color plate section). However, since only mean swing ratios were reported, it is unclear how much control these drummers had over their performances. Furthermore, there was no control for tempo drift, which might have influenced the measurements. Friberg and Sundström (2002) analyzed audio recordings of four internationally known jazz drummers, measuring the ride cymbal pattern in the audio signal. They found quite some variability in the performances. Participant AN had the smallest variation within each excerpt, and TW showed the largest variation, especially at slow tempi (see Figure 2b in color plate section). 1 The tempo range of the recordings used was between approximately 110-330 bpm. The authors concluded that swing ratio decreased with increasing tempo, suggesting the relation between swing ratio and 1 It is unclear whether tempo drift played a role in this study. Music Perception VOLUME 25, ISSUE 5, PP. 471 476, ISSN 0730-7829, ELECTRONIC ISSN 1533-8312 2008 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSION TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS S RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS WEBSITE, HTTP://WWW.UCPRESSJOURNALS.COM/REPRINTINFO.ASP. DOI:10.1525/MP.2008.25.5.471

472 Henkjan Honing & W. Bas de Haas FIGURE 1. Swing and music notation. (A) Though commonly written as eighth notes (1:1), swing eighth notes are typically performed somewhere between triple (2:1) or dotted (3:1) eighths. (B) The swing excerpt as used in this study. The ride cymbal pattern was used for measuring the swing ratio. tempo to be linear. Furthermore, they found the absolute duration of the short note in the long-short pattern of the swing rhythm to be constant at about 100 ms for the medium to short beat durations (see Figure 3a in color plate section), suggesting a threshold on note duration that was argued to be due to perceptual factors. However, the systematic probing of the effect of tempo was constrained by the relatively small set of available recordings. In summary, both studies showed a tempo-dependent swing ratio. Swing performance is not invariant under tempo transposition, that is, it cannot be transposed in tempo by multiplying all durations with a constant factor (if swing would be tempo-independent, it would match a straight horizontal line in Figure 2). This is in line with the more general hypothesis that expressive timing in music performance does not scale proportionally with tempo. This hypothesis referred to as the tempo-specific timing hypothesis was tested in a series of perceptual studies (Honing, 2006; Honing & Ladinig, 2006, 2008) using commercial recordings from the classical, jazz, and rock repertoire. These web-based listening experiments showed that listeners are able, on the basis of expressive timing judgments, to decide whether an audio recording is real or tempo-transformed (i.e., originally performed at another tempo, but made similar in overall tempo to the real recording). In summary, both in perception and performance, timing appears to be tempo-dependent. In the current study we try to replicate the findings in music performance, using an experimental setup in which three professional jazz drummers perform a swing fragment on a full MIDI drum kit. This experiment differs from the earlier studies in a number of ways. First, we systematically probed the swing ratio at different beat durations in a musically realistic range (between approximately 100-240 bpm). This was done to complement the earlier studies that presented fewer measurements at larger beat durations (slower tempi). Second, we collected repeated performances to check for consistency, given the within excerpt variability for different drummers reported in Friberg and Sundström (2002) (N.B. Collier & Collier, 1996 does not report on a measure of variability). Third, we asked drummers to perform on a full MIDI drum kit, which was conceived as a trade-off between a real concert situation (like some of the recordings used in Friberg and Sundström) and a single drum pad lab setup (like Collier & Collier), balancing experimental control with the ecological validity of the measurements obtained. We will focus here on three questions: How much control do drummers actually have over their swing ratio? Does swing ratio indeed scale linearly with tempo (as suggested by Friberg and Sundström, 2002, but not by Collier & Collier, 1996)? And is there indeed a threshold in note duration around 100 ms (as suggested by Friberg and Sundström)? Method Participants Three professional drummers participated in the experiment. JK, JL, and MS are well-known Dutch jazz musicians, each professionally active between fifteen (JK) and thirty (MS) years and formally trained between ten (MS)

Swing Once More 473 and sixteen (JL) years at established conservatories, in styles ranging from bebop and cool to more recent genres. Materials The musical material consisted of a typical jazz swing excerpt (see Figure 1b). The ride cymbal pattern was used for extracting the swing ratios. An audio example of the excerpt was presented once at the beginning of the experiment (with metronomic 2:1 eighth note ratios at tempo 174 bpm), as part of a briefing on what to expect, and with a 5-10 minute interval before the first recording was made. The musical score (see Figure 1b) was available throughout the recording session. Equipment The participants performed on a Yamaha DTXpress III MIDI drum kit. The MIDI output was recorded using Steinberg Nuendo (Version 3.2) with a typical MIDI delay of 3-10 ms (Loy, 1985). The drum sounds of the drum kit (sample set funky sound ), along with a metronome, were presented over headphones (Behringer HPS3000). All three participants were used to studio recording and familiar with performing in such a semi-artificial setup. Procedure The participants performed the swing excerpt (see Figure 1b) at nine different beat durations: 250, 300, 340, 380, 420, 460, 500, 540, and 580 ms. They were instructed to play naturally, yet only the notated notes. A trial started with a metronome indicating the desired tempo (a randomized selection of the nine beat durations), after which 16 repetitions were performed. A metronome was presented along with each repetition to control for tempo drift. Each participant performed two trials, resulting in 64 measures (16 2 2) per tempo per drummer. Analysis The data were preprocessed and restructured using custom made software in Common Lisp (Steele, 1990), removing, e.g., spurious notes and occasional double onsets. The statistical analyses were done using SPSS (Version 15.0). Inter-onset intervals (IOIs) were calculated by taking the difference of two subsequent eighth note onsets in the cymbal pattern (see Figure 1b). These were related to the score duration, effectively transforming the data to a l/tempo measurement, and then log-transformed. These measurements are referred to as a log-transformed normalized IOI profile (Desain & Honing, 1994). 2 Results The overall results are shown in Figures 2c and 3b (see color plate section). We will discuss these below in more detail. Consistency With regard to the question of control we looked at two measures. First, standard deviations of swing ratios showed that all three drummers had a similarly low variability in producing the swing ratios (see error bars in Figure 2c in color plate section), especially when compared to those reported in the Friberg and Sundström (2002) study (see Figure 2b in color plate section). Second, we looked at correlations within tempi as a measure of consistency between the two trials. Average correlations within tempi of the log-transformed normalized IOI profile 3 revealed that each drummer performed relatively consistently (JK: r(162) =.49, p <.0001; JL: r(162) =.60, p <.0001; MS: r(162) =.66, p <.0001). 4 While the drummers showed a highly consistent swing ratio and performed with little variability, there were considerable differences between their individual drumming styles. This difference was confirmed by an 2 A simple duration scale (consisting of raw IOIs) cannot be used to calculate correlations between timing profiles because different note durations in the score would swamp any timing effects and simply result in high correlations. A beat duration scale, the inverse of a tempo scale, does not suffer from that difficulty. In addition, these 1/tempo measurements are log-transformed arguing that consistency in performances measured by correlation of log duration (or log tempo) would theoretically coincide better with perceived similarity of timing patterns. Repp (1994) acknowledged this issue and consequently adapted the use of log-transformed IOIs. In fact, log-transformed IOIs make it more difficult to show nonproportional scaling. 3 See Footnote 2 and Desain and Honing (1994, p. 288) for a discussion on this particular transformation. IOIs are normalized with respect to a triplet interpretation of the score (see Figure 1b), i.e., using the ratio 2:1 as the norm, instead of 1:1 (as notated). 4 Normalization has a large effect on these correlations. When the reference is taken to be a notation in eighth notes (i.e., 1:1 instead 2:1) the correlations of log-transformed IOIs become.99,.98, and.99 respectively. These correlations should hence be interpreted in relative terms, not their absolute value.

474 Henkjan Honing & W. Bas de Haas TABLE 1. Average Correlations Between Tempi. Beat Duration Drummer Beat Duration 580 540 500 460 420 380 340 300 250 JK 580 1 540.24 1 500.46.33 1 462.04.44.22 1 420.11.39.25.58 1 380.27.35.36.33.32 1 340.50.18.41.14.15.38 1 300.52.23.41.17.29.41.44 1 250.43.29.13.43.33.03.26.28 1 Average.25 JL 580 1 540.35 1 500.25.51 1 462.39.47.72 1 420.35.57.76.73 1 380.35.54.78.74.79 1 340.21.38.47.44.50.50 1 300.40.46.44.57.53.48.30 1 250.43.58.72.77.81.75.55.63 1 Average.10 MS 580 1 540.75 1 500.75.71 1 462.71.71.76 1 420.67.77.72.74 1 380.78.80.70.72.72 1 340.38.51.50.53.51.49 1 300.23.28.18.19.32.23.08 1 250.51.61.54.62.74.49.41.32 1 Average.25 Grand Average.20 ANOVA with drummer and tempo as factors (3 and 9 levels, respectively), and with swing ratio as the dependent variable. As was already suggested by Figure 2c, the influence of tempo was highly significant, F(8,3419) = 1113.82, p <.0001, h p2 =.72, as was the effect of drummer, F(2,3419) = 376.94, p <.0001, h p2 =.18, and the interaction of tempo and drummer, F(16,3419) = 177.65, p <.0001, h p2 =.45. 5 In short, each 5 These results are different from those reported in Friberg and Sundström (2002, p. 338). drummer had a significantly different swing ratio for each tempo. Swing Ratio as a Function of Tempo Next we looked at how the swing ratio was adapted to the overall tempo. As a measure of the relation between expressive timing and tempo, for each drummer correlations of log-transformed normalized IOI profiles were calculated between tempi. Table 1 shows the average correlations between tempi. Overall these correlations were very low, r(321) =.20, p <.0001, indicating

Swing Once More 475 that swing timing did not scale proportionally with tempo. However, no evidence was found for a linear interpretation. 6 Apparently, swing ratio does not simply scale linearly with tempo. Absolute Duration as a Function of Tempo Finally, we looked at the absolute duration of the second note as a function of tempo. Friberg and Sundström (2002) suggested that there was a lower limit to the duration of the second note (see dotted line in Figure 3 in color plate section). In this study however, we found the second note duration to be linear in beat duration, r(3446) =.94, p <.0001, without support for a lower limit around 100 ms. (There is one peculiar difference: JL s performance at a beat duration of 250 ms.) Discussion The results show that professional jazz drummers have enormous control over their timing. They are able to time their performance with a precision of milliseconds over a considerable amount of repetitions (see Figure 3b in color plate section). Similar levels of control have been reported in the expressive timing literature (Clarke, 1999; Palmer, 1999) mainly focusing on classical piano music. Interestingly, even when drummers are in complete control (i.e., not dependent on interaction with other musicians, such as the data presented in Friberg & Sundström, 2002), the swing ratio is not kept constant, but is systematically adapted to the overall tempo. While the results presented in Friberg and Sundström are based on a small and relatively arbitrary set of recordings, the approach of analyzing real recordings is an attractive alternative to the method used in the current study. However, the challenge is to find recordings of the same pieces in a variety of tempi, which is not always easy (cf. Honing & Ladinig, 2008). 6 Due to space restrictions we refer the reader to Haas and Honing (2007) and Haas (2007) for alternative interpretations and a preliminary model. Furthermore, we found no evidence for the swing ratio to scale linearly with tempo, as was suggested in Friberg and Sundström (2002). While there is some evidence for linearity between beat durations of 250 ms and 350 ms (see Figure 2c in color plate section), at longer beat durations the swing ratio seems to stabilize around a swing ratio close to 2.2:1. Clearly, a more complex model is needed to capture the scaling of swing ratio with tempo in jazz drumming. In Haas and Honing (2007) some alternatives are explored. However, the purpose of the current study was not to model swing, but to see whether we could find evidence for tempo-specific timing in jazz drumming. In accordance with some earlier studies (e.g., Desain & Honing, 1994; Friberg & Sundström, 2002), and contrasting other studies suggesting proportional scaling (e.g., MacKenzie & Van Eerd, 1990; Repp, 1994), this study provides further support for the tempo-specific timing hypothesis (Honing, 2006) jazz experts adapt their timing to the tempo of their performance to obtain (or sustain) the effect of swing. Author Note Thanks are due to Joost Kroon, Joost Lijbaart, and Marcel Serierse for their time and great drumming. Leon Dubbers is thanked for helping with the recordings and providing the MIDI drum kit. We thank Leigh M. Smith, Anders Friberg, and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on an earlier draft. Last but not least, Anders Friberg and Geoffrey Collier are thanked for generously sharing their measurements. W. Bas de Haas is currently at Utrecht University. His research was partly supported by the Dutch ICES/KIS III bsik project MultimediaN. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Henkjan Honing, Music Cognition Group, ILLC/Universiteit van Amsterdam, Nieuwe Doelenstraat 16, 1012 CP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-MAIL: honing@uva.nl References BUTLER, M. J. (2006). Unlocking the groove: Rhythm, meter, and musical design in electronic dance music. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. BUTTERFIELD, M. W. (2006). Power of anacrusis: Engendered feeling in groove-based musics. Music Theory Online, 12, 4. CLARKE, E. F. (1999). Rhythm and timing in music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 473-500). New York: Academic Press. COLLIER, G., & COLLIER, J. (1996). The swing rhythm in jazz. In B. Pennycook & E. Costa-Giomi (Eds.), Proceedings of the

476 Henkjan Honing & W. Bas de Haas 4th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 477-480). Montreal: McGill University. DESAIN, P., & HONING, H. (1994). Does expressive timing in music performance scale proportionally with tempo? Psychological Research, 56, 285-292. FRIBERG, A., & SUNDSTRÖM, A. (2002). Swing ratios and ensemble timing in jazz performance: Evidence for a common rhythmic pattern. Music Perception, 19, 333-349. GRIDLEY, M. C. (1985). Jazz styles: History and analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. HAAS, W. B. DE. (2007). The role of tempo in groove and swing timing. Unpublished master s thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands. HAAS, W. B. DE, & HONING, H. (2007). Groove, swing and the role of tempo: A model and some preliminary empirical evidence. In C. Beckett & M. Hall (Eds.), Proceedings of the Society for Music Perception and Cognition (p. 105). Montreal: Concordia University. HONING, H. (2006). Evidence for tempo-specific timing in music using a web-based experimental setup. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 780-786. HONING, H., & LADINIG, O. (2006). The effect of exposure and expertise on timing judgments: Preliminary results. In M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, & M. Costa (Eds.) Proceedings of the International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 80-85). Bologna, Italy: University of Bologna. HONING, H., & LADINIG, O. (2008). Exposure influences expressive timing judgments in music. Manuscript submitted for publication. IYER, V. (2002). Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming in African-American music. Music Perception, 19, 387 414. LOY, G. (1985). Musicians make a standard: The MIDI phenomenon. Computer Music Journal, 9, 8-26. MACKENZIE, C. L., & VAN EERD, D. L. (1990). Rhythmic precision in the performance of piano scales: Motor psychophysics and motor programming. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 375-408). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. MADISON, G. (2001). Different kinds of groove in jazz and dance music as indicated by listeners ratings. In H. Lappalainen (Ed.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Symposium on Systematic and Comparative Musicology and Third International Conference on Cognitive Musicology (pp. 108-112). Jyväskylä, Finland: University of Jyväskylä. PALMER, C. (1997). Music performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 115-138. REPP, B. (1994). Relational invariance of expressive microstructure across global tempo changes in music performance: An exploratory study. Psychological Research, 56, 269-284. SCHULLER, G. (1989). The swing era: The development of jazz, 1930-1945. Oxford: Oxford University Press. STEELE, G. L. (1990). Common lisp, the language (2nd ed.). Bedford, MA: Digital Press.

HENKJAN HONING & W. BAS DE HAAS, FIGURE 2. Swing ratio as a function of beat duration, as measured by (A) Collier and Collier (1996) (N.B. Showing only the measurements in the range faster than 650 ms), (B) Friberg and Sundström (2002), and (C) the current study. Error bars show the standard deviation. (N.B. Collier & Collier reported only means). HENKJAN HONING & W. BAS DE HAAS, FIGURE 3. Absolute duration of the second note of the swing rhythm as a function of beat duration, as measured by (A) Friberg and Sundström (2002) and in (B) the current study. The dotted line marks the 100 ms threshold as suggested by Friberg and Sundström. Error bars show the standard deviation (N.B. Friberg & Sundström reported only means).