Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations

Similar documents
Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones

Sound quality in railstation : users perceptions and predictability

On viewing distance and visual quality assessment in the age of Ultra High Definition TV

Embedding Multilevel Image Encryption in the LAR Codec

REBUILDING OF AN ORCHESTRA REHEARSAL ROOM: COMPARISON BETWEEN OBJECTIVE AND PERCEPTIVE MEASUREMENTS FOR ROOM ACOUSTIC PREDICTIONS

Motion blur estimation on LCDs

The Brassiness Potential of Chromatic Instruments

QUEUES IN CINEMAS. Mehri Houda, Djemal Taoufik. Mehri Houda, Djemal Taoufik. QUEUES IN CINEMAS. 47 pages <hal >

Noise assessment in a high-speed train

Compte-rendu : Patrick Dunleavy, Authoring a PhD. How to Plan, Draft, Write and Finish a Doctoral Thesis or Dissertation, 2007

Learning Geometry and Music through Computer-aided Music Analysis and Composition: A Pedagogical Approach

An overview of Bertram Scharf s research in France on loudness adaptation

Influence of lexical markers on the production of contextual factors inducing irony

A PRELIMINARY STUDY ON THE INFLUENCE OF ROOM ACOUSTICS ON PIANO PERFORMANCE

A study of the influence of room acoustics on piano performance

Spectral correlates of carrying power in speech and western lyrical singing according to acoustic and phonetic factors

Regularity and irregularity in wind instruments with toneholes or bells

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

Workshop on Narrative Empathy - When the first person becomes secondary : empathy and embedded narrative

Perceptual assessment of water sounds for road traffic noise masking

PaperTonnetz: Supporting Music Composition with Interactive Paper

No title. Matthieu Arzel, Fabrice Seguin, Cyril Lahuec, Michel Jezequel. HAL Id: hal

Visual Annoyance and User Acceptance of LCD Motion-Blur

Interactive Collaborative Books

On the Citation Advantage of linking to data

Laurent Romary. To cite this version: HAL Id: hal

Artefacts as a Cultural and Collaborative Probe in Interaction Design

A joint source channel coding strategy for video transmission

Reply to Romero and Soria

Effects of headphone transfer function scattering on sound perception

La convergence des acteurs de l opposition égyptienne autour des notions de société civile et de démocratie

Translating Cultural Values through the Aesthetics of the Fashion Film

Experiments on tone adjustments

A new conservation treatment for strengthening and deacidification of paper using polysiloxane networks

Natural and warm? A critical perspective on a feminine and ecological aesthetics in architecture

Open access publishing and peer reviews : new models

From SD to HD television: effects of H.264 distortions versus display size on quality of experience

The Diverse Environments Multi-channel Acoustic Noise Database (DEMAND): A database of multichannel environmental noise recordings

Musical instrument identification in continuous recordings

A new HD and UHD video eye tracking dataset

Analysis of car door closing sound quality

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Philosophy of sound, Ch. 1 (English translation)

Adaptation in Audiovisual Translation

Primo. Michael Cotta-Schønberg. To cite this version: HAL Id: hprints

Measures and models of real triodes, for the simulation of guitar amplifiers

Synchronization in Music Group Playing

Releasing Heritage through Documentary: Avatars and Issues of the Intangible Cultural Heritage Concept

Indexical Concepts and Compositionality

Translation as an Art

A comparative case study of indoor soundscape approach on objective analyses and subjective evaluations of libraries

Video summarization based on camera motion and a subjective evaluation method

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

Editing for man and machine

Spatial-frequency masking with briefly pulsed patterns

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Creating Memory: Reading a Patching Language

HUMAN PERCEPTION OF SINUSOIDAL ROTATIONAL STEERING WHEEL VIBRATION.

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

The Lecture Contains: Frequency Response of the Human Visual System: Temporal Vision: Consequences of persistence of vision: Objectives_template

Improvisation Planning and Jam Session Design using concepts of Sequence Variation and Flow Experience

Corpus-Based Transcription as an Approach to the Compositional Control of Timbre

Effect of room acoustic conditions on masking efficiency

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

Consistency of timbre patterns in expressive music performance

Opening Remarks, Workshop on Zhangjiashan Tomb 247

ANALYSIS-ASSISTED SOUND PROCESSING WITH AUDIOSCULPT

Acoustical evaluation of the Carnyx of Tintignac

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Hidden melody in music playing motion: Music recording using optical motion tracking system

The pitch of short-duration vibrato tones

Who s afraid of banal nationalism?

Multipitch estimation by joint modeling of harmonic and transient sounds

Pseudo-CR Convolutional FEC for MCVideo

Mechanical response characterization of saxophone reeds

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

Hybrid active noise barrier with sound masking

Vibratory and Acoustical Factors in Multimodal Reproduction of Concert DVDs

Decision Problem of Instrumentation in a Company involved in ISO 50001

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

A Comparative Study of Variability Impact on Static Flip-Flop Timing Characteristics

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Artifactualization: Introducing a new concept.

Glottal open quotient in singing: Measurements and correlation with laryngeal mechanisms, vocal intensity, and fundamental frequency

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

Some problems for Lowe s Four-Category Ontology

OMaxist Dialectics. Benjamin Lévy, Georges Bloch, Gérard Assayag

Limiting factors for the active suppression of structural chatter vibrations using machine s drives

Sonic Ambiances Bruitage -Recordings of the Swiss International Radio in the Context of Media Practices and Cultural Heritage

A Matlab toolbox for. Characterisation Of Recorded Underwater Sound (CHORUS) USER S GUIDE

Non-linear propagation characteristics in the evolution of brass musical instruments design

ASE 369 K Measurements and Instrumentation. LAB #9: Impulse-Force Hammer; Vibration of Beams

Multisensory approach in architecture education: The basic courses of architecture in Iranian universities

Swept-tuned spectrum analyzer. Gianfranco Miele, Ph.D

JOURNAL OF BUILDING ACOUSTICS. Volume 20 Number

ADVANCED PROCEDURES FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC NOISE EVALUATION

A framework for aligning and indexing movies with their script

The multimodal dining experience - A case study of space, sound and locality

Coming in and coming out underground spaces

Transcription:

Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations Carmen Rosa Hernandez, Etienne Parizet To cite this version: Carmen Rosa Hernandez, Etienne Parizet. Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations. Société Française d Acoustique. Acoustics 2012, Apr 2012, Nantes, France. 2012. <hal-00810661> HAL Id: hal-00810661 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00810661 Submitted on 23 Apr 2012 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of scientific research documents, whether they are published or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers. L archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Proceedings of the Acoustics 2012 Nantes Conference 23-27 April 2012, Nantes, France Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations C.R. Hernandez and E. Parizet Laboratoire Vibrations Acoustique INSA Lyon, 25 bis avenue Jean Capelle, Villeurbanne, F-69621 Lyon, France carmen-rosa.hernandez-yanez@insa-lyon.fr 1083

23-27 April 2012, Nantes, France Proceedings of the Acoustics 2012 Nantes Conference The existence of masking effects for vertical vibrations of seated subjects was investigated. In the first experiment, detection thresholds of sinusoids at six different frequencies (between 30 and 80 Hz) were estimated using a 3-AFC with a three-down one-up rule. Then, thresholds were estimated in the presence of a vibratory masking stimulus: a band pass noise (10 to 20 Hz) at three different levels (between 0.1 and 0.315 m/s 2 ). The presence of this band-pass noise increased the threshold of all studies frequencies. At detection, the overall level of the signal (masker noise plus sinusoid) was greater than the level of the masker noise alone by a value close to the just noticeable differences in level for this noise. Results suggested that the detection was related to a global energy level discrimination process. Finally, some indicators explaining this phenomenon are presented. 1 Introduction During daily activities, humans are exposed to multiple sources of vibratory stimuli. In very few cases it is a single stimulus; in most cases it is the result of the contribution of different signals. In the literature, some studies [1-9] analyze the human response to a single sinusoidal vibration stimulus, in the vertical direction, and the effect of some variables (frequency, duration, body support elements, body posture, etc.). In [5], the perception threshold of a vertical sinusoidal signal in the presence of a second sinusoidal stimulus to a lower frequency is presented; results suggest that the subjects can more easily detect the test signal when its frequency is close to the base signal. In this case, there are some indications that the presence of a second signal facilitates the detection of test stimulus, but the opposite possibility, i.e. the masking effect has not been explored. This study investigates the existence of masking effect for vertical vibration of seated subjects; the signal test is a sinusoidal vibratory stimulus and the mask is a band-pass noise vibratory stimulus. Some indicators that contribute to understand the mechanism of detection are presented. 2 Experimental Method All experiments were conducted using a test bench located in the Laboratoire Vibrations Acoustique (LVA- INSA de Lyon), France. A spring supported platform on which a seat is fixed, is connected to an electrodynamic shaker (type LDS V650). This transmits the vibration in the vertical direction to the seat of the bench; a data acquisition system (type NVGate OROS ) and a piezoelectric accelerometer (PCB Piezotronics, Model 333B32) which is placed at the bottom of the seat are used to analyze and record the movement. Using special software developed for this investigation, a vibration signal is generated. Then, the computer transmits this signal to an amplifier (type PA 1000L) through a data acquisition card (type Plug.n.DAQ, Roga). A flat circular base wood seat (0.305 m diameter and 0.02 m thick), is the surface through which the vibration is transmitted. The seat does not have any support for the back, head or arms. The subject is asked to sit down in an upright position as see in Figure 1. His feet are placed on a fixed platform. Some experiments were conducted at the same positions (no backrest and stationary footrest) [7]. The obtained thresholds were not similar than those obtained with not stationary footrest. In all experiments, the stimuli are vibratory signals in vertical direction. Two types of stimuli have been used: the masking and the test stimulus. Figure 1: Subject position during tests. The masker is a narrowband noise (10-20 Hz) at three different magnitude levels (100, 105 and 110 db). When both the masker and sinusoidal signals are simultaneously present, the risk of detection due to the values at hollow positions is high. In order to avoid this risk, the amplitude of signal test was multiplied by the contour of the mask stimulus. Therefore, the test stimulus is a sinusoidal modulated amplitude signal. The test frequency is between 30 to 80 Hz. Within this frequency range the movement influences, other than vertical direction, can be despised. To examine the masking effect, the difference between absolute and masked thresholds is required; hence, both thresholds have been estimated. Due to the variation in the overall level energy arising from the simultaneous presentation of two stimuli (test and mask stimuli), the just noticeable difference (JND) of the masking signal has been estimated at three different magnitudes levels (100, 105 and 110 db). All tests have been carried out using three alternative forced choice (3AFC) methods in conjunction with a threedown one-up rules algorithm. In all studies, the test and mask signal duration were 2.0 seconds (including 50 ms fade at the ends). The task of the subjects was to identify which interval was different from the two others. No feedback was provided to the subject. The order of test stimuli presentations was randomized. 1084

Proceedings of the Acoustics 2012 Nantes Conference The procedure for determining the thresholds and the JND was terminated after 10 reversals: a point where the stimulus magnitude reversed direction at either a peak or a trough. The thresholds and JND were calculated from the mean of the last four reversals, omitting the first two. The final value is the median of all participants. All parameters estimated in this study are expressed in terms of the acceleration: db and m/s 2 which is the recommended method for the quantification of human exposure to vibration in the relevant international standards [1]. Nine (3 female, 6 male) and twelve (3 female, 9 male) healthy subjects participated in thresholds and JND tests respectively. 23-27 April 2012, Nantes, France 50 Hz. The most important sensitivity change is between 30 and 50 Hz. 3 Results and Discussion 3.1 The absolute threshold The Individual, median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of the absolute threshold estimations are presented in Figure 2. Figure 3: The median and the inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of absolute threshold of whole-body vertical vibration, some data are based on [2] and [3]. 3.2 The masked threshold The masked threshold has been estimated by using a test stimulus at the frequencies 30, 35, 40, 50, 60 and 80 Hz; the mask signal was only at 110 db level amplitude. The individual, median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of masked threshold as frequency function are shown in Figure 4. Figure 2: The Individual, median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of the absolute threshold. Some differences between the values of individual thresholds across the frequencies range have been found. The median and the inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of absolute threshold are shown in Figure 3; the results of other studies are displayed for comparison. In general, the thresholds values are similar to those shown in Figure 3 in spite of the differences within the tests signals. An explicit comparison between the data is not possible because the differences conditions (psychophysical procedures, system for generating motion, stimuli, etc.) may influence the final absolute threshold value [10-11]. It can be observed that the frequency effect on the absolute threshold is not the same for all range. From the statistical analysis, with a significance level of 0.05 (for all the cases) a significant difference between 30 Hz and all other frequencies (except to 40 Hz) was detected. However, between 35 Hz and all other frequencies, the sensitivity change is very low (no significant differences), except for Figure 4: The Individual, median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of masked thresholds. Sensitivity differences between individual values were observed. Nevertheless, some similarity with respect to the trend of the masked threshold has been found. 1085

23-27 April 2012, Nantes, France Proceedings of the Acoustics 2012 Nantes Conference In Figure 5, are presented the median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of masked threshold estimated; to facilitate the analysis of results, the absolute threshold of perception is also displayed. Figure 6: Median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of masked threshold. Figure 5: The median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of absolute and masked threshold. Significant differences in all frequency range have been found between the absolute threshold and the masked threshold (0.05 significance level, Wilcoxon, p <0.05). The masked threshold exhibits a tendency to decrease as function of test signal frequency. Significant differences at most of frequencies of masked threshold were found (0.05 significance level, Wilcoxon, p <0.05). In general the masked threshold shown values considerably higher than those corresponding to the absolute threshold at the same frequency; the results suggest that detection of the test signal is affected by the presence of the mask signal at 110 db of amplitude level. Using the test signal only at 35 Hz and the mask signal at 100, 105 and 110 db magnitude levels, the masked threshold has been estimated. The masked threshold as a function of the mask signal level, is shown in Figure 6, the absolute threshold is shown too for a better analysis. The masked threshold values are significantly higher than the absolute threshold values (more than 10 db) at all mask signal levels. According to the results, it appears likely that the mask signal level affects the detection of the signal test at 35 Hz; the tendency suggests that the effect of mask signal level is more important as it rises. 3.3 The just noticeable difference (JND) The absolute values, median and inter-quartile range (25-75th percentiles) of JND in level estimated in the present investigation, are shown in the first line (*) Table 1. The JND in level reported by [3], [12], [13], and [14] are between 0.52 and 1.5 db, however those values were obtained from sinusoidal signals and they are considered only for reference purposes but not for direct comparison. Table 1: Just noticeable difference in level (absolute values) (*) [db], Minimum level energy expected (**) [db], Level energy reported (***) [db]. Mask signal level [db] 100 105 110 (*) 1.9 (1.4-2.5) 2.4 (1.9-3.2) 1.7(1.4-1.8) (**) 101.55 106.91 111.34 (***) 101.18 105.92 110.80 The minimum energy level required to detect a change in the mask signal (mask signal + JND), is shown in the second line (**) Table 1: those are the values at which should have been detected the energy change of the mask signal. The energy levels, at which the detection took place (mask signal + test signal), are shown in third line (***) Table 1: when comparing the expected energy level for detection with the value at which the detection in fact occurred, we observed some differences. A similar analysis was conducted for each of the subjects who participated to both experiences. In most cases (16 over 18), when the signal was detected the difference between the level of the combination (mask signal + JND) and the level of the masker alone, was lower than the JND's mentioned in Table 1. This can indicate that a frequency discrimination process occurred. Thus, the energy level required for this frequency discrimination is below than the level required to intensity shift discrimination. 1086

Proceedings of the Acoustics 2012 Nantes Conference 4 Conclusions In the present work, the frequency dependence of the absolute threshold is similar to the results presented in other studies; however, explicit comparison to this database is not possible. Disagreement can be attributed to differences in body posture and body support elements [4] and the use of different psychophysical methods [15]. Since the whole body vibration can be detected by vision, hearing and vestibular senses [2], other aspects such as the duration of the stimulus, different systems for vibratory movement generation, groups of subjects and the test environment, could also influence the final thresholds values. The stimulus detection in the presence of the masking signal has been prevented. Therefore, it is certainly clear that the masking phenomenon occurred. The masked thresholds values were considerably higher than those absolute thresholds. The threshold decreases as a function of test frequency and the mask effect is more important at high than at low intensity masker levels. Maybe frequency discrimination at the detection moment occurred. Some priority areas for future research are suggested: analyze the effect of the measurement method by using other psychophysical procedures; evaluate the minimum intensity masker level required to achieve the masking effect; assess the influence the bandwidth of mask signal. References 23-27 April 2012, Nantes, France [1] International Organization for Standardization ISO 2631-1 Part 1, Mechanical vibration and shockevaluation of human exposure to whole-body vibration part 1: General requirements, (1997) [2] M. Griffin, Handbook of human vibration, Academic Press, New York, (1991) [3] M. Bellmann, V. Mellert, H. Remmers, R. Weber, Experiments on the perception of whole-body vibration, Proceedings of the 35 th UK Group Meeting on Human Response to Vibration, University of Southampton, Southampton, 13-15 September, pp. 355-364, (2000) [4] M. Morioka, M. Griffin, Absolute thresholds for the perception of fore-and-aft, lateral, and vertical vibration at the hand, the seat, and the foot, Journal of Sound and Vibration 314, pp. 357-370, (2008) [5] F. Ljunggren, J. Wang, A. Agren, Human vibration perception from single and dual frequency components, Journal of Sound and Vibration 300, pp. 13-24, (2007) [6] T. Miwa, Evaluation methods for the vibration effect, part 1: measurements of threshold and equal sensation contours of whole body for vertical and horizontal vibrations, Industrial Health 2, pp. 183-205, (1967) [7] K. Parsons, M. Griffin, Whole-Body vibration perception thresholds, Journal of Sound and Vibration 121, pp. 237-258, (1988) [8] L. Louda, Perception and effect of the mixture of two vertical sinusoidal vibrations on sitting man. Work Environment Health, vol. 7, n 1, pp. 62-66, (1970) [9] K. Parsons, Vibration perception thresholds and their application to building vibration. Presented at the Meeting of the United Kingdom Informal Group on Human Response to Vibration, Edinburgh, (1981) [10] T. Fairley, M. Griffin, The apparent mass of the seated human body: vertical vibration. Journal of Biomechanics, vol 22, n 2, p 81-94, (1989) [11] P. Holmlund, L. Lundstrom, L. Lindberg, Mechanical impedance of the human body in vertical direction. Applied Ergonomics, vol 31, p 415-422, (2000) [12] Y. Matsumoto, S. Maeda, Y. Oji, Influence of frequency on difference thresholds for magnitude of vertical sinusoidal whole body vibration. Industrial Health, n 40, pp. 313 319, (2002) [13] M. Morioka, M. Griffin, Difference thresholds for intensity perception of whole-body vertical vibration: Effect of frequency and magnitude, Journal of Sound and Vibration 107 (1), pp. 620-624, (2000) [14] N. Forta, Vibration intensity difference thresholds. Ph.D. thesis, University of Southampton, (2009) [15] A. Hesse, Comparison of several psychophysical procedures with respect to threshold estimates, reproducibility and efficiency. Acustica, vol. 59, pp. 263 273, (1986) 1087