Compose yourself: The Emotional Influence of Music

Similar documents
MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

THE SOUND OF SADNESS: THE EFFECT OF PERFORMERS EMOTIONS ON AUDIENCE RATINGS

Can parents influence children s music preferences and positively shape their development? Dr Hauke Egermann

The relationship between properties of music and elicited emotions

Final Project: Music Preference. Mackenzie McCreery, Karrie Chen, Alexander Solomon

Therapeutic Sound for Tinnitus Management: Subjective Helpfulness Ratings. VA M e d i c a l C e n t e r D e c a t u r, G A

Expressive information

The Sound of Emotion: The Effect of Performers Emotions on Auditory Performance Characteristics

The Effect of Music Tempo on the Psychophysiological Measures of Stress

INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC

Environment Expression: Expressing Emotions through Cameras, Lights and Music

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

The psychological impact of Laughter Yoga: Findings from a one- month Laughter Yoga program with a Melbourne Business

12/7/2018 E-1 1

Katie Rhodes, Ph.D., LCSW Learn to Feel Better

Emotions perceived and emotions experienced in response to computer-generated music

The Effects of Stimulative vs. Sedative Music on Reaction Time

EMOTIONS IN CONCERT: PERFORMERS EXPERIENCED EMOTIONS ON STAGE

VivoSense. User Manual Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) Analysis Module. VivoSense, Inc. Newport Beach, CA, USA Tel. (858) , Fax.

Affective response to a set of new musical stimuli W. Trey Hill & Jack A. Palmer Psychological Reports, 106,

Foundation - MINIMUM EXPECTED STANDARDS By the end of the Foundation Year most pupils should be able to:

Interpretations and Effect of Music on Consumers Emotion

Exploring Relationships between Audio Features and Emotion in Music

Elements of Music. How can we tell music from other sounds?

Exercise 1: Muscles in Face used for Smiling and Frowning Aim: To study the EMG activity in muscles of the face that work to smile or frown.

Composing Affective Music with a Generate and Sense Approach

DEMENTIA CARE CONFERENCE 2014

Making music with voice. Distinguished lecture, CIRMMT Jan 2009, Copyright Johan Sundberg

Music Assessment Key Stage 3. Moving towards next step: A (creating and evaluating) Developing at that step: C (remembering and understanding)

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Klee or Kid? The subjective experience of drawings from children and Paul Klee Pronk, T.

The intriguing case of sad music

Quantifying Tone Deafness in the General Population

WASD PA Core Music Curriculum

Beyond Happiness and Sadness: Affective Associations of Lyrics with Modality and Dynamics

Perception of emotion in music in adults with cochlear implants

Lesson 14 BIOFEEDBACK Relaxation and Arousal

Does Music Directly Affect a Person s Heart Rate?

Scheme of Work for Music. Year 1. Music Express Year 1 Unit 1: Sounds interesting 1 Exploring sounds

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

Theatre of the Mind (Iteration 2) Joyce Ma. April 2006

HOW COOL IS BEBOP JAZZ? SPONTANEOUS

Assessment may include recording to be evaluated by students, teachers, and/or administrators in addition to live performance evaluation.

Music Mood Classification - an SVM based approach. Sebastian Napiorkowski

MODELING MUSICAL MOOD FROM AUDIO FEATURES AND LISTENING CONTEXT ON AN IN-SITU DATA SET

4th Grade Music Music

Affective Priming. Music 451A Final Project

Subjective Emotional Responses to Musical Structure, Expression and Timbre Features: A Synthetic Approach

Running head: THE EFFECT OF MUSIC ON READING COMPREHENSION. The Effect of Music on Reading Comprehension

PRESCHOOL (THREE AND FOUR YEAR-OLDS) (Page 1 of 2)

1. BACKGROUND AND AIMS

Does Music Effect your Heart Rate? By: Carson Buss and Breylin Soto. PHEOCS Investigation

German Center for Music Therapy Research

MEANINGS CONVEYED BY SIMPLE AUDITORY RHYTHMS. Henni Palomäki

Satoshi Kawase Soai University, Japan. Satoshi Obata The University of Electro-Communications, Japan. Article

RHYTHM. Simple Meters; The Beat and Its Division into Two Parts

Therapeutic Function of Music Plan Worksheet

MOTIVATION AGENDA MUSIC, EMOTION, AND TIMBRE CHARACTERIZING THE EMOTION OF INDIVIDUAL PIANO AND OTHER MUSICAL INSTRUMENT SOUNDS

Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription

2nd Grade Music Music

SUPER BOWL ADVERTISING 2017 TEASER REPORT

Zooming into saxophone performance: Tongue and finger coordination

Searching for the Universal Subconscious Study on music and emotion

Music and the emotions

Effects of Musical Tempo on Heart Rate, Brain Activity, and Short-term Memory Abstract

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PURE AND APPLIED RESEARCH IN ENGINEERING AND TECHNOLOGY

A Categorical Approach for Recognizing Emotional Effects of Music

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

STRAND I Sing alone and with others

Automatic Generation of Music for Inducing Physiological Response

3rd Grade Music Music


The Effects of Background Music on Non-Verbal Reasoning Tests

KINDERGARTEN-CURRICULUM MAP

Music Curriculum Map 2017/18

CHILDREN S CONCEPTUALISATION OF MUSIC

BRAIN-ACTIVITY-DRIVEN REAL-TIME MUSIC EMOTIVE CONTROL

Tinnitus: The Neurophysiological Model and Therapeutic Sound. Background

Electronic Musicological Review

Montana Instructional Alignment HPS Critical Competencies Music Grade 3

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

Thoughts and Emotions

Version 5: August Requires performance/aural assessment. S1C1-102 Adjusting and matching pitches. Requires performance/aural assessment

5th Grade Music Music

Physiological arousal response to differing musical genres

Walworth Primary School

MISS. Copyright 2004 by R. I. Kotov, S. B. Bellman & D. B. Watson MISS

The Healing Power of Music. Scientific American Mind William Forde Thompson and Gottfried Schlaug

Modulating musical reward sensitivity up and down with transcranial magnetic stimulation

A Comparison of Average Pitch Height and Interval Size in Major- and Minor-key Themes: Evidence Consistent with Affect-related Pitch Prosody

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

A prototype system for rule-based expressive modifications of audio recordings

Music, emotion, and time perception: the influence of subjective emotional valence and arousal?

Playing Body Percussion Playing on Instruments. Moving Choreography Interpretive Dance. Listening Listening Skills Critique Audience Etiquette

Discovering GEMS in Music: Armonique Digs for Music You Like

EXPECTATIONS at the end of this unit. some children will not have made so much progress and will:

The Effect of Musical Lyrics on Short Term Memory

Artificial Social Composition: A Multi-Agent System for Composing Music Performances by Emotional Communication

NFMHS Center for Arts and Media DANCE. Program Descriptions/ Audition Requirements

Psychophysiological measures of emotional response to Romantic orchestral music and their musical and acoustic correlates

Transcription:

1 Dr Hauke Egermann Director of York Music Psychology Group (YMPG) Music Science and Technology Research Cluster University of York hauke.egermann@york.ac.uk www.mstrcyork.org/ympg Compose yourself: The Emotional Influence of Music One of the most important motivations for listening to music is its emotional effect on us. Listeners often report that they listen to music to calm them down, to stimulate them, to bring them into a positive mood, or to experience emotions such as melancholy or nostalgia. Therefore, listening to music is a unique way to experience and engage with different contrasting emotions, helping us to understand and regulate our mood in conjunction with many different situations. This makes music an important part of our overall mental wellbeing. We at the York Music Psychology Group teamed-up with Bang & Olufsen to find out how music creates emotional responses in us. We conducted a study that shows, how listening to selected pieces of music elicit contrasting subjective and physiological responses that are emotionally-varied and at the same time enjoyed by listeners. When studying emotional responses to music, researchers often ask their participants to rate the intensity of several basic emotions that are thought to be culturally universal and often include happiness, fear, anger, sadness, and peacefulness (Zenter & Eerola, 010). In addition to that, they also measure how these emotions are accompanied by physiological changes in heart rate or skin conductance, which are both indicators of the arousal of the autonomous nervous system.

But why would music be able to induce such responses? Well, first we might have learned to associate certain musical characteristics with certain emotions. In that case, music would not be emotional directly. However, the compelling evidence presented in several studies speaks against the fact that emotional responses to music are only learned. To some degree, certain simple acoustical and musical features might be able to induce emotions in us. This can be observed, when musicians are asked to play an originally neutral piece of music with a certain emotional expression like that of happiness or sadness: studies have shown that those characteristics used when we express our emotions through our voice and body movements are then employed (Juslin & Laukka, 00; Giordano, et al., 014). For example, highly aroused emotional states are typically expressed with fast movements, calm states with slow. Happiness is expressed with a lot variability in the pitch of a spoken voice, whereas sadness is expressed with a lack of variability. Thus, musicians use the same expressive features that we also use when we express our feelings through speech or movement. This could mean that when we respond to music emotionally, it is because we feel the human emotional expressions in it. In this study, we wanted to show, how in four selected pieces of music, each with a typical expressive emotional character, induce contrasting emotional responses in listeners at the level of subjective emotional feelings and physiological arousal. Methods We recruited 0 participants within the University of York. Their mean age was 8 years (SD=10 years), 1 were female, and 18 were currently enrolled as students. We selected four music pieces that each featured a specific configuration of musical characteristics that are typical for one of four different emotional expressions (see Table 1).

Table 1. Description of Music Stimuli Piece No. Title Artist 1 Piece by Piece Into the Mist Happy Pharrell Eivør Williams Musical Characteristics Very fast Medium (70 Slow (48 BPM) (160 BPM) BPM) Legato/slow Staccato/fast Staccato/fast voice onsets voice onsets voice onsets Low High High Percussive Rough and Soft (low high (medium Sharp high (Strong high Eivør Tempo Articulation Loudness Timbre Musical Expression Peacefulness Happiness Anger 4 9 Crimes Damian Rice Medium (7 BPM) Legato/slow voice onsets Low Soft (low high Sadness Participants were invited to come in two equally-sized groups to the Rymer Auditorium at the Department of Music, University of York (see Figure 1A). The four stimuli were presented through high-end studio monitors in two orders: For the first group in the order as shown in Table 1, the second group listened to them in the reverse order. A B Figure 1. A) Participants in Group listening to music. B) Shimmer GSR+ sensor with skin conductance electrodes. Listener experiences were assessed using our Audience Response System on multiple levels: while participants filled in an online questionnaire with background

4 information before and after the experiment, they also retrospectively reported their subjective experience of the music presented after each piece using a 5-point intensity rating scale for feeling five basic emotions. For this, they were each equipped with an ipad mini. Additionally, we monitored throughout the experiment our participant s skin conductance and their heart rate. For skin conductance measurements, we attached two electrodes to their non-dominant hand s middle and index finger, and a photo plethysmograph sensor to their non-dominant side s ear lobe (see Figure 1B). To test if music increases or decreases physiological arousal, we additionally recorded a one-minute long silence period before every stimulus. The physiological data were sampled with Shimmer GSR+ sensors and subsequently processed in the programming environment Matlab. Here, we calculated the mean skin conductance and heart rate for every stimulus and listener. Results As Figure A shows that each of the four selected pieces induced a different typical emotional profile in ratings of subjective experience. Piece by piece featured a slow tempo, low dynamic range, and sung with a soft voice. These features are typically expressive of peacefulness and induced these feelings in our listeners. It had a very similar profile to 9 Crimes. This song, was also experienced as sad, a finding that we had expected. Unlike we expected, Into the Mist was not experienced as having any strong emotional quality at all, even though it featured very rough synthesiser sounds. Instead, it achieved its highest rating on the emotion fear. Finally, the fast and very rhythmic song Happy had the most positive effect on our listeners: it received the highest happiness ratings, and was not experienced negatively at all. Emotions such as anger, sadness, or fear would be normally experienced as negative and unpleasant in a non-musical context. However, in this study, when those emotions were induced through music, participants reported to experience them as pleasant. Figure B shows that on average, all pieces were rated as moreor-less pleasant. The song Happy, that received the highest happiness ratings, also received the highest pleasantness rating, followed in high pleasantness by 9 Crimes which also induced high sadness in our listeners.

5 A Mean Intensity Rating 5 4 1 Most intense emotion ever induced by music Happiness Peacefullness Anger Fear Sadness No emotion induced at all 5 Very Pleasant B Mean Pleasantness Rating 4 1 Very Unpleasant Figure. A) Mean emotion rating separated by emotion and music piece. B) Mean Pleasantness Ratings separated by piece. The physiological measurements presented in Figure A indicate that the music generally had a physiologically calming and relaxing effect on our listeners, as on average, skin conductance was lower during listening to the four pieces compared to the four silence recordings before each of the. Figure A also shows that this effect was strongest for 9 Crimes, which was also the song rated with the highest sadness ratings by listeners. Figure B shows mean heart rate during listening to the four pieces

A Group Mean Skin Conducdance (μs).5.5 1.75 Silence Music 6 B Group Mean Heart Rate BPM 1.5 7 71.5 71 70.5 70 69.5 69 Silence Music Figure 4. Physiological responses to four pieces. A) shows difference between skin conductance in responses to silences and music pieces and B) shows difference between heart rate in responses to silences and music pieces. and the silent periods recorded right before the four pieces. While heart rate did not change strongly during Into the Mist and 9 Crimes, it increased during listening to Piece by Piece and Happy, which were also the two pieces that induced the strongest positive emotions of happiness and peacefulness. Therefore, as in previous studies, the heart rate measurements seem to correspond to the experienced positivity of the emotions experienced. Conclusion Measurements in this study show that the different pieces that were selected to express different emotional characteristics also induced corresponding emotional effects in our listeners. Musical characteristics such as tempo, articulation, loudness, and timbre are typically associated with expressing specific emotions (see Table 1) and were able to induce, therefore, emotional responses that mirrored those expectations. This might be based on a mechanism that is similar to emotional

7 contagion or empathy, where we feel the emotions that someone displays in front of us. Accordingly, our participants experienced feelings of peacefulness, happiness, fear, or sadness. Even though the latter two feelings would normally be associated with unpleasant experiences, when experienced in the context of music, there were reported to be rather pleasant. This was supported by the physiological measurements in this study: the music had a calming and relaxing physiological effect on everyone, and the peaceful and happy songs increased the heart rate of the participant. We can therefore conclude that listening to music like the selected songs might be understood as a means of experiencing contrasting positive and negative emotions in a safe environment. Experiencing negative emotion in music does not have any direct consequences for our everyday lives, however we might be able use it in order to learn something about how to it feels like to be in a negative emotional state. In this study, listeners enjoyed this form of stimulation and it helped them to relax and calm down. We conclude that taking time off to listen to music has an important function in managing to cope with emotional challenges we experience. References Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (00). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 19(5), 770 814. Giordano, B. L., Egermann, H., & Bresin, R. (014). The Production and Perception of Emotionally Expressive Walking Sounds: Similarities between Musical Performance and Everyday Motor Activity. PloS One, 9(1), e115587. http://doi.org/10.171/journal.pone.0115587 Zentner,M., & Eerola, T. (010). Self-report measures and models of musical emotions. In P. N. Juslin & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications (pp. 185 ). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.