Radiating beauty" in Japan also?

Similar documents
Running head: FACIAL SYMMETRY AND PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS 1

The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and Attitude According to Congruity-Incongruity

PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS. Elaine Hatfield and Richard L. Rapson. University of Hawai i

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

Age differences in women s tendency to gossip are mediated by their mate value

Affective response to a set of new musical stimuli W. Trey Hill & Jack A. Palmer Psychological Reports, 106,

The relationship between shape symmetry and perceived skin condition in male facial attractiveness

Brief Report. Development of a Measure of Humour Appreciation. Maria P. Y. Chik 1 Department of Education Studies Hong Kong Baptist University

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

The Effects of Web Site Aesthetics and Shopping Task on Consumer Online Purchasing Behavior

INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC

The Roles of Politeness and Humor in the Asymmetry of Affect in Verbal Irony

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Despite the widespread adoption of stalking legislation, there is no definitive antistalking

Inducing change in user s perspective with the arrangement of body orientation of embodied agents

Psychological wellbeing in professional orchestral musicians in Australia

THE SOUND OF SADNESS: THE EFFECT OF PERFORMERS EMOTIONS ON AUDIENCE RATINGS

Sample APA Paper for Students Interested in Learning APA Style 6 th Edition. Jeffrey H. Kahn. Illinois State University

REVIEW ARTICLE IDEAL EMBODIMENT: KANT S THEORY OF SENSIBILITY

The Impact of Humor in North American versus Middle East Cultures

Cognitive modeling of musician s perception in concert halls

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

van Schaik, P. (Paul); Ling, J. (Jonathan)

hprints , version 1-1 Oct 2008

Peak experience in music: A case study between listeners and performers

Klee or Kid? The subjective experience of drawings from children and Paul Klee Pronk, T.

WHY DO PEOPLE CARE ABOUT REPUTATION?

The interaction of cartoonist s gender and formal features of cartoons*

Claim: refers to an arguable proposition or a conclusion whose merit must be established.

Singing in the rain : The effect of perspective taking on music preferences as mood. management strategies. A Senior Honors Thesis

Texas Music Education Research

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

THE ROLE OF SIMILAR HUMOR STYLES IN INITIAL ROMANTIC ATTRACTION. Justin Harris Moss

Effect of Compact Disc Materials on Listeners Song Liking

Effect of coloration of touch panel interface on wider generation operators

ScienceDirect. Humor styles, self-efficacy and prosocial tendencies in middle adolescents

Natural Scenes Are Indeed Preferred, but Image Quality Might Have the Last Word

Can parents influence children s music preferences and positively shape their development? Dr Hauke Egermann

Modeling perceived relationships between melody, harmony, and key

LAUGHTER IN SOCIAL ROBOTICS WITH HUMANOIDS AND ANDROIDS

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN DICHOTOMOUS THINKING AND MUSIC PREFERENCES AMONG JAPANESE UNDERGRADUATES

Emotional Intelligence

23/01/51. Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the. VEP waveform. How are ERP related to gender? Event-Related Potential (ERP)

I like those glasses on you, but not in the mirror: Fluency, preference, and virtual mirrors

A perceptual study on face design for Moe characters in Cool Japan contents

Chapter Two: Long-Term Memory for Timbre

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

University of Stirling, Stirling, FK9 4LA, Scotland, UK

Disputing about taste: Practices and perceptions of cultural hierarchy in the Netherlands van den Haak, M.A.

Preference Tendencies for Musical Instrument Sounds

Crowding, Contagion, and Laughter

Effect of sense of Humour on Positive Capacities: An Empirical Inquiry into Psychological Aspects

The Confusion of Predictability A Reader-Response Approach of A Respectable Woman

Dissociating Averageness and Attractiveness: Attractive Faces Are Not Always Average

Subjective Emotional Responses to Musical Structure, Expression and Timbre Features: A Synthetic Approach

PROVERBS AS RECOMMENDATIONS REGARDING RISK TAKING

Example of an APA-style manuscript for Research Methods in Psychology. William Revelle. Department of Psychology. Northwestern University

Construction of a harmonic phrase

Moralistic Criticism. Post Modern Moral Criticism asks how the work in question affects the reader.

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

Understanding the Mediating Role of Responsibility in Perceptions of Stalking. Adrian J. Scott, Emma Sleath, Lorraine Sheridan & Simon C.

MUSICAL MOODS: A MASS PARTICIPATION EXPERIMENT FOR AFFECTIVE CLASSIFICATION OF MUSIC

Running head: THE EFFECT OF MUSIC ON READING COMPREHENSION. The Effect of Music on Reading Comprehension

Improving music composition through peer feedback: experiment and preliminary results

Set-Top-Box Pilot and Market Assessment

2. Measurements of the sound levels of CMs as well as those of the programs

When Do Vehicles of Similes Become Figurative? Gaze Patterns Show that Similes and Metaphors are Initially Processed Differently

The effect of male timbre vocal modeling in falsetto and non-falsetto on the singing and pitch accuracy of second grade students

Effect of room acoustic conditions on masking efficiency

Research & Development. White Paper WHP 228. Musical Moods: A Mass Participation Experiment for the Affective Classification of Music

The Epistolary Genre from the Renaissance Until Today. even though it is less popular than some other mainstream genres such as satire or saga, for

Subjective evaluation of common singing skills using the rank ordering method

How about laughter? Perceived naturalness of two laughing humanoid robots

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Faculty of Environmental Engineering, The University of Kitakyushu,Hibikino, Wakamatsu, Kitakyushu , Japan

MANUSCRIPT FORMAT FOR JOURNAL ARTICLES SUBMITTED TO AMMONS SCIENTIFIC, LTD. FOR POSSIBLE PUBLICATION IN PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR

Sex differences in preferences for humor produced by men or women: Is humor in the sex of the perceiver? [word count = <2500]

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

Personality Types and Sense of Humor and their Association with Teachers Performance Improvement

Navigating on Handheld Displays: Dynamic versus Static Peephole Navigation

An Evolutionary Perspective on Humor: Sexual Selection or Interest Indication?

BBC Trust Review of the BBC s Speech Radio Services

Electronic Musicological Review

The Concepts and Acoustical Characteristics of Groove in. Japan

Introductory Comments: Special Issue of EJOP (August 2010) on Humor Research in Personality and Social Psychology

Running Head: IT S JUST A JOKE 1

MANOVA COM 631/731 Spring 2017 M. DANIELS. From Jeffres & Neuendorf (2015) Film and TV Usage National Survey

Correlation between Groovy Singing and Words in Popular Music

Individual differences in prediction: An investigation of the N400 in word-pair semantic priming

II. Overview of Movie Theaters

Quantify. The Subjective. PQM: A New Quantitative Tool for Evaluating Display Design Options

SIGNS, SYMBOLS, AND MEANING DANIEL K. STEWMT*

Comparing Books Held by Japanese Public Libraries: Outsourcing versus Local Government Management

Deconstruction is a way of understanding how something was created and breaking something down into smaller parts.

Largeness and shape of sound images captured by sketch-drawing experiments: Effects of bandwidth and center frequency of broadband noise

Sexual Selection I. A broad overview

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

An Analytical Approach to The Challenges of Cultural Relativism. The world is a conglomeration of people with many different cultures, each with

Master thesis. The effects of L2, L1 dubbing and L1 subtitling on the effectiveness of persuasive fictional narratives.

Transcription:

Jupdnese Psychological Reseurch 1990, Vol.32, No.3, 148-153 Short Report Physical attractiveness and its halo effects on a partner: Radiating beauty" in Japan also? TAKANTOSHI ONODERA Psychology Course, Division of Primary Education, Tokai Women's Junior College, Kagamihara, Gifu 504 MASAKI MIURA Department of Educational Psychology, Faculty of Education, Kyoto University, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606 We studied physical attractiveness effects and examined whether radiating beauty effects (Sigall & Landy, 1973) would be induced in Japan. In the two experiments, 201 female and 128 male subjects rated pictures of a man and a woman. The description found in the questionnaire informed the subjects that two stimulus people were associated, or unassociated. In addition, attractiveness of female stimulus was manipulated by the two kinds of pictures, either attractive or unattractive. The results of the two experiments demonstrated the consistent pattern of the stereotype "Beauty is good, but not masculine". However, we could not confirm the generalized halo effects called "radiating beauty effects". That is, the male associated with an attractive female was not perceived favorably and the male with an unattractive female was not perceived unfavorably. Unexpected effects of the association and the subjects sex were revealed. Female subjects place high value on association with the opposite-sex. Implications of these results are discussed with attention to Japanese arranged marriage. Key words: physical attractiveness, facial appearance, interpersonal attraction, halo effects, beauty stereotype, sex stereotype, implicit personality, interpersonal perception. Numerous studies focusing on physical attractiveness have been conducted (see Adams & Crossman, 1978; Bull & Rumsey, 1988; Dion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972; Patzer, 1985). Most studies revealed that physical attractiveness is a very important factor in judging others. Although some people display psychological resistance, failing thus to realize that a book is often judged by its cover, the results of a preceding study conducted in Japan indicated also that there is a beauty stereotype phenomenon (Onodera, 1989). This stereotype phenomenon consists mainly in believing that "what is would like to thank Mrs. Kimiko Ikegami (Kyoto University) for her valuable assistance on these experiments. We also wish to thank Dr. Hafsi Mohamed (Nara University), Mr, Joseph Stavoy (Tokai Women's College), Mr. John Williams (Tokai Women's College) and Mr. Simon Scanes (Phillips University-Japan) for their reading of the manuscript and/or helpful comments on English expression. beautiful is good". Thus, a beautiful woman is perceived as having socially desirable traits. Besides the advantage for the beautiful woman herself, Sigall and Landy (1973) reported that the man associated with the beautiful woman also benefits from her. They explain that the effects of the woman's attractiveness had radiating effects on her companion. Moreover, Meiner and Sheposh (1977) showed that intelligence as well as physical attractiveness confer a benefit upon not only an endowed woman but also her associated man. The purpose of the present study was to confirm the physical attractiveness effects and to examine the radiating effects of beauty in Japan. Experiment 1 Method Subjects were 149 female college students. The experimental design was

Physical attractiveness and its halo effect 149 a 2 ~2 factorial design-attractiveness of the female (attractive condition vs. unattractive condition) and the malefemale association (associated condition vs. unassociated condition). The two levels of physical attractiveness were manipulated by black-and-white pictures of a face, and the association by a questionnaire based description. Two pictures of a male and a female were pasted to the questionnaire. While the picture of the male was the same through all the conditions, the pictures of the female were of two kinds, a picture of a physically attractive person and one of an unattractive person. Those pictures were selected based on the results of a pretest. In the pretest, three males and three females evaluated 80 female pictures taken from a year-book and ranked them with respect to physical attractiveness. We selected the pictures which were evaluated as the most attractive and the most unattractive. Another factor, "association", was manipulated by a questionnaire based description. In the association condition, the female stimulus person and the male stimulus person in the pictures were described as a couple and who had been associated with each other for five years. On the other hand, in the unassociation condition, there was no description suggesting the existence of a relationship between the two stimulus persons. Procedure Subjects were told that the purpose of the present experiment was to study how people can accurately judge others on the basis of very few cues. Each subject was then, randomly given one of four questionnaires corresponding to the four conditions. Subjects were instructed to judge the target person's personality traits from the pictures. On the questionnaire, a 7- point scale was presented and eleven trait related items were printed for rating on the basis of the scale. Dependent. Measures Subjects evaluated both the female target and male target with regard to the eleven items. The female target was evaluated by the following traits: (1)- beauty (2)-intelligence (3)-kindness (4)- self confidence (5)-positiveness (6)-cheerfulness (7)-sex appeal (8)-strong will (9)- femininity (10)-tenderness (11)-friendliness. For the male target, "attractiveness" and "masculinity" were used instead of "beauty" and "femininity". Other items were the same in rating both stimulus. Results A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed on the eleven dependent measures for each target stimulus. The evaluation of the female target showed two significant overall main effects: effect of association, F(11, 134)= 2.08, p<.025 and effect of attractiveness, F(11, 134)=38.24, p<.001. The overall interaction of association and attractiveness was not significant but marginal, F(11, 134)=1.70, p<.079. Then, univariate analyses of variance were performed. Table 1 and Table 2 show the mean scores of the significant items for female target evaluation. Concerning the interaction, there is a tendency that the associated-attractive female is considered more beautiful and the associated-unattractive female as the least beautiful. Although univariate F-tests of other items did not indicate a significance, the associated-attractive female was perceived as high in "intelligence", "kindness", "sex appeal", "femininity", tenderness" and "friendliness". Except for "kindness", the unassociatedunattractive female was rated as the lowest among these items. On the other hand, the unassociated-unattractive female was rated high in "self confidence", positiveness" and "strong will". For the evaluation of the male target,

150 T. Onodera and M. Miura Table 1 Mean scores of significant items for female target (Experiment 1) Note. For missing values, one case was dropped from this analysis. Figures in parentheses indicate SDs. Table 2 Mean scores of significant items for female target (Experiment 1) Note. Figures in parentheses indicate SDs, there were not any overall significant effects or tendencies (main effect of attractiveness: F(11, 135)=1.40, p<.179; interaction of association and attractiveness: F(11, 135)=1.44, p<.160). Discussion For the female target evaluation, the results of the previous study (Onodera. 1989) were supported. While the attractive female was perceived to he high in the most socially desirable traits, she was perceived low in some stereotypically masculine traits ("self confidence", "positiveness" and "strong will"); whereas the unattractive female was perceived as high in all those traits. These results confirmed the existence of a strong stereotype of beauty in Japan also. However, other findings were unexpected. There was no significant effect for the evaluation of the male target person, but there was a strong main association effect for the evaluation of the female target person. Table 1 shows that subjects perceived the associated female high in "kindness", "femininity" and "tenderness", but low in positiveness". Furthermore, there is a tendency to perceive the associated female high in "cheerfulness", "friendliness", and low in "self confidence" (for all the items; p<.2). Thus, we may say that the associated female was stereotypically perceived as having feminine traits, but not

Physical attractiveness and its halo effect 151 Table 3 Mean scores of significant items (Experiment 2) Note. Figures in parentheses indicates SDs. masculine traits. This perception pattern is somewhat correspondent to the pattern of beauty effect. Moreover, all independent measures indicate significant simple main effects: sex of subjects (F(11, 162)=3.36, p<.001); association (F(11, 162)=2.22, p<.d16); attractiveness (F(ll, 162)=25.01, p<.001). Experiment 2 The purpose of Experiment 2 was to confirm the stability of the findings in Experiment 1 and explore how the sex of the subjects affects the way the stimuli are perceived. Method Fifty-two female subjects and 128 male subjects were recruited from an introductory psychology course in a university. The same stimuli and procedure as in Experiment 1 were used, as were dependent measures, and the sex of subjects factor was included in Experiment 2. Results Results of MANOVA on the eleven dependent measures of female target stimuli yield a significant three-way interaction, attractiveness ~ association x sex of subjects (F(11, 162)=2.52, p<.006). There was also a significant two-way interaction for attractiveness ~ association (F(l 1, 162)= 2.02, p<.030). In the three-way interaction of MA- NOVA, the following five items, \"intelligence", "kindness", "positiveness", strong ill" and "tenderness" were significant in ANOVAs ("positiveness" p<.01 ; others p<.05). Table 3 shows the means of each condition. The same pattern of effects was found among intelligence", "kindness" and "tenderness". And the same pattern was found between positiveness" and "strong will". In the former group of items, high ratings in the unattractive-associated-female subject condition were prominent. Furthermore, female subjects tended to highly evaluate the attractive-unassociated female target stimulus. In the latter group of items, high ratings in the unattractiveunassociated-female subject condition are common. Thus, high ratings in the unattractive-associated condition of the female subjects seems to be an important cause of the three-way interaction. This view was supported by separate ANOVAs performed with each factor. While all the items which showed significant three-way effects revealed significant at-

152 T. Onodera and M. Miura trac iveness ~ association effects in the female subject condition (" kindness ", p<.05: others, p<.02), there was no significant effect in the male subject condition (all: ns). We divided all eases into two groups according to their sex, and compared only the results of female subjects with those in Experiment I. MANOVA of female subjects revealed a significant two-way interaction (attractiveness ~ association: p<.019) and two significant main effects (association: p<.001; attractiveness: p<.001). These results arc consistent with those in Experiment 1 except that the nearly significant effect of the interaction of attractiveness ~ association effect (p<.079) is significant in Experiment 2 (p<.019). The female subjects perceived the unattractive female target as being intelligent, kind and tender when she had a boyfriend. On the other hand, the unattractive female who had a boyfriend was perceived to he weak-willed and passive. The greatest effect in both Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 was the simple attractiveness main effect. That is, while an attractive female was significantly perceived as high in " beauty ", " intelligence ", " sex appeal ", " femininity ", " tenderness " (all: p<.01), an unattractive female was perceived as high in " strong will " (p<.005). The association effect shows an associated female was perceived as high in " beauty", " cheerfulness", " sex appeal" and " friendliness " p<.05), whereas an unassociated female was perceived high in " self confidence " and " positiveness " (both: p<.05). These results confirm the stability of the attractiveness effect and association effect. In the items concerning " strong will ", " self confidence " and " positiveness ", an unassociated female or an unattractive female was evaluated as high, and in other items, an attractive or an associated female was evaluated as high. Another confirmation was obtained from the male target evaluation. All elfeets were insignificant for female subjects of both Experiment 2 and Experiment I. The results of MANOVA of the total cases for the male target evaluation showed that the association effect was significant (F(11, 162)=2.07, p<.025). The univariate analysis showed that " attractiveness ", " intelligence ", " sex appeal ", " masculinity " (all: p<.05) and " tenderness " (p<.001) have reliable effects. The male who was associated with a female got significantly higher ratings in those items. General Discussion The results of the two experiments consistently indicated that there arc no radiating effects as observed by Sigall and Landy (1973). Furthermore, the results of analysis in Experiment 2 indicated the significant effects of the subject's sex. These effect were not expected from either Sigall and Landy's experiment (1973) or Meiners and Sheposh's (1977). Japanese females seem to regard the association as valuable. Thus, an unattractive female can receive a good evaluation from the same sex if she has a boyfriend. However, a Japanese male is not affected by the association factor, and the unattractive female is always perceived as lower by the Japanese male. This asymmetry between male and female can be explained in terms of sex difference, concerning orientation of romantic relationships. For example, Patzer (1985) quoting from Lehner writes that in Japanese marriages " the men are all looking for good-looking women and the women are all looking for men who can support them well ". Thus, Japanese female subjects could perceive that the associated female must have traits to achieve success regardless of her physical attractiveness. In contrast, Japanese male subjects might lay emphasis on attractiveness rather than association. Moreover, the associated male was perceived to possess desirable traits in both sexes. These findings suggest that the association generally has a greater value in Japan

Physical attractiveness and its halo offect 153 than in Western countries. Marriages in Japan are often arranged by other people. This system called " OMIAI " is arranged as a formal blind date for marriageable persons. This system helps the persons who have difficulties finding a partner to marry. However, some young people put a high value on a love-marriage rather than on an arranged marriage. In a sense, the person who can find a partner by him/her self may be regarded as having desirable attributes. Thus, the emphasis of association could be explained by the fact that finding a partner by oneself is not necessarily a matter of course in Japan. Although higher interactions were found in the female target evaluation, the main effects of attractiveness were consistent and far greater than the interactions. The attractive female was believed to be more beautiful, intelligent, kind, cheerful, sexy, feminine, tender and friendly, but less self-confident, weak-willed and passive. That the attractive female was perceived low in some attributes corresponds to the reports by Cash and Janda (1984). Cash and Janda note that " If a woman wants to succeed in a nian's world, she had better not look too feminine " (p. 48). It is possible that the attractive female is placed at a disadvantage in Japan when the situation demands strong-will, positiveness, or self-confidence, or other stereotypically masculine attributes. References Adams, G. R., & Grossman, S. M. 1978 Physical attractiveness: A Cultural imperative. New York: Libra Publishers, Inc. Bull, R., & Rumsey, N. 1988 The social psychology of facial appearance. New York: Springer-Verlag Inc. Cash, T. F., & Janda, L. H. 1984 The eye of the beholder. Psychology Today. Vol. 18, No. 2, December. Pp. 46-52. Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. 1972 What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285-290. hiciners, M. L., & Sheposh, J. P. 1977 Beauty or brains: Which image for your rnate? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 262-265. Onodera, T. 1989 A study about types of beauty and beauty stereotypes. Memoirs of Staff Members of Tokai Women's Junior College, 15, 113-122. (In Japanese) Patzer, G. L. 1985 The physical attractiveness phenomena. In E. Aronson. (Ed.), Perspectives in socialprychology. New York: Plenum Press. Sigall, H., & Landy, D. 1973 Radiating beauty: Effects of having a physically attractive partner on person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 218-224. (Received June 28, 1989; accepted Jan. 20, 1990)