Screech, Hoot, and Chirp: Natural Soundscapes and Human Musicality

Similar documents
Review of Bug Music: How Insects Gave Us Rhythm and Noise. David Rothenberg Picador pp., Paperback

Music without Borders When birds trill and whales woo-oo, we call it singing. Are we serious? By SUSAN MILIUS

Primates have been laughing for 10m years

SYMPHONY OF THE RAINFOREST Part 2: Soundscape Saturation

Voices From the Deep. Description. Objectives. Essential Questions. Background Information

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

Toward a New Comparative Musicology. Steven Brown, McMaster University

Existing Music Science

Supplemento n. 6 a «Illuminazioni» n. 18 (ottobre-dicembre 2011) Alessandra Anastasi THE SINGING OF PRIMATES

3. His hardworking mother was a housekeeper for a rich family in Juarez. 9. Gabriel s family bought a mansion in Juarez a few years ago.

Aural Architecture: The Missing Link

Chapter 10 - Non-verbal Information and Artistic Expression in the Symbolosphere and Its Emergence through Secondary Perception

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

What are the true functions of creation stories (myths)? How should they be viewed today?

Björn Merker: neuroscientist/zoömusicologist

Palace Fields Primary School Whole School Music Curriculum Overview Academic Year Autumn 1 Autumn 2 Spring 1 Spring 2 Summer 1 Summer 2

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

A Confusion of the term Subjectivity in the philosophy of Mind *

Native Americans have long been aware

Rhythm and Melody Aspects of Language and Music

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

This excerpt from. The Origins of Music. Nils L. Wallin, Björn Merker and Steven Brown, editors The MIT Press.

WHY DO VEERIES (CATHARUS FUSCESCENS) SING AT DUSK? COMPARING ACOUSTIC COMPETITION DURING TWO PEAKS IN VOCAL ACTIVITY

Music, Art, and Biology: Critical Analysis and Creative Response Courtney Brown. Class Description

Endless Forms. Citation. As Published Publisher. Version

Sexual Selection I. A broad overview

Fisk Street Primary School Curriculum. The Arts. Music

Jeff Larson and Paul J. Larson

OVER THE YEARS, PARTICULARLY IN THE PAST

eds. Nils L. Wallin, Bjorn Merker, and Steven Brown Cambridge, Massachusetts: A Bradford Book, The MIT Press, 2000, 498pp, ISBN

The Rime of the Ancient Mariner. Study Guide Notes

Enigma Variations Theme (Enigma), Variations 11 (G. R. S.), 6 (Ysobel) & 7 (Troyte)

A Musical Species. By Caroline Atkinson

English Language Arts Test Listening Selection

Aposematic Model vs. Sexual Selection Model of Human Evolution

Motivation: BCI for Creativity and enhanced Inclusion. Paul McCullagh University of Ulster

Grade Level Expectations for the Sunshine State Standards

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

Patel, Iversen, Bregman & Schulz 1. Studying synchronization to a musical beat in nonhuman animals

World Music. Music of Africa: choral and popular music

Abstract. Warning Colors by Robert McClure. flute in G and third flute doubling piccolo), two oboes, one english horn in F, two clarinets in Bb,

Trevor de Clercq. Music Informatics Interest Group Meeting Society for Music Theory November 3, 2018 San Antonio, TX

Essay on evolution of man as a tool making animal

A Planned Course Statement for. Music Theory, AP. Course # 760 Grade(s) 11, 12. Length of Period (mins.) 40 Total Clock Hours: 120

Chapter 6: Ways of knowing Emotion (p. 145)

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

A Hybrid Model of Painting: Pictorial Representation of Visuospatial Attention through an Eye Tracking Research

hit), and assume that longer incidental sounds (forest noise, water, wind noise) resemble a Gaussian noise distribution.

What Can Experiments Reveal About the Origins of Music? Josh H. McDermott

Students will know Vocabulary: steady beat melody rhythm patterns loud/soft fast/slow improvise

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

DIATHEMATIKON PROGRAMMA CROSS-THEMATIC CURRICULUM FRAMEWORK

Primary Music Objectives (Prepared by Sheila Linville and Julie Troum)

Advanced Placement Music Theory Course Syllabus Greenville Fine Arts Center


Ron Nagorcka: musician/composer/zoömusicologist

Music Progression Map

An Introduction to Evolutionary Musicology

Soundscape and Listening

The Origins of Future Consciousness

Celine Granjou The Friends of My Friends

K-12 Performing Arts - Music Standards Lincoln Community School Sources: ArtsEdge - National Standards for Arts Education

Chapter 7 -- Secular Medieval Music

Years 3 and 4 standard elaborations Australian Curriculum: Music

Talia Elbaz, Claudia Comte s Forest of Carved Reliquaries, Whitewall, July 23, 2018

Inuit Lesson 1 Introduction to Inuit Culture and Drum Dancing By Alexander Duff 7 th, 8 th Grade

The evolution of the music faculty: a comparative perspective

The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective

Enigma Variations Theme (Enigma), Variations 11 (G. R. S.), 6 (Ysobel) & 7 (Troyte)

Changes in fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus) song over a forty-four year period in New England waters

Subjective evaluation of common singing skills using the rank ordering method

A FUNCTIONAL CLASSIFICATION OF ONE INSTRUMENT S TIMBRES

MUSIC (MU) Music (MU) 1

44. Jerry Goldsmith Planet of the Apes: The Hunt (opening) (for Unit 6: Further Musical Understanding)

PUBLIC SCHOOLS OF EDISON TOWNSHIP DIVISION OF CURRICULUM AND INSTRUCTION. Chamber Choir/A Cappella Choir/Concert Choir

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A.

Music (MUSIC) Iowa State University

Coastal Carolina University Faculty Senate Consent Agenda March 4, 2015 COLLEGE OF HUMANITIES AND FINE ARTS

1 Introduction to evolutionary psychology

Music Department. Handbook

Key Skills to be covered: Year 5 and 6 Skills

15th International Conference on New Interfaces for Musical Expression (NIME)

What is music as a cognitive ability?

Chapter 2 Musical and Historical Encounter: Inuit Communities Text: Beverly Diamond Online Instructor s Manual: J. Bryan Burton

Chapter 117. Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills for Fine Arts Subchapter A. Elementary, Adopted 2013

Media Contacts PI. Delia Nicholls +61 (0) Rebecca Fitzgibbon +61 (0)

Music Technology Advanced Unit 3: Music Technology Portfolio 2

Effectively Managing Sound in Museum Exhibits. by Steve Haas

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

A Plea for Human Nature

Improvisation In the Elementary General Music Classroom

Design considerations for technology to support music improvisation

Music. Last Updated: May 28, 2015, 11:49 am NORTH CAROLINA ESSENTIAL STANDARDS

The Evolution of Music in Comparative Perspective

EMPLOYMENT SERVICE. Professional Service Editorial Board Journal of Audiology & Otology. Journal of Music and Human Behavior

Music Performance Panel: NICI / MMM Position Statement

THEORY AND DECISION LIBRARY

Harlan County Schools Curriculum Guide Arts and Humanities Grade 4

BIBB 060: Music and the Brain Tuesday, 1:30-4:30 Room 117 Lynch Lead vocals: Mike Kaplan

Transcription:

Screech, Hoot, and Chirp: Natural Soundscapes and Human Musicality By: Donald A. Hodges Hodges, D. (2004). Screech, hoot, and chirp: Natural soundscapes and human musicality. Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition. Evanston, IL, 636-638. ***Note: This version of the document is not the copy of record. Reprinted with express permission of the author. Link to Full Text: http://www.icmpc8.umn.edu/proceedings/icmpc8/pdf/author/mp040181.pdf Abstract: The earth is not a silent planet. It is filled with an immense variety of geophanies (the sounds of inanimate nature such as waterfalls, thunderstorms, and wind) and biophanies (the sounds of animals). These sounds of nature are at once both familiar and mysterious. A group of scientists and musicians engaged in the BioMusic Project has gathered to consider the music of nature and the nature of music. The BioMusic mission statement is To research musical sounds as a basic ingredient in the intuitive, nonverbal processes of communication, to expand our definition of music, to enlarge our view of its role in biodiversity and human development, and to create appropriate attitudinal changes (http://biomusic.org). The BioMusic Project has a number of implications for music psychology as a number of questions are being considered: What is music? Do only humans have music or do some animals have music, too? Why are we (humans) musical? What are the similarities and differences between human music and animal soundmaking? What are the influences of biophonies and geophonies on human music? Conversely, what is the impact of anthrophony (human sounds, including music) on biophony? How do human sounds impact natural soundscapes? Answers to all these questions and many others have profound implications for music psychologists who are trying to explicate the phenomenon of the human musical experience. Article: BACKGROUND The earth is not a silent planet. It is filled with an immense variety of geophanies (the sounds of inanimate nature such as waterfalls, thunderstorms, and wind) and biophanies (the sounds of animals). These sounds of nature are at once both familiar and mysterious. We have all heard many animal and nature sounds either in the wilds or at least on television. But perhaps many of us have not stopped to consider how these natural soundscapes and human musicality intersect. A group of scientists and musicians engaged in the BioMusic Project has gathered to do exactly that: to consider the music of nature and the nature of music. The BioMusic mission statement is To research musical sounds as a basic ingredient in the intuitive, nonverbal processes of communication, to expand our definition of music, to enlarge our view of its role in biodiversity and human development, and to create appropriate attitudinal changes (http://biomusic.org). Some of the objectives of BioMusic are: To research and reveal the linkages between musical sounds in all species.

To explore, exploit, and expand the vast and varied aural interfaces and interactions within and between species (i.e., the music of nature). To explore the musical sounds produced by humans and other species considered in this context (i.e., the nature of music). AIMS The purpose of this presentation is to present information regarding BioMusic and to raise issues that have implications for music perception and cognition. MAIN CONTRBUTION Animal Soundmaking Before examining some of the larger issues in connection with music psychology, a few, brief examples of non-human vocalizations are presented to help set the stage. Male tree-hole frogs, living in the rainforest of Borneo, actively adjust the frequency of their calls over a wide frequency range to match varying resonating characteristics of water-filled logs (Lardner & Lakim, 2002). Further research has shown that females routinely select males that do the best job of emitting a resonant sound. Male humpback whales create extended vocalizations that are common to a pod (Gray, Krause, Atema, Payne, Krumhansl, & Baptista, 2001). Over a breeding season this song is varied so that by the next season it is completely changed (Payne, 2000). Whale vocalizations utilize many features that bear similarities to human music, such as improvisation, imitation, rhythm patterns, phrases, pitch intervals, formal structures, and even rhyming schemes. As with whales, songbirds, nearly half of the 9,000 species of birds, invest their songs with many of the same characteristics as human music (Gray et al., 2001; Whaling, 2000). Although males are the primary singers, a practice of antiphonal singing, called duetting, occurs between males and females (Slater, 2000). In duetting, a male and female bird alternate phrases in an exchange so tightly interwoven it can sound as if only one bird is singing. Ape vocalizations range from high-pitched squeals to pant-hoots to duetting, although singing, in general, is practiced perhaps by as little as 11% of primate species (Geissmann, 2000). Bonobos are subjects of intensive cognitive science research and successfully communicate with humans through sign language and lexigrams (http://www.gsu.edu/webprj01/cas/wwwjjp/public_html/depart/faculty/srumbaugh.htm). Recently a group of Bonobos began interacting with humans in musical ways, using instruments and exhibiting other abilities that may be considered musical (P. Gray, personal communication). BIOPHONY AND THE NICHE HYPOTHESIS As these few examples illustrate, the sonic world in which humankind evolved is filled with an incredible array of detectable patterns. Modern living has detached us from natural soundscapes, but for our ancient ancestors, their very survival depended upon the ability to detect patterns in these sounds, derive meaning from them, and adjust their behavior accordingly. Wind and water noises, bird calls, monkey screeches, and tiger growls all had meaning. Beyond this, many (if not all) animal sounds were suffused with an emotional content (Hauser, 2000). They screamed in

pain, roared a challenge, or offered enticements for mating. Darwin contended that human musicality arose out of the emotional content of animal sound-making when he said that musical tones and rhythm were used by our half-human ancestors, during the season of courtship, when animals of all kinds are excited not only by love, but by the strong passions of jealousy, rivalry, and triumph (1897/nd, 880). Early humans would have heard these sounds not in isolation but holistically as a sound tapestry. Krause s (2002) niche hypothesis likens biophonies to a symphonic score. A spectrogram of the sounds of the forest or around a pond shows that each species produces sounds that occupy particular frequency and temporal niches. These sounds are important mating calls, for example and they wouldn t be very effective if they were to become lost among all the other sounds. Thus, each animal has learned over the millennia to create sounds that occupy a very particular niche in the overall biophony, insuring that those for whom the sound is intended can identify and respond to it. Growing up in a particular sonic environment growing up both in the sense of the individual and of the generations over thousands of years it is quite natural that humans would make attempts to mimic the sounds of nature. With our great brains we moved easily from mimicry to elaboration, extension, synthesis, and eventually the creation of novel sounds. Thus, we occupy our own niche in the natural order of sounds, but we are not content to remain in that niche. As a dramatic example, Krause (2002) finds that it now takes 2,000 hours of field recording to acquire one hour of usable material; the reason for this is that it is nearly impossible to find natural habitats that are not invaded by human sounds (what Krause calls anthrophony). Much of the earliest music would have been vocal (and other bodily sounds), and many of the earliest instruments would have been biodegradable, having been made of reeds, wood, or skins, and thus lost in the mists of time. Nevertheless, there are evidences of early music. Scientific evidence has documented the use of flint blades for musical purposes perhaps 40,000 years ago (Cross, Zubrow, & Cowan, 2002). Acoustical analyses of caves show that those places where the acoustics are best are accompanied by many paintings; those places where the acoustics are poor have few or no cave paintings. Thus, the best places to view the artwork of the cave appear to have been the best places to hear music or chants (Allman, 1994, 216). Also found in the caves and elsewhere are whistles, flutes, and mammoth bones that may have been used as drums or as Ice Age xylophones (Hodges & Haack, 1996; Kunej & Turk, 2000). IMPLICATIONS The BioMusic Project has a number of implications for music psychology. Merely having scientists (a whale specialist, a bioacoustician, an ornithologist, a Paleolithic flute specialist, a larynx/syrinx specialist, and a neurologist) and musicians talking and working together is important. The BioMusic Team is working with the Science Museum of Minnesota, the Association of Science and Technology Centers, and the Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology to develop and mount a traveling museum exhibit on biomusic. In planning the exhibit, issues such as the following have arisen: What is music? Do only humans have music or do some animals have music, too? Why are we (humans) musical? What are the similarities and differences between human music and animal soundmaking? What are the influences of biophonies and

geophonies on human music? In some cultures, it is astounding how closely their music reflects the complex rhythms, polyphonies, and sonic textures of the habitats where they live and hunt (Krause, 1998, p. 76). Conversely, one might ask about the impact of anthrophony on biophony. How do human sounds impact natural soundscapes? Answers to all these questions and many others have profound implications for music psychologists who are trying to explicate the phenomenon of the human musical experience. REFERENCES Allman, W. 1994. The stone age present. New York: Simon and Schuster. Cross, I., Zubrow, E., & Cowan, F. 2002. Musical behaviours and the archaeological record: A preliminary study. In J. Mathieu (ed.) Experimental Archaeology. British Archaeological Reports International Series. 1035, 25-34. Darwin, C. 1897/nd. The descent of man. New York: Modern Edition. Geissmann, T. 2000. Gibbon songs and human music from an evolutionary perspective. In The origins of music, ed. N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown., 103-123. Cambridge: The MIT Press. Gray, P., Krause, B., Atema, J., Payne, R., Krumhansl, C., and Baptista, L. 2001. The music of nature and the nature of music. Science, 291(January 5), 52-54. Hauser, M. 2000. The sound and fury: Primate vocalizations as reflections of emotion and thought. In The origins of music, ed. N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown., 77-102. Cambridge: The MIT Press. Hodges, D. and P. Haack. 1996. The influence of music on human behavior. In Handbook of music psychology, ed. D. Hodges, 469-555. San Antonio, TX: IMR Press. Krause, B. 1998. Into a wild sanctuary. Berkeley, CA: HeydayBooks. Krause, B. 2002. Wild soundscapes: Discovering the voice of the natural world. Berkeley, CA: Wilderness Press. Kunej, D. & Turk, I. 2000. New perspectives on the beginnings of music: Archeological and musicological analysis of a middle Paleolithic bone flute. In The origins of music, ed. N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown., 235-268. Cambridge: The MIT Press. Lardner, B., & Lakim, M. (December 5, 2002). Tree-hole frogs exploit resonance effects. Nature, 420, p. 475. Payne, K. 2000. The progressively changing songs of humpback whales: A window on the creative process in a wild animal. In The origins of music, ed. N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown., 135-150. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

Slater, P. 2000. Birdsong repertoires: Their origins and use. In The origins of music, ed. N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown., 49-63. Cambridge: The MIT Press. Whaling, C. 2000. What s behind a song? The neural basis of song learning in birds. In The origins of music, ed. N. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown., 65-76. Cambridge: The MIT Press.