Visual Ecology Defined

Similar documents
Neural Hardware for Vision

Anthro 1401, University of Utah Evolution of Human Nature Study Guide. Alan Rogers

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

Colour and Vision: Through the Eyes of Nature, Natural History Museum, London, 15

Chapter 12: Introduction to Module 2 Evolution

Toward a New Comparative Musicology. Steven Brown, McMaster University

Television History. Date / Place E. Nemer - 1


Part 1: A Summary of the Land Ethic

Magicicada, 2016: They re Back!

Introducing the Read-Aloud

Bio 259: Comparative Vertebrate Anatomy

Aposematic Model vs. Sexual Selection Model of Human Evolution

LIVING COLOR Written and illustrated by Steve Jenkins

What is the lowest contrast spatial frequency you can see? High. x x x x. Contrast Sensitivity. x x x. x x. Low. Spatial Frequency (c/deg)

Significant Differences An Interview with Elizabeth Grosz

Processing. Electrical Engineering, Department. IIT Kanpur. NPTEL Online - IIT Kanpur

Lesson Objectives. Core Content Objectives. Language Arts Objectives

The Philosophy of Human Evolution

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A.

Color in Information Visualization

The untimely birth of Children s books about evolution,

IMAGINATION AT THE SCHOOL OF SEASONS - FRYE S EDUCATED IMAGINATION AN OVERVIEW J.THULASI

Screech, Hoot, and Chirp: Natural Soundscapes and Human Musicality

Primates have been laughing for 10m years

Voices From the Deep. Description. Objectives. Essential Questions. Background Information

WHAT MIGHT THE WORLD BE LIKE IF IT WAS

Missouri Show-Me Standards Addressed: Knowledge SC 4 Performance 1.6, 3.5, 3.6, 4.1

Style, Tone, and Mood

DIBELS 8 th Edition Benchmark - Grade 8 Scoring Booklet

At the pre-concert forum on January 9th, we heard from Professors Michael

81 of 172 DOCUMENTS UNITED STATES PATENT AND TRADEMARK OFFICE PRE-GRANT PUBLICATION (Note: This is a Patent Application only.

Sound visualization through a swarm of fireflies

Spatial cognition: Where are we? Cognitive mapping or associative learning?

Understanding Human Color Vision

8K Resolution: Making Hyperrealism a Reality

Aural Architecture: The Missing Link

mirasol Display Value Proposition White Paper

Communication Studies Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

nmbo CELEBRATING THE ARTS MARCH 2016

Deep Dive into Curved Displays

SALTY DOG Year 2

MICHAEL RICE ARCHITECT ALL RIGHTS RESERVED.

James Prosek: An Un-Natural History

SYMPHONY OF THE RAINFOREST Part 2: Soundscape Saturation

Synopsis This module introduces communication, outlines theoretical ideas and aspects of Visual Communication with selected examples.

Bismarck, North Dakota is known for several things. First of all, you probably already know that Bismarck is the state capitol. You might even know

A S AND C OUNTY A LMANAC

Using the Brain to Learn, Laugh, and Continuously Improve

HOW MUCH WILLIAMSBURG SOCCER FIELD CAPACITY WAS ADDED BY CONVERTING TO ARTIFICIAL TURF VS. HOW MUCH WOULD BE ADDED BY INSTALLING LIGHTS?

For Chief Marker's and Markers' Use Only Section A Section B Section C Section D Total I II I II I II I II Chief Marker s Signature

Natural Selection in the Expressional Principles in Darwin s Expression of Emotions (1872)

Ask-a-Biologist Transcript Vol 034 (Guest: Peter Vukusic)

A Musical Species. By Caroline Atkinson

Natural Kinds and Concepts: A Pragmatist and Methodologically Naturalistic Account

IN THE MOMENT: he Japanese poetry of Haiku is often introduced to young children as a means

Looking at Movies. From the text by Richard Barsam. In this presentation: Beginning to think about what Looking at Movies in a new way means.

Features Horizon Electronic Marquee s Provide Years of Performance. spectrumhorizon.com

4 - In this essay, the author observes that Hokusai s work presents is an interaction of humans with.

SONY OLED FEATURES OLED TV

Elements of a Television System

TOOLS FOR A BETTER VIEW

Published in: International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 29(2) (2015):

Keynote speech evolutionary biology Example of an existing collaboration and highlight of recent research results A Keynote

Domains of Inquiry (An Instrumental Model) and the Theory of Evolution. American Scientific Affiliation, 21 July, 2012

-ation. -ion. -sion. -ous. Austin s Amazing Bats. Spelling Words

Some comments on visual perception and the use of video playback in animal behavior studies

Intra-frame JPEG-2000 vs. Inter-frame Compression Comparison: The benefits and trade-offs for very high quality, high resolution sequences

Existential Cause & Individual Experience

Literature Guide for Natalia Romanova's Once There Was a Tree

Mind, Thinking and Creativity

BIOS 3010: Ecology, Dr Stephen Malcolm

Evolution essay titles. Evolution essay titles.zip

METADESIGN. Human beings versus machines, or machines as instruments of human designs? Humberto Maturana

Reality According to Language and Concepts Ben G. Yacobi *

Leicestershire Amphibian and Reptile Network

Kuro is the future of Pioneer flat-panel displays. It s a world without boundaries. A place where the senses evolve, colours are felt and sounds can

The Origin of Species The Making of a Theory

The Moral Animal. By Robert Wright. Vintage Books, Reviewed by Geoff Gilpin

Questions 1 30 Read the following passage carefully before you choose your answers.

But, if I understood well, Michael Ruse doesn t agree with you. Why?

Hi everyone. My name is and I ve come here today to talk to you about being an engineer. So what is an engineer?

2005 UltraVision CineForm Plasma and LCD HDTV

Sony XBR 4K Best Z Series Features

Beatty on Chance and Natural Selection

The Science of Storytelling

Sight and Sensibility: Evaluating Pictures Mind, Vol April 2008 Mind Association 2008

Review of Bug Music: How Insects Gave Us Rhythm and Noise. David Rothenberg Picador pp., Paperback

The. Sharp Sale! Japan s No.1 LCD TV* LCD Panels made in Japan. Promotion Period: 1 July - 31 July 2009 LED TV. 2.3cm

Creative writing. A form poem. A syllable poem. A haiku. Let s write poetry!

Misc Fiction Irony Point of view Plot time place social environment

at Kettle s Yard 29 September - 18 November 2007

Fry Instant Phrases. First 100 Words/Phrases

WATCHMASTER IP THERMAL SURVEILLANCE SYSTEMS

Getting Images of the World

IEEE Santa Clara ComSoc/CAS Weekend Workshop Event-based analog sensing

Year 3 and 4 Grammar: Fronted Adverbials Learning From Home Activity Booklet

Downloaded from

How To Make Your Carvings Come To Life

The theory of data visualisation

Transcription:

1 Introduction A tiny fruit fly, hardly bigger than a speck of dust, drifts along the edge of a pond in the morning sunlight, keeping a constant course and height through the unpredictable buffeting of the morning breeze. Despite its minuscule size, its silhouette against the empty sky has betrayed the fly s passage to an alert predator. A dragonfly perched on the pond side rises in a buzz of wings, aims to intersect the fruit fly s path, and plucks it from the air in a graceful upward swoop. Returning to its perch to enjoy its catch, the dragonfly is spotted by a hovering kestrel far above; with a blur of motion, the kestrel snatches up the dragonfly and carries it to her home not far away. As the mother kestrel settles onto the edge of her nest, two tiny babies greet her arrival by stretching their necks upward. Tempted by her nestlings colorful, bobbing mouths, she tears up their breakfast and shoves a piece into each one. Each of these creatures was guided by its eyes as it carried out its accustomed behaviors the fruit fly heading steadily toward its chosen destination; the dragonfly as it sighted the fly, computed its course, and snatched it from the sky; the kestrel as it picked out the dragonfly against the tangle of pondside vegetation and unerringly plucked it from its perch; the nestlings as they spotted their mother s familiar shape descending from the sky; and the kestrel again as it responded to their irresistible opened mouths. Each animal s eyes allowed it to execute the behavior necessary for its survival (although, admittedly, not all survived this quiet morning). How do these various visual systems function? How is it that the fruit fly was so beautifully adapted for visual control of flight in air that buffeted its minuscule body? What allowed the dragonfly to see such a tiny spot in the sky, to recognize it as nearby prey and not a bird passing far above, and to derive the geometry required to intercept it? How could the kestrel see a blue, toothpick- sized form against masses of green vegetation from so far above? Why were its babies mouths so attractive? For that matter, how was it that the fruit fly never spotted the approaching dragonfly, and what allowed the dragonfly to see the speck of a passing fly while missing the predatory stoop of the kestrel? These are the kinds of questions that are addressed in the field of visual ecology.

2 Chapter 1 Visual Ecology Defined Visual ecology can be broadly defined as the study of how visual systems function to meet the ecological needs of animals, how they have evolved for proper function, and how they are specialized for and involved in particular visual tasks. Researchers who work at various levels of inquiry, from genes to behavior, call themselves visual ecologists, but all are primarily concerned with how animals use vision for natural tasks and behaviors. Although the outcomes of visual ecological research may well have implications for health or may be applicable for use in engineering or technology, the research itself centers on the animal of interest and on how it employs its visual system to meet its own ecological needs. A researcher who studies the retinas of squirrel monkeys to learn more about human visual physiology is not a visual ecologist; one who does nearly identical research to learn how the monkeys discriminate ripe from unripe fruit is. Thus, the kinds of questions that spark visual ecological research are some of the oldest in biology, some predating even the earliest science: How can predators sight prey at times when humans are essentially blind? Does my dog (or horse, or cat) see color? Do seals see equally well both underwater and on the shore? And many others that readers have probably asked themselves about vision in other animals. A particularly beautiful (literally so) illustration of how an artist s sensitive eye registers principles of visual ecology is seen in the Japanese cloisonné vase in figure.. Figure 1.1 Cloisonné vase with sunfish, made using gold wire and enamels. The artist is Yoshitaro Hayakawa, Ando Cloisonne Company, Japan, 1910. (Stephen W. Fisher collection. Photographed by J. Dean, johndeanphoto.com, used with permission)

Introduction 3 The artist, Yoshitaro Hayakawa, has created a convincing underwater scene, with the viewer just a short distance from the sunfish that seems to float only millimeters beneath the vase s surface. The fish s colors, patterned no doubt for communication with its conspecifics, are brilliant; they stand out in contrast to the water itself. A second fish, only slightly further away, has lost some of its brilliancy and clarity but still is well defined and colorful. The third fish (at the right margin of the vase) is almost lost in the underwater haze, fading into the background. Only the red on the posterior edge of its dorsal fin remains to signal what kind of fish it might be. Here we see clearly how water absorbs the colors of objects viewed through it, simultaneously hiding them in a veil of light scattered into the viewing path. This is not all that has captured the artist s attention. The water near the surface is brightly lit with white light, but as the depth increases, the water s color changes continuously to a rich blue- green. The plants and rocks on the bottom are bluish or a muddy green, as the extremes of the spectrum have been filtered away by the colored waters inhabited by the fish. As with the distant fish, absorption and multiple- path scattering have obscured the colors and details of the background objects. Hayakawa was clearly a keen observer of nature; one suspects that he would have made an excellent visual ecologist. Many of the principles we will be visiting in this book are seen in this one lovely object: properties of light in natural environments, color vision in animals and its relationship to signaling, and the ability both to signal and to hide using the same color palette. To illustrate visual ecological thinking in a slightly more scientific framework, consider a biological question that concerns vision (see the illustration in figure 1.2). All songbirds have excellent color vision far better than our color sense based on a complicated set of photoreceptors coupled to oil- droplet filters. Surprisingly, despite their radically different appearances and plumage, all these birds have nearly identical color vision. A visual physiologist would be interested in the receptor properties, the wiring of the birds retinas, how the color information is processed, and other functional issues. At another extreme, a behavioral biologist might study what aspects of the color patterns or sexual displays of males make them desirable to females. Visual ecologists would certainly be interested in knowing these things as well, but their focus would be on how the visual systems of songbirds fit generally into the birds needs. Are photoreceptor sets, similar across species, adaptive for high- quality color vision in the habitats occupied by songbirds, or are they in some way limited by the plumage colors that songbirds have? In other words, is the color vision of songbirds an evolutionary product of their light environments, or has it evolved for the requirements of signaling? Do birds choose to display their plumage at particular times of day, or in particular locations, where lighting makes their color patterns most visible? Do male birds arrange their displays to be seen from a particular vantage point? Do they choose backgrounds that enhance these colors? To answer questions such as these, measurements are needed of the environmental light at times of interest or biological significance, the properties of the color receptors themselves, the ways in which the plumage, background, and possibly even entire environmental scenes reflect light. Depending on the scope of the research, the visual ecologist might also consider the optics of the birds eyes and their specializations to function at particular light levels (in the case of songbirds, perhaps in the open vs. under forest canopy), and possibly even to the optics of feather reflectivity and color production. Any single visual ecological study might not include all these questions, but these (and many others with regard to songbird vision) all lie within the purview of the field. The unifying principle is that all these

4 Chapter 1 Figure 1.2 Female cardinal evaluating a male in sunlight, against natural foliage. White dashed lines indicate the natural illumination, the red ones the light reflected from the cardinal, and the green ones the light from the foliage. The female must discriminate the male from the background and evaluate the color and quality of his plumage using the color-vision system present in her eye. (Illustration by E. Cronin) are components of the natural visual tasks in which songbirds are engaged and that the research focus is on what the animals actually require from, and can achieve with, their visual systems. Eyes and Their Evolution The ability to sense the presence of light dates back to some of the earliest forms of life on Earth, but it took most of the history of life thereafter to evolve truly functional visual systems capable of imaging scenes and objects and of providing timely information about changes in the visual surround. The first recognizable eyes appear in the fossil record in the Cambrian, some half-billion years ago; incredibly, these are already well-developed compound eyes, not terribly different from eyes functional in some animals today (figure. ). Equally impressive is the discovery of fossils showing that highly complex eyes in an amazing diversity of animals appeared within a few millions or tens of millions of years of evolution. In fact, it has been argued that the appearance of the first truly competent eyes acted as a catalyst for rapid animal evolution and diversification (see Parker, ; Land and Nilsson, ). The rise of high-quality vision enabled animals to remain well oriented during travel at high speed and opened up a new world of long-distance animal interactions

Introduction A B C D Figure 1.3 Fossilized compound eyes of two trilobite species, Bojoscutellum edwardsii (Barrande) (A,C) and Eophacops trapeziceps (Barrande) (B,D), showing dorsal (A,B) and lateral (C,D) views. These eyes were fossilized approximately 450 500 million years ago but show many features of modern compound eyes. (Photographs by E. Clarkson) via predation, predator evasion, and communication. Unfortunately, until time machines become available to researchers, hypotheses about the visual ecology of these animals must remain untested, although comparing them with modern species having similar body plans and inhabiting analogous environments is fruitful for speculation. In recent years, due to the availability of ever more efficient molecular approaches and tools for examining animal phylogenies, there has been an explosion of interest in the evolution of eyes and photoreceptors. This book is not about visual evolution, but visual ecologists are intensely interested in how eyes became mated to their ecological and behavioral functions, and simultaneously, how they are limited by the constraints of their ancestry. But here in the Introduction, it is worth taking a look at how the functions and adaptations discussed in this book might have come into being. Nilsson ( ) has convincingly argued that eyes evolved through a series of four discrete stages, each characterized by a particular task related to photoreception, ultimately leading to high-quality vision. Advances in the underlying biological technology enabled each leap to the next, more competent stage (figure. ). According to Nilsson, once a reasonable way to detect light appeared, living organisms immediately gained a way to measure time of day, day length, depth, the passage of a shadow, and other highly adaptive abilities. Adding directionality and then low-resolution vision and ultimately high-resolution vision placed ever-higher demands on the light-sensing machinery, including improved ways to get enough light into the photosensing cells to provide a useful signal. Nilsson s ( ) view of vision advancing through a series of steps, each permitting a major step in sensory capability, has much to offer. It has the additional utility of providing a view of visual evolution through adaptation for specific ecological functions. 5

6 Chapter 1 Efficient photopigment regeneration Screening pigment Membrane stacking Focusing optics Spatial vision: high resolution Task complexity Spatial vision: low resolution Directional scanning-photoreception Non-directional photoreception Time Figure 1.4 A view of visual evolution as a punctuate process, advancing through a series of steps that represent signifi cant leaps in visual capabilities. Visual ecology has considered organisms at each of the four steps, but by far the greatest interest is in those that have reached the two fi nal steps, where spatial vision comes into play. (After Nilsson, 2009) Themes in Visual Ecology In reading this book you will come to recognize recurring themes that appear in chapter after chapter. One has already been introduced the significance of evolution, and evolutionary history, in shaping the ways in which animals use, and are even able to use, their visual systems. All of vision is united by reliance on a single class of protein molecules (discussed in detail in chapter 3). These are the opsins, clearly special because no visual system that has advanced beyond the very first of Nilsson s stages uses any other molecular design for light reception. All opsins in every seeing creature, from jellyfish to your favorite advanced animal have descended from a common ancestral molecule, possibly some sort of melatonin receptor (figure 1.5). Today, they fall into specific opsin families, either three or four, depending on who is making the argument. Most animals have many opsins available in their genomes, generally of more than one family, and these are the film in the camera of vision. No visual ecologist ignores their properties! A second theme that recurs in many chapters is that of matched filters, an idea introduced by Rüdiger Wehner (1987) in a much- quoted essay in honor of the vision scientist Hansjochem Autrum s 75th birthday. A matched filter is a sensory construct that serves as a reduced model of expected events or qualities in the outside world. Wehner s original essay gave examples from several sensory systems, with vision getting the lion s share. You will meet many examples of matched filters in eyes of animals

OTGRPS Introduction 7 R opsins Group 4 opsins Cnidops C opsins Figure 1.5 Evolution of modern opsins. From an ancestor in the center of the circle, the opsins known today split into four families in this reconstructed phylogeny: C-opsins (used in vertebrate photoreceptors), Cnidops (in jellyfi sh), R-opsins (in major invertebrate groups such as arthropods and molluscs), and a hodgepodge group named group 4 opsins, which have many functions, most (but not all) not used in visual photoreception. OUTGRPS stands for the outgroups used to construct the phylogeny. (Figure prepared by M. Porter) in this book. Filters work by excluding information, and in doing so they simplify the analytical task required to analyze what is left. Filters also let things through, so a more positive view is that they admit just the things an animal needs to know. Rather than run through a series of examples now, we just encourage you to make your way into the book! An Overview of the Book The chapters in this book explore at increasing levels of inquiry the topics that are of interest in visual ecology today. We begin with the properties of light and the photic environment: the basic physics of light, including intensity, spectrum, and polarization; natural sources, distributions, and temporal features of light; how light is

8 Chapter 1 changed as it is absorbed and scattered by air or natural waters; the effects of natural structures and surfaces including the surfaces of living things on light s properties; and ultimately the statistical features of the natural scenes that animals view. Light measurements are important to nearly every visual ecological topic because the photic features in the environments inhabited by a given animal species, and within which it behaves, define the limits of its sensory abilities and shape the possibilities for particular visual tasks. Light s fundamental properties limit the performance of even perfect photochemical and optical systems, but its variation among habitats permits species to specialize and thereby outperform their competitors, predators, or prey. We proceed to consider these photochemical and optical systems in the next three chapters, discussing how photoreceptors intercept light and convert it to a usable biological signal, how the pigments and cells of vision vary among animals, and how the properties of these components affect a given receptor s sensitivity to light s intensity, spectrum, and polarization. Eyes consist of photoreceptor arrays that capture and transduce light, but the performance of these receptor surfaces is ultimately determined by the optics of the eyes within which they are found. These chapters provide the foundation for the rest of the book, which examines how eyes and photo receptors become specialized for an enormous diversity of visual tasks and how various species excel in some ecologically appropriate subsets of all these possibilities. These specializations and tasks are considered at mounting levels of integration throughout the rest of the book, moving in the next three chapters through design- based processes such as spatial, color, and polarization vision. The final five chapters of the book tackle higher- level aspects of visual ecological concerns: motion vision, seeing in attenuating media, dim- light vision, visual orientation, and ultimately communicating with other animals. As the book proceeds, we revisit the animals introduced in the first paragraph of this chapter (and many other fascinating creatures) to examine how their vision enables and limits their behavior. Because humans are such visual animals, it is very difficult to escape thinking of animal vision in human terms. The reader must resist this temptation, as it is nearly always thoroughly misleading. Humans evolved from primate ancestors with a very specialized set of visual needs, and like all primates humans devote a disproportionate fraction of their central nervous processing capacity to visual analysis. Human vision met the needs of our ancestors, but it is only one of a literal infinity of possible solutions of how an animal might see; in fact today, in the days of virtual reality, three- dimensional cinema, and heads- up displays in our machines, it is easy to forget that what we give ourselves to see is specifically designed (by us!) to be processed by our visual systems. We see increasingly artificial scenery. The visual worlds of animals are fundamentally different. More important, they are interpreted by alien modes of processing that are barely analogous to our intuitive concepts of vision. As the reader progresses through this book or just samples its contents, he or she should always bear in mind that the sensory world of each species is unique and that it is risky, and almost always misleading, to make analogies to human visual experience. Given all that has just been said, perhaps it is just as well that we have not yet even attempted a definition of vision! There are many kinds of photoreceptors, and many organs that are clearly eyes of some sort, that sense light levels and their changes but provide at best a very rudimentary sense of space or visual scenes. For the purposes of this book, a visual sense must provide both spatial and temporal information at rates fast enough to enable complex behavior beyond such simple activities as

Introduction 9 photoperiodism or circadian timing. There is no clear line at which complex behavior begins, of course, but we are here mostly concerned with those controlled through the input into two (occasionally one) usually anterior visual organs connected to a discrete neural center in what is clearly a brain. With this as a tentative definition of vision, we now proceed to consider the field of visual ecology.