Similar documents
Conversation Analysis, Discursive Psychology and the study of ideology: A Response to Susan Speer

1/8. The Third Paralogism and the Transcendental Unity of Apperception

TROUBLING QUALITATIVE INQUIRY: ACCOUNTS AS DATA, AND AS PRODUCTS

GENERAL WRITING FORMAT

Conversational Analysis C H A P T E R 5

Welcome to the UBC Research Commons Thesis Template User s Guide for Word 2011 (Mac)

SOUTH AFRICAN SUGAR TECHNOLOGISTS ASSOCIATION INSTRUCTIONS FOR AUTHORS

Title: Narrative as construction and discursive resource Author: Stephanie Taylor

AKAMAI UNIVERSITY. Required material For. DISS 990: Dissertation RES 890: Thesis

Reply to Stalnaker. Timothy Williamson. In Models and Reality, Robert Stalnaker responds to the tensions discerned in Modal Logic

How to write a RILM thesis Guidelines

Discourse analysis is an umbrella term for a range of methodological approaches that

Note that Schegloff's interpretion of what is taking place in the event is used in the analysis instead of the participants' feedback.

What is Character? David Braun. University of Rochester. In "Demonstratives", David Kaplan argues that indexicals and other expressions have a

Abstract Several accounts of the nature of fiction have been proposed that draw on speech act

GUIDELINES FOR THE PREPARATION OF A GRADUATE THESIS. Master of Science Program. (Updated March 2018)

Department of American Studies M.A. thesis requirements

Writing Cover Letters

Thesis/Dissertation Preparation Guidelines

Author Guidelines Foreign Language Annals

Information & Style Sheet for Dissertations and Theses 1

How to Write a Paper for a Forensic Damages Journal

SOCIAL AND CULTURAL ANTHROPOLOGY

Conclusion. One way of characterizing the project Kant undertakes in the Critique of Pure Reason is by

International Human Rights Law Review. Scope. Ethical and Legal Conditions. Submission. Instructions for Authors

WEB FORM F USING THE HELPING SKILLS SYSTEM FOR RESEARCH

Department of American Studies B.A. thesis requirements

Revista CS Journal School of Law and Social Sciences. Call for papers - Issue No. 21 Urban Challenges

INSTRUCTIONS FOR PAPER SUBMISSION

Formats for Theses and Dissertations

Review Your Thesis or Dissertation

Review Your Thesis or Dissertation

Internal assessment details SL and HL


INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL EXCELLENCE (IJEE)

AlterNative House Style

Aspects of Talk Show Interaction:

On Recanati s Mental Files

INDEX. classical works 60 sources without pagination 60 sources without date 60 quotation citations 60-61

Manuel Bremer University Lecturer, Philosophy Department, University of Düsseldorf, Germany

SocioBrains THE INTEGRATED APPROACH TO THE STUDY OF ART

Necessity in Kant; Subjective and Objective

Film sound: Applying Peircean semiotics to create theory grounded in practice

Editorial Policy. 1. Purpose and scope. 2. General submission rules

Culture, Space and Time A Comparative Theory of Culture. Take-Aways

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International Advanced Subsidiary and Advanced Level. Published

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

THESIS AND DISSERTATION FORMATTING GUIDE GRADUATE SCHOOL

Covington High School Intermediate Concert Band Syllabus

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

What Can Experimental Philosophy Do? David Chalmers

Faceted classification as the basis of all information retrieval. A view from the twenty-first century

Torture Journal: Journal on Rehabilitation of Torture Victims and Prevention of torture

Understanding Plagiarism

Title of the Paper (16 pt. Times New Roman, Bold, Centered)

College of MUSIC. James Forger, DEAN UNDERGRADUATE PROGRAMS. Admission as a Junior to the College of Music

PHILOSOPHY. Grade: E D C B A. Mark range: The range and suitability of the work submitted

The Embedding Problem for Non-Cognitivism; Introduction to Cognitivism; Motivational Externalism

Introduction: A collection of notes and papers on epistemics in Conversation Analysis. Michael Lynch. May 7, 2018

Exploring touch: A review of Matthew Fulkerson s The First Sense

Indexing Inferables and Organizational Shifts: 'No'- Prefaces in English Conversation

Journal of Undergraduate Research Submission Acknowledgment Form

Université Libre de Bruxelles

KABARAK UNIVERSITY GUIDELINES TO PREPARING RESEARCH PROPOSAL, THESIS/PROJECT POST GRADUATE STUDENT GUIDE

CONTINGENCY AND TIME. Gal YEHEZKEL

Philosophy of Science: The Pragmatic Alternative April 2017 Center for Philosophy of Science University of Pittsburgh ABSTRACTS

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

Guide for Authors Danish Journal of Management & Business

University of Missouri St. Louis College of Education. Dissertation Handbook: The Recommended Organization and Format of Doctoral Dissertations 2014

University of Wollongong. Research Online

Appendix 6: Specifications for Higher Degree Theses and Portfolios

Organizing Your Essay. A Review of Paragraph Structure

Goldie on the Virtues of Art

GUIDELINES FOR THE CONTRIBUTORS

Writing Guide for Academic Papers

20.109: Writing Results and Materials & Methods Sections

University of Groningen. Tinnitus Bartels, Hilke

The Art of Improvising: The Be-Bop Language

VISUAL ARTS. Overview. Choice of topic

Journal of Open Learning and Research Communication

On The Search for a Perfect Language

GUIDELINES FOR THE PREPARATION OF WRITTEN ASSIGNMENTS

HIST The Middle Ages in Film: Angevin and Plantagenet England Research Paper Assignments

UGRC 110 Academic Writing


Critical Discourse Analysis. Dr. Raz COM400 Fall 2015

MA International Relations Style Sheet: Formal Guidelines for Seminar Papers and MA Theses

Robert Pirsig offers a critique of academic writing.

HISTORY ADMISSIONS TEST. Marking Scheme for the 2015 paper

Imagination Becomes an Organ of Perception

Introduction to Academic Research and Writing

Paper Evaluation Sheet David Dolata, Ph.D.

Guidelines for Reviewers

WG2: Transcription of Early Letter Forms Brian Hillyard

The Aesthetic Experience and the Sense of Presence in an Artistic Virtual Environment

Health Professions Council Education & Training Panel 5 July 2007 NORDOFF ROBBINS MUSIC THERAPY CENTRE - MA MUSIC THERAPY

PHL 317K 1 Fall 2017 Overview of Weeks 1 5

Jokes and the Linguistic Mind. Debra Aarons. New York, New York: Routledge Pp. xi +272.

Types of Publications

0 6 /2014. Listening to the material life in discursive practices. Cristina Reis

Transcription:

MURDOCH RESEARCH REPOSITORY This is the author s final version of the work, as accepted for publication following peer review but without the publisher s layout or pagination. The definitive version is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/14614456080100040602 McHoul, A. (2008) Book review: EMANUEL A. SCHEGLOFF, Sequence Organization in Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis. Vol. 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2007, xvi + 300 pp. Discourse Studies, 10 (4). pp. 576-581. http://researchrepository.murdoch.edu.au/9929/ Copyright: 2008 SAGE It is posted here for your personal use. No further distribution is permitted.

Emanuel A. Schegloff, Sequence Organization in Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis, Volume 1. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2007. xvi+300pp. $**.**. Reviewed by Alec McHoul, School of Media Communication & Culture, Murdoch University. I have no doubt whatsoever that this book will become and remain the definitive work on the analysis of sequencing in conversation and that the projected series of volumes on conversation analysis (CA) as a whole will become and remain the definitive work on CA. It is thorough, comprehensive, rigorous and accessible to beginners and seasoned practitioners alike. And this is no more or less than we should expect from one of the founders of CA itself with a remarkable publication record in the field spanning some 40 years. When the book arrived in the mail, I immediately leapt to the wrong conclusion: that the sequence of the title indicated that the central theme would be the turntaking (or multi-party) problem the question of who gets to speak in a conversation and when. Indeed sequence, sequential and so on have been used this way by several conversation analysts including Sacks himself (see Sacks, 1970; McHoul, 2005). Yet, while aspects of turn-taking do turn up in the book where required, along with matters such as repair, topic and preference organisation, the central theme of this work is the organisation of talk in terms of what Schegloff marks as the central unit of sequencing: the adjacency pair. i Adjacency pairs consist of two, often but not always, contiguous turns at talk: a first pair part (FPP) spoken by one speaker and a second (SPP) by another. They consist then of such familiar things as: question + answer greeting + greeting offer + acceptance/rejection request + granting/refusal telling + return telling Schegloff s take on this is that fundamental (or base ) adjacency pairs are the ultimate building blocks for longer, and sometimes extremely long, stretches of talk. How this is accomplished by actual speakers in actual conversations is by what Schegloff refers to as expansion. Accordingly, the first seven of the book s fourteen chapters are devoted entirely to the achievement of expansion and the remaining seven concern corollaries of the expansion phenomenon, one of which I will deal with in some detail below precisely because of its exceptionality: topicproffering sequences. 1

Expansions of adjacency pairs occur in three fundamental ways: as preexpansions, insert expansions and post-expansions and, significantly enough (bolstering Schegloff s case for the fundamental unit argument), these expansions are often, but not always, themselves constructed as adjacency pairs. The neatness of this approach cannot be underestimated. It shows how massively conversational sequences get worked up as nested turns and how this nesting works around the fundamental sense (for participants) that things get done in conversations by virtue of the upcoming arrival of something, a SPP, and then by virtue of the base adjacency pair s consequences for the rest of the talk. The first of these is best instanced by pre-expansions; things such as pre-invitations (pp.29-34). Invitations are tricky matters. They can meet with acceptance or refusal as their SPPs. And, as Schegloff shows with meticulous attention to detail (which I cannot rehearse here, see chapter 5), the first of these (acceptance) is preferred and the second (refusal) is dispreferred where these terms, preferred and dispreferred refer to a social/interactional feature of sequences... not a psychological one (p.61; original italics). To this extent, both parties to an invitation, inviter and invitee, may have an interest in avoiding an upcoming SSP in its dispreferred form. (And this goes for a whole range of other adjacency pairs, not just for invitations.) The pre-invitation is a solution to this problem. Taking an example from Schegloff, we can see this at work (p.31). Here John wants to invite Judy (and just possibly a third party) over for a talk. 1 Jud: Hi John. 2 Joh: Ha you doin <say what r you doing. 3 Jud: Well, we re going out. Why. Line 2 is the pre-invitation. What it does is to foreshadow a possible upcoming invitation, contingent upon Judy (in the pre-spp) saying, for example, that she has no plans for the evening or some such kind of response. If she does that, John can know that the dispreferred base SPP, a refusal, is less likely than the preferred option, an acceptance. As it turns out, Judy s response to the pre-invitation signals that, were he to have simply made the invitation (that is, without the pre-), the response would have been in the dispreferred form, a refusal. The pre- technique, therefore, avoids the arrival of both the FPP itself, the invitation, and the dispreferred SPP that would have been the likely response to it. What we see here and this is but a tiny fraction of what Schegloff has to say about pre-expansions (he deals in copious detail with six types of them) is not merely a general feature of conversational structure; it is that, but it is also a highly specific and locally useable technique for getting some particular thing done, here and now, in this place, by these particular participants. This is one of the main, as it were, theoretical-methodological upshots of this book. It is aptly signalled by one of the book s epigraphs from Goethe: The general and the 2

particular converge; the particular is the general appearing under various conditions. Schegloff, dare I say, expands on this in his final chapter, Summary and Applications (which I strongly advise any reader to look at first). His remarks are important for anyone who still thinks that CA is either purely structure-seeking (nomothetic) or purely a micro indulgence in transcript fragments (idiographic). He writes:... saying of some turn (or turn-constructional unit, or series of turns) that it is a pre-invitation, or an insert expansion, or a repair sequence, etc. is not the end of analysis; indeed, it is not even the beginning of analysis. It is the preparation of a bit of the target data for analytic inspection, with a candidate possible direction of analysis ready for assessment (pp.260-261; original italics). Let s have a look at this policy at work in a particular instance, one kind of insert expansion. Insert expansions consist of a pair (or several pairs) of turns occurring in the space between the base FPP and the base SSP. They are, if you like, wedged in between the two primary parts of the base adjacency pair. Schegloff makes a further and extremely interesting distinction between two types of insertion: those which orient themselves backwards with respect to the already uttered FPP and those which orient themselves forwards to the yet-to-be-uttered SPP. These two, postfirst insert expansions and pre-second insert expansions, Schegloff claims, are qualitatively different as we shall see after looking at an example (p.102). 1 A: Were you uh you were in therapy with a private doctor? 2 B: Yah. 3 A: Have you ever tried a clinic? 4 B: What? 5 A: Have you ever tried a clinic? 6 B: ((sigh)) No, I don t want to go to a clinic. Here lines 3 and 6 constitute a base adjacency pair. Lines 4 and 5 are the insert expansion oriented in some way to the FPP. What I find interesting about this is Schegloff s claim that such post-firsts are routinely accomplished as repairs (pp.100-106). This is one, and probably the only one, point on which I am not completely convinced by Schegloff s handling of the matter. Though my uptake of it could be considered a minor quibble which it probably is it may have some consequences for Schegloff s general structural claim about the qualitative differences between post-firsts and pre-seconds. What I m trying to get at here is that I am not completely convinced that, for example though it goes for some other cases too lines 4 and 5 above do actually constitute a repair, with line 4 as the other initiation of the correction and line 5 as the self correction. On one reading my objection is weak. This first take hears B s What? as a signal that he has not properly heard A s question 3

and is asking for a repeat. Here I would simply have to ask: is requesting a repeat a form of initiating a correction? It would seem prima facie not to be, at least in the sense that turns 3 and 5 are identical in every respect. A is effectively and hearably bringing the implication that precisely nothing was in need of correction. The second reading brings, I think, a slightly stronger argument against the repair thesis concerning post-first insertions. Here we could hear What? as some kind of objection to (or at least disagreement with) A s very suggestion that B attend a clinic for whatever problem B has; and the emphasis + questioning intonation at least suggest the plausibility of this reading though prolongation of the vowel would have convinced me somewhat more. Again, and I necessarily leave the question dangling: is an expression of objection to, or disagreement with, a suggestion or piece of advice strictly an instance of repair? ii So there s at least a glimmer of a possibility that post-first insertions do things other than just repair and this has consequences for Schegloff s claim about the qualitative distinction between post-firsts and pre-seconds. That is, if my suspicions carry any weight at all (and this would require much further analytic investigation) the following may need some revision: Post-first insert sequences are generic in the sense that they are not differentiated according to the type of sequence in which they figure. Other-initiated repair is a practice with its own characteristics and organization fitted into a larger organization of repair which provides resources for dealing with trouble... Pre-second insert expansions are (with few exceptions) type-specific... That is, these types of insert sequences are preliminary to some particular type of second pair part which has been made relevant next by the type of first pair part to which it is responding (p.106). If I m right, then the contrast (generic vs. type-specific) may not be quite so definite as it is stated here. Interestingly enough, in the sentence following the above passage, Schegloff goes on to say that post-firsts in fact clarify their respective FPPs. In many respects, I m much happier with this formulation, on the proviso we accept that not all clarifications are repairs and vice versa. Still, in the absence of further investigation, the point remains moot; and, besides, who am I to go picking bones with Manny Schegloff where any aspect of CA is concerned?! Now here s one thing I strongly admire about this book and about Schegloff s approach generally. One way of putting this is to say that he s not afraid of exceptions to his own overall findings; in fact, he often uses them to distinct analytic advantage. I m mindful here of an early paper by Schegloff on the openings of phone calls (Schegloff, 1968). Having put forward a highly intuitive rule, that persons called take first turn and callers take second turn, Schegloff decides to reject this generalisation on the basis of a single piece of data where the person called is distracted at the very point of their turn slot and, hence, does not 4

speak first. This one piece of data convinces Schegloff that the first turn is in fact the phone ringing, occasioned, of course, by the caller. So the missing first turn turns out actually to be a missing second turn: a finding with significant consequences for the analysis of phone talk that I have no space to rehearse here. The present book contains something similar, though on a larger scale. Up until the start of chapter 8, Schegloff works with (again a highly intuitive) hypothesis, or proto-rule: preferred responses are sequence-closure relevant and dispreferred responses are sequence-expansion-relevant (p.169). That is, when the parties are audibly in agreement, there s little more to be said on that topic and a new sequence is on the cards. When there s trouble, disagreement, etc. there s cause for expansion. But in chapter 8, Schegloff puts forward a class of sequences that reverse this proto-rule. These are topic-proffering sequences. They routinely begin with questions or, more strictly, with utterances in interrogative form. Here s Schegloff s first example. 1 Ava: That s goo[d 2 Bee: [Dihyuh have any cl You have a class 3 with Billy this te:rm? Having reached the end of a topic sequence, Bee opens up the possibility of Billy becoming the next topic of talk and she does this with a characteristic interrogative. Following this Ava offers a clearly preferred response: Yeh he s in my abnormal [psychology] class. What s significant about this is that the preferred response generates further talk to that topic, while a dispreferred would tend to the opposite pole: sequence closure. Hence a reversal of the proto-rule. A rough picture of this could be as follows: FPP Question and Topic-proffering Interrogative form + and Action performed specific content [ Billy, etc.] SPP Positive/preferred or Negative/dispreferred Sequence expanded or Sequence truncated Topic-sequence opened or Topic-sequence closed Hence we have a structure that reverses the regularly found relations between preferreds/dispreferreds and sequence expansions/sequence closures. As ever, thinking back to Goethe, it is never good enough to close off one s formulations of the general ( rule ) until all the particulars are in. And all the particulars with a nod in the direction of Sir Karl Popper are never in. 5

Finally, something of further importance arises from my little sketch above, hence the indicative italics. That is, FPPs routinely consist of three ingredients: form, content and action. And Schegloff, throughout the book, makes a good deal of this as an important way of looking at adjacency pairs. For example the FPP, discussed in several places in the book, Would you like a cup of coffee? has an interrogative form (it looks on the surface like a question). It has particular content (reference to someone s present desires, coffee, etc.). And it performs a particular action (offering). And in such circumstances at least the form/action couplet is routinely doubly acknowledged in the SPP: say, Yes, thank you very much or No thanks, I ve just had one. The yes or no answer the question, while the thank you or thanks acknowledge the offer. And we can see this no more clearly than in the instance of (joking or cheeky) SPPs that deliberately take the actional aspect of the FPP in the wrong way. (Coulter, 1976: 1; modified) Teacher: How many people don t have paper? ((Looks around)) Student: Five. Significantly enough then, not just a single utterance (the FPP) contains socialactional significance. At least the whole adjacency pair and, if any, its expansions, need to be inspected before that can be seen (cf. McHoul, 1987) and in actual (transcribed) instances. This puts a whole new complexion on studies of discourse and society generally and shows how CA can be critical to such studies. In particular, and this is a very important point which Schegloff modestly relegates to a footnote (p.78, fn10), the old mechanical stand-by of speech-act theory (to name just one candidate) may now have a more considered and alternative; one that is responsive to actual materials, open to re-analysis, and thereby promising a firmly grounded approach to the critical question of how discourse, in and as actual practice, accomplishes social facts. Notes i Shegloff makes the following distinctions which put the matter succinctly: The organization of practices for turn-taking serves to provide for the distribution of opportunities to talk among parties to an interaction. The organization of practices for constructing turns-at-talk serves to shape what is said in those opportunities and how. The practices of action formation allow the composition of a turn... in the position in which it is placed, to constitute some possible recognizable action, and to be understandable as some possible action that is, what is done in those opportunities. Sequence organization organized around its core sequential practice and unit, the adjacency pair relates a series of these productions into coherent courses 6

of action, and thereby harnesses the resources of talk-in-interaction to getting things done (p.263; original italics). ii A basic membership-categorisation analysis could show that there are differences between the predicates [seeing {being in therapy with?} a private doctor] and [being a clinic patient]. The first could possibly be ascribed to just about anybody in the society; the second to someone with a more chronic condition. B s What?" would seem, then, to be an attempt to stave off his/her being taken as a possible incumbent in the latter category. References Coulter, J. (1976) On the Notion of a Sequential Object. Paper presented to the Conference on Ethnomethodology and the Sociology of Language. Sociology Department and Human Interaction Laboratory, State University at Buffalo, NY, February 12, 1976. McHoul, A. (2005) Aspects of Aspects: On Harvey Sacks s Missing Book. Human Studies, 28(2): 113 128. McHoul, A. (1987) Why There are No Guarantees for Interrogators. Journal of Pragmatics, 11(4): 455 471. Sacks, H. (1970) Aspects of the Sequential Organization of Conversation. Unpublished typescript. Schegloff, E.A. (1968) Sequencing in Conversational Openings. American Anthropologist, 70(6): 1075 1095. 7