THE PERSISTENCE OF LOUDNESS IN SPEECH FREQUENCIES INFLUENCE OF ECOLOGICAL CONTEXT.

Similar documents
An overview of Bertram Scharf s research in France on loudness adaptation

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Experiments on tone adjustments

Do Zwicker Tones Evoke a Musical Pitch?

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 (3), March /2000/107(3)/1589/9/$ Acoustical Society of America 1589

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Electrical Stimulation of the Cochlea to Reduce Tinnitus. Richard S. Tyler, Ph.D. Overview

INTENSITY DYNAMICS AND LOUDNESS CHANGE: A REVIEW OF METHODS AND PERCEPTUAL PROCESSES

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

Beltone True TM with Tinnitus Breaker Pro

Client centred sound therapy selection: Tinnitus assessment into practice. G D Searchfield

Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords

Residual Inhibition Functions in Relation to Tinnitus Spectra and Auditory Threshold Shift

Binaural summation of loudness: Noise and two-tone complexes

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

The mid-difference hump in forward-masked intensity discrimination a)

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Residual inhibition functions in relation to tinnitus spectra and auditory threshold shift

Chapter 2 Measurement of Loudness, Part I: Methods, Problems, and Pitfalls

Reliability of Tinnitus Loudness Matches under Procedural Variation

Neural Correlates of Auditory Streaming of Harmonic Complex Sounds With Different Phase Relations in the Songbird Forebrain

Effects of Remaining Hair Cells on Cochlear Implant Function

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

The perception of concurrent sound objects through the use of harmonic enhancement: a study of auditory attention

Effect of room acoustic conditions on masking efficiency

Individual Differences in Loudness Processing and Loudness Scales

S. S. Stevens papers,

Critical Review: Is there evidence to support that hearing aids benefit adults in the reduction of tinnitus perception?

Informational Masking and Trained Listening. Undergraduate Honors Thesis

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Pitch perception for mixtures of spectrally overlapping harmonic complex tones

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Edge-Aware Color Appearance. Supplemental Material

Release from speech-on-speech masking in a front-and-back geometry

August Acoustics and Psychoacoustics Barbara Crowe Music Therapy Director. Notes from BC s copyrighted materials for IHTP

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

MASTER'S THESIS. Listener Envelopment

Voice segregation by difference in fundamental frequency: Effect of masker type

Hearing Research 219 (2006) Research paper. Influence of musical and psychoacoustical training on pitch discrimination

Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Problem Points Score USE YOUR TIME WISELY USE CLOSEST DF AVAILABLE IN TABLE SHOW YOUR WORK TO RECEIVE PARTIAL CREDIT

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

PRODUCT SHEET

Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch

Aalborg Universitet. The influence of Body Morphology on Preferred Dance Tempos. Dahl, Sofia; Huron, David

Tinnitus. Definition. Prevalence. Neurophysiological Model. Progressive Tinnitus Management

Auditory streaming of amplitude modulated sounds in the songbird forebrain

Why are natural sounds detected faster than pips?

Auditory Streaming of Amplitude-Modulated Sounds in the Songbird Forebrain

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Hearing Research 327 (2015) 9e27. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Hearing Research. journal homepage:

COGNITIVE INTERFERENCE IN THE PERCEPTION OF PITCH AND LOUDNESS IN A FIVE-NOTE MUSICAL PATTERN DISSERTATION. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

Music 175: Pitch II. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

The Effects of Stimulative vs. Sedative Music on Reaction Time

Effects of headphone transfer function scattering on sound perception

I. INTRODUCTION. Electronic mail:

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

Supplemental Material for Gamma-band Synchronization in the Macaque Hippocampus and Memory Formation

Reconstruction of Ca 2+ dynamics from low frame rate Ca 2+ imaging data CS229 final project. Submitted by: Limor Bursztyn

Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations

Beltone Tinnitus Breaker Pro: Breaking the tinnitus cycle. Snehal Kulkarni, Au.D. Michael Piskosz, M.S.

The Human Features of Music.

1. BACKGROUND AND AIMS

Pitch is one of the most common terms used to describe sound.

A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL STUDY OF LOW AMPLITUDE SUPERSONIC AIRCRAFT NOISE AND OTHER TRANSIENT SOUNDS

Inhibition of Oscillation in a Plastic Neural Network Model of Tinnitus Therapy Using Noise Stimulus

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

FOR IMMEDIATE RELEASE. Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs) The following Q&A was prepared by Posit Science. 1. What is Tinnitus?

Determination of Sound Quality of Refrigerant Compressors

ADVANCED PROCEDURES FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC NOISE EVALUATION

Calibration of auralisation presentations through loudspeakers

Physicians Hearing Services Welcomes You!

THE PSYCHOACOUSTICS OF MULTICHANNEL AUDIO. J. ROBERT STUART Meridian Audio Ltd Stonehill, Huntingdon, PE18 6ED England

Chapter 2 Tinnitus Treatment as a Problem Area

DIFFERENCES IN TRAFFIC NOISE MEASUREMENTS WITH SLM AND BINAURAL RECORDING HEAD

UNDERSTANDING TINNITUS AND TINNITUS TREATMENTS

EE373B Project Report Can we predict general public s response by studying published sales data? A Statistical and adaptive approach

University of Groningen. Tinnitus Bartels, Hilke

JOURNAL OF BUILDING ACOUSTICS. Volume 20 Number

HEARING SOLUTIONS JAN 2013 MONTHLY MEETING TINNITUS PRESENTED BY DR KUPPERMAN

The presence of multiple sound sources is a routine occurrence

Springer Series in Information Sciences 22

Tinnitus Assessment Appointment

INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC

Centre for Economic Policy Research

A 5 Hz limit for the detection of temporal synchrony in vision

The Cocktail Party Effect. Binaural Masking. The Precedence Effect. Music 175: Time and Space

Transcription:

THE PERSISTENCE OF LOUDNESS IN SPEECH FREQUENCIES INFLUENCE OF ECOLOGICAL CONTEXT. Ernest M. Weiler, Kathleen Cross, Sophia Boudouris, Suzanne Boyce, Laura Kretschmer, David E. Sandman & Joseph Steger Mail # 394, Communication Sciences & Disorders, ernest.weiler@uc.edu, College of Allied Health Sciences, University of Cincinnati, OH, USA 45267 Abstract The initial loudness of a tone persists unless it is near threshold or at high frequencies. Three studies are discussed showing that ICP Loudness Adaptation appeared at all frequencies tested (at 60 db), while simple adaptation loudness appeared only at 8000 Hz. Correlation of ICP adaptation with TEOAE effects and ecological context is discussed. The initial loudness of an ongoing level tone tends to persist (with some exceptions). Why should this be so? It has been long known that steady stimulation of auditory neurons produces declining neural activity (for example, Derbyshire & Davis, 1935). The term loudness adaptation has frequently been used to refer to a decline in the loudness of a level intensity stimulus, in a manner that may parallel the declining neural activity. When the loudness fails to decline, it may be said that loudness "failed to adapt". Is there a measure of loudness adaptation related to the presumed decline in peripheral auditory activity? The question is complicated since Doucet & Relkin (1997) and Relkin & Doucet (1997) have shown that the association between neural activity and loudness loses strength as intensity increases. Investigating the question, Hood (1950) presented a level tone in one ear and asked listeners to match the loudness by adjusting the intensity of a contralateral intermittent comparison tone. The average decline in loudness was matched by about a 22 db decline in contralateral intensity and supported a viable model of loudness coding (Small, 1963; Weiler & Hood, 1977). Weiler & Hood (1977) based their test of the model on auditory "units" rather than direct neural activity. There is evidence of some interaural "induced" adaptation (Bocca & Pestalozza, 1959; Botte, Canevet, & Scharf, 1982; Scharf, 1983; Ward, 1973; Weiler, Gold, Sandman, & Warm, 1992). Monaural techniques promise freedom from putative binaural effects. As described below, there has been controversy about the differences in loudness adaptation found with Simple Adaptation (SA) procedures as opposed to that found with the Ipsilateral Comparison Paradigm (ICP). The minimal approach, called the Simple Adaptation (SA) procedure by Scharf, (1983) asks listeners to give loudness judgments for a level monaural tone without any use of comparison techniques. The tone decay observed at frequencies around 1000 Hz in normal listeners may be limited to intensities within about 30 db SL. In more recent studies, Miskiewicz, Scharf, Hellman, and Meiselman (1993), and Hellman, Miskiewicz and Scharf (1997) extended the frequency range to 16,000 Hz. These authors indicate evidence of increasing Simple Adaptation above about 4000 Hz. Weiler, Sandman, and Dou (1997) noted this appearance of Simple Adaptation at higher intensities and frequencies could also be described as using stimuli outside the speech range, and urged further study.

The Ipsilateral Comparison Paradigm (ICP) was the term used by Dange et al. (1993) to refer to the reference-based procedure used by Weiler, Sandman, and Pederson (1981). Weiler et al. assumed that a sensory comparison stimulus was necessary for cognitive re-evaluation of the level on-going baseline stimulus, and that when the sensory referent was provided the listener would perceive a decline in loudness stemming from the declining neural activity. They presented a continuous 1000 Hz tone monaurally for 7 minutes with a sensory referent in the form of a five sec. 20 db increment stimulus superimposed over the continuous tone every 30 seconds. Participants were instructed to provide Loudness Magnitude Estimates (LMEs) when signaled. Dange, et al. (1993), like Weiler, et al. (2000), found loudness adaptation for a wide variety of intensities at 1000 Hz. Tannen et al. (1996) reported similarly strong ICP adaptation from 250 to 4000 Hz at about 60 dba. Loudness Adaptation and the Speech Range Based on the conjectures of Weiler, Sandman, and Dou (1997), a series of 3 studies have been completed comparing SA and ICP adaptation inside and outside the primary speech frequencies. The speech range is typically cited as extending from approximately 500 to 4000 Hz, although some speech information is found above and below these values. For the studies considered here, 8000 Hz is considered clearly outside the primary speech range. The following research questions were considered: 1) Is ICP loudness adaptation found inside and outside the speech range? 2) Is SA adaptation at 60 db found only outside the speech range? Study #1, served as the pilot study and used a mixed design. The 40 listeners were randomly assigned to one of four equal groups to be tested with both SA and ICP methods at the frequencies of 1000, 4000, 6000, or 8000 Hz, at 60 db HL. Magnitude Estimates of Loudness at the start and end of the procedure were used to calculate the proportional change, (after Scharf, 1983). The ICP method revealed significant loudness adaptation (decline) for all four frequencies. The SA showed significant loudness adaptation (.37 or 37% decline) only at 8000 Hz, and was comparable to the ICP value (.38 or 38%). Weiler, Maguire, Dou & Warm, (Fechner Day, 1999) concluded this data, obtained by Maguire supported the expectations of Weiler, Sandman, and Dou (1997). Study #2, was a completely within group design which focused on 4000, 6000 and 8000 Hz. The purpose was to determine whether the conclusions of Study #1 could be repeated. All 20 listeners were tested by Boudouris with SA and ICP at 60 db HL for all three frequencies. As in study 1, the ICP method yielded significant adaptation at all frequencies while the SA method revealed significant adaptation only at 8000 Hz-outside the primary speech range. However, unlike study 1, the significant SA adaptation at 8000 Hz was lower than the ICP value Study #3, the Current Comprehensive Study Like Study #2, Study #3 is a completely within group design. The purpose of the present study was to extend the frequency range examined by including 250, 500, 1000, and 8000 Hz; and also to determine if the SA and the ICP show identical adaptation at 8000 Hz, (Study 1) or if the SA value is less than the ICP (Study 2).

The Listeners were 20 voluntary subjects, F=17, M=3, ranging in age from 18 to 55 yrs. (mean age 22-23 yrs) with normal hearing. As in Studies 1 & 2, all had normal hearing at 20 db HL at 250, 500, 1000, 2000, 4000 and 8000 Hz (ANSI, 1996). The Instrumentation was the same as in Studies 1 & 2. The ICP and SA methods were performed on a calibrated GSI-16 audiometer, in an IAC sound booth. The Procedures were the same as in the previous studies-- each participant completed a brief training period and all tones were presented at 60 db HL. The ICP presented a continuous monaural tone for 5 min., with a 10 db HL increment (the referent), added to the 60 db HL continuous tone every 30 sec. for 10 sec. During the ICP procedure, listeners were cued to assign magnitude estimates of loudness to each presentation of the baseline intensity and the intermittent referent stimulus. The SA procedure consisted of a continuous monaural tone presented for 5 min. with regular magnitude estimates of loudness solicited on the same schedule used for the ICP. Thus for both procedures, a total of 19 judgments were solicited. The participants were instructed to assign a loudness of "100" to the first baseline stimulus and subsequently to increase or decrease the number, depending on whether they perceived the tone to increase or decrease in loudness. If they perceived no change in the tone, they were instructed to use the same number given in the previous judgment. The 8 conditions formed by the combination of SA and ICP procedures at 250, 500, 1000 and 8000 Hz were presented in random order to each of the listeners. Results for Study #3 Student t-tests revealed that the ICP adaptation was significant at all frequencies. The SA adaptation once again was significant at only 8000 Hz- outside the primary range of speech frequencies. Like Study #2, the SA at 8000 Hz was significantly less than the ICP adaptation. The present study # 3 is compared with Study 2 in Table 1. Table 1: Mean Proportions of Loudness Adaptation for Studies 2 & 3. 250 Hz 500 Hz 1000 Hz 4000 Hz 6000 Hz 8000 Hz Study 2 ICP x x x.32*.37*.44* SA x x x.07.04.15* Study 3 ICP.29*.23*.29* x x.54* SA.02 -.01.01 x x.18 * p<.05, df=19 The adaptation is significantly greater than.00. For the present study the Pearson correlation showed a significant relationship between ICP and SA adaptation at 8000 Hz only (r=.46, df=18, p<.05). This is similar to Study 2 at 8000 Hz (r=.62, df=18, p<.05).

Discussion of 3 studies In all 3 studies the ICP procedure revealed statistically significant loudness adaptation (decline) at all frequencies tested; and in all 3 studies the SA procedure showed significant loudness adaptation only at 8000 Hz, outside the speech range. (See Figure 1). Both studies 2 and 3 found SA adaptation at 8000 Hz to be no more than half the ICP loudness adaptation, whereas the pilot Study 1 found nearly identical adaptation at 8000 Hz. Anomalies of variance and covariance at 6000 Hz in Studies 1 and 2 were tentatively related to the transition from inside to outside the speech range. Significant correlations between ICP and SA adaptation were found only at 8000 Hz. The presence of SA adaptation at 8000 Hz was consistent with the findings of Miskiewicz et al (1993) and Hellman et al (1997). Induced Adaptation and Recalibration Nieder et al. (ASA, 2001) have reported "Recalibration", or a decline in loudness during short term 2AFC studies. Scharf (ISP, 2001) has considered the possibility that this may be the same phenomenon shown by ICP loudness adaptation in the present study. We have not yet found support for this hypothesis in on-going exploratory studies. Weiler and Cobb (1982) had studied ICP adaptation at various referent durations but found no adaptation for a 500 ms

increment (Nieder's longest). Dange et al. (1993), Tannen (1996) and Weiler et al. (2000) found no support for the hypothesis that a sort of central "contrast" effect was responsible for ICP loudness adaptation. "Induced" monaural loudness adaptation appears to be that same "contrast" hypothesis rejected by Dange et al. and others. Dange et al. (1993) and Weiler et al. (2001) found strong effects for decremental referents which were apparently not considered by Nieder et al. or Scharf. Monaural adaptation--peripheral or Central? Weiler, Sandman and Pederson (1981) argued their monaural method (now called the ICP) was more closely tied to peripheral auditory events than the previous binaural techniques Following Collet et al. (1992), Dou, et al. (1999) found significant correlations between suppression of transient evoked otoacoustic emissions (TEOAEs) with classic tone decay, and furthermore with ICP and SA adaptation at 4000 Hz. Classic tone decay and ICP adaptation correlated with each other. This is evidence for a peripheral origin for ICP loudness adaptation, albeit not as simple as Weiler et al. (1981) had hoped for. Ecological Context, and Helson's AL. There is an ecological validity to the absence of simple loudness adaptation (SA) at the primary speech frequencies and above 30 db SL. If one were to be distracted by a simple loudness decline as the auditory neurons showed fatigue, then an additional burden would be placed on the speech recognition system. A level "platform" is desirable for the detection of the rapid and subtle changes that comprise the speech signals. Whatever the mechanism for the absence of simple loudness adaptation, its absence in the speech frequencies has a practical ecological value. Helson (1964) wrote "all psychophysical scaling methods are fundamentally bipolar rating scales with the neutral or zero category provided by the organism whether or not it is incorporated into the scale." The voice frequencies may well be a contextual anchor for the human organism. Peripheral adaptation may be perceived as loudness adaptation within speech frequencies when the ICP provides the referent stimulus. When a steady, redundant, middle intensity tone is presented within the speech range, the SA is at zero, and may serve as the zero point against which changing stimuli are perceived. REFERENCES Bocca, E., and Pestalozza, G. (1959). Auditory adaptation: Theories and facts. Acta Otolaryngologica, 50, 349-353. Botte, M-C, Canevet, G. & Scharf, B. (1982). Loudness adaptation induced by an intermittent tone. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 72, 727-739. Collet, L., Veuillet, E., Micheyl, C., Amabile, J.C. & Morgon, A.(1992). Involvement of medial olivo-cochlear system in loudness adaptation. Advances in Biosciences, 83, 297-303. Dange, A., Warm, J.S., Weiler, E., Nelson, T. & Dember, W. (1993). Ipsilateral loudness adaptation: Robust effects independent of contrast, intensity, and psychophysical method. Journal of General Psychology, 120, 217-244. Derbyshire, A.J., and Davis, H. (1935). The action potentials of the auditory nerve. American Journal of Physiology, 113, 476-504.

Dou, H., Weiler, E.M., Kretschmer, L, Sandman, D.E. (1999). Peripheral factors in loudness adaptation with OAE effects. abs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 2207. Presented at the 138th meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Columbus. Doucet, J.R. & Relkin, E.M. (1997). Neural contributions to the perstimulus compound action potential: Implications for measuring the growth of the auditory nerve spike count as a function of stimulus intensity. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, pp 2720-2734. Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-Level Theory. p. 73. Harper & Row, NY, NY. Hellman, R., Miskiewicz, A., & Scharf, B. (1997). Loudness adaptation and excitation patterns: Effects of frequency and level. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, pp 2176-2185. Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-Level Theory. Harper & Row. NY. NY. Hood, J.D. (1950). Studies in auditory fatigue and adaptation. Acta Otolaryngology, Supplement 92, 1-57. Miskiewicz, Scharf, Hellman and Meiselman (1993). Loudness adaptation at high frequencies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 94, 1281-1286. Nieder, B., Buus, S., Florentine, M., & Scharf, B. (2001). Effect of duration and level of the recalibration tone on the amount of loudness recalibration. abs. 2aPPa7. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 2349. 141st Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Chicago, Il. Relkin, E.M., & Doucet, J.R. (1997). Is loudness simply proportional to the auditory nerve spike count?, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 2735-2740. Sandman, D.E., Weiler, E.M., and Pederson, L.M. (1982). Perstimulatory adaptation by magnitude estimation: effects of time of day, level of continuous tone, and relative level of periodic increments. Journal of Auditory Research, 22, 65-69. Scharf, B. (2001). Sequential Effects in Loudness. ISP Proceedings, Fechner Day 2001, Leipzig, Germany. Scharf, B. (1983). Loudness adaptation. In J. V. Tobias and E. D. Schubert (Eds.), Hearing research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 1-56). New York: Academic Press. Small, A.M. (1963). Auditory adaptation. In J. Jerger (Ed), Modern Developments in Audiology (pp.287-336). New York: Academic Press. Tannen, R.S. (1996). Loudness adaptation and the ICP: Signal frequency and laterality. M.A. thesis, University of Cincinnati, Ohio. Ward, W.D. (1973). Adaptation and fatigue. In J. Jerger (Ed.), Modern Developments in Audiology (2nd ed., pp. 301-344). New York: Academic Press. Weiler, E.M., Boyce, S., Boudouris, S., Maguire,T., Kreschmer, L.W. and Sandman, D.E. Loudness constancy and loudness adaptation: Inside and outside the speech range, ISP Proceedings, Fechner Day 2000. Weiler, E.M. & Cobb, F. (1982). Duration of increment, magnitude estimation adaptation, and a proposed loudness function. Journal of Auditory Research, 22, 233-239. Weiler, E.M., Gold, L.S., Sandman, D.E., and Warm, J.S. (1992). Four triggering factors in loudness adaptation. Journal of General Psychology, 119, (4), 325-334. Weiler, E.M. & Hood, J.D. (1977). An improved model for loudness coding during auditory adaptation. Audiology, 16, 499-506. Weiler, E.M., Maguire,T., Dou, H. and Warm, J.S.: Context, Adaptation Level Theory, and Recalibration: Issues in Loudness Adaptation. ISP Proceedings, Fechner Day 1999, Tempe, AZ). Weiler, E.M., Sandman, D.E., & Dou, H-W. (1997). Puzzles of loudness constancy, loudness adaptation, and speech frequencies. abs. 4pPPa10. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 3171. The 133 rd meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, State College, PA. Weiler, E.M. Sandman, D.E., Nelson, W.D., Janson-Pinto, J., Dange, A.J., Dember, W. M., & Warm. J. S. (2000). Ipsilateral loudness adaptation over multiple intensity levels. Journal of General Psychology, 127. Weiler, E.M., Sandman, D.E., and Pederson, L.M. (1981). Magnitude estimates of loudness adaptation at 60 db SPL. British Journal of Audiology, 15, 204-205.