} ihzeucan Juseum. Warblers (Vermivora pinus) Spectrographic Analysis of Variation in. the Songs of a Population of Blue-winged

Similar documents
song, and the phrase, with the highest frequency); 4, lower part of

DISCRIMINATION BETWEEN REGIONAL SONG FORMS IN THE NORTHERN PARULA

PATTERNS OF SINGING IN A POPULATION OF THE PLAIN TITMOUSE

SONGS OF THE THRUSHES (TURDIDAE), WRENS (TROGLODYTIDAE), AND MOCKINGBIRDS (MIMIDAE) OF EASTERN NORTH AMERICA

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

WHY DO VEERIES (CATHARUS FUSCESCENS) SING AT DUSK? COMPARING ACOUSTIC COMPETITION DURING TWO PEAKS IN VOCAL ACTIVITY

Hill Mynah (Bertram 1970), and the White- Shore Canal. Ponds and marshes were interspersed with shrubs, grassy meadows, and forested areas.

Song Types, repertories and son variability in a population of Chestnut-Sided Warblers

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

CHANGES WITH TIME IN THE SONGS OF A POPULATION OF CHAFFINCHES

Repertoire matching between neighbouring song sparrows

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Red-winged blackbirds Ageliaus phoeniceus respond differently to song types with different performance levels

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

A test for repertoire matching in eastern song sparrows

A comparative approach to vocal learning: intraspecific variation in the learning process

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

2013 Music Style and Composition GA 3: Aural and written examination

TESTS OF THE FUNCTION OF THE SONG REPERTOIRE IN BOBOLINKS

leucophrys, and certain other species, but no juncos. In January 1959 they

Different Responses to Different Song Types in American Redstarts

LONG-TERM PERSISTENCE OF SONG DIALECTS IN THE MOUNTAIN WHITE-CROWNED SPARROW

Precision testing methods of Event Timer A032-ET

Olga Feher, PhD Dissertation: Chapter 4 (May 2009) Chapter 4. Cumulative cultural evolution in an isolated colony

SONG CHARACTERISTICS AND VARIATION IN A POPULATION OF BANANAQUITS ON PUERTO RICO

Song Dialect of Oriental Magpie-robin (Copsychus saularis) in Northern Thailand

SINGING ORGANIZATION DURING AGGRESSIVE INTERACTIONS AMONG MALE YELLOW-RUMPED CACIQUES

Preliminary Study in whether land management affects dialect formation in Bobolinks

FLOW INDUCED NOISE REDUCTION TECHNIQUES FOR MICROPHONES IN LOW SPEED WIND TUNNELS

FREQUENCY VARIATION IN SONGS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES (PARUS ATRICAPILLUS)

Spare Parts, Accessories, Consumable Material for Older Design Recorders

Responses of male Red-eyed Vireos (Vireo olivaceus) to song playback varying in rate and cadence. H. Lynn Bradman. University of Nebraska

Analysis and Discussion of Schoenberg Op. 25 #1. ( Preludium from the piano suite ) Part 1. How to find a row? by Glen Halls.

IP Telephony and Some Factors that Influence Speech Quality

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Effectively Managing Sound in Museum Exhibits. by Steve Haas

Experimental evidence that distinct song phrases in the Grey-cheeked Fulvetta Alcippe morrisonia permit species and local dialect recognition

Human Hair Studies: II Scale Counts

Ranging of songs with the song type on use of different cues in Carolina wrens: effects of familiarity

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Singing Behavior and Geographic Variation in the Songs of the Veery (Catharus Fuscescens) Across the Appalachian Mountains

Impact of Clock Content on the CDR with Propose Resolution

Electrical and Electronic Laboratory Faculty of Engineering Chulalongkorn University. Cathode-Ray Oscilloscope (CRO)

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

CM3106 Solutions. Do not turn this page over until instructed to do so by the Senior Invigilator.

Atypical Song Reveals Spontaneously Developing Coordination between Multi-Modal Signals in Brown- Headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater)

Gyorgi Ligeti. Chamber Concerto, Movement III (1970) Glen Halls All Rights Reserved

Simple Harmonic Motion: What is a Sound Spectrum?

SONG REPERTOIRES AND SONG SHARING BY AMERICAN REDSTARTS

Auditory Illusions. Diana Deutsch. The sounds we perceive do not always correspond to those that are

Music in Practice SAS 2015

SINGING BEHAVIOR AND GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN THE SONGS OF THE VEERY (CATHARUS FUSCESCENS) ACROSS THE APPALACHIAN MOUNTAINS COURTNEY L.

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

THE SONG RELATIONSHIPS OF FOUR SPECIES OF GROUND CRICKETS

MUSICAL INSTRUMENT RECOGNITION WITH WAVELET ENVELOPES

Received 27 July ; Perturbations of Synthetic Orchestral Wind-Instrument

The Cocktail Party Effect. Binaural Masking. The Precedence Effect. Music 175: Time and Space

Flight Data Recorder - 10

Linear Time Invariant (LTI) Systems

Optimizing Digital Transfer of U-matic Video Recordings Leo Backman/DigiOmmel & Co.

Salt on Baxter on Cutting

No. 122 supplement - (Vol.VII) October 1996

Cadet Music Theory Workbook. Level One

Please fax your students rhythms from p.7 to us AT LEAST THREE DAYS BEFORE the video conference. Our fax number is

Informational Masking and Trained Listening. Undergraduate Honors Thesis

Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

6.5 Percussion scalograms and musical rhythm

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

Eventide Inc. One Alsan Way Little Ferry, NJ

hit), and assume that longer incidental sounds (forest noise, water, wind noise) resemble a Gaussian noise distribution.

Acoustic and neural bases for innate recognition of song

COZI TV: Commercials: commercial instructions for COZI TV to: Diane Hernandez-Feliciano Phone:

Comparison Parameters and Speaker Similarity Coincidence Criteria:

SREV1 Sampling Guide. An Introduction to Impulse-response Sampling with the SREV1 Sampling Reverberator


Semester 1 Semester 2

NOVEL DESIGNER PLASTIC TRUMPET BELLS FOR BRASS INSTRUMENTS: EXPERIMENTAL COMPARISONS

2. AN INTROSPECTION OF THE MORPHING PROCESS

Divider Motion heading graphics

Instrumental Performance Band 7. Fine Arts Curriculum Framework

Tonal Polarity: Tonal Harmonies in Twelve-Tone Music. Luigi Dallapiccola s Quaderno Musicale Di Annalibera, no. 1 Simbolo is a twelve-tone

Florida Performing Fine Arts Assessment Item Specifications for Benchmarks in Course: Chorus 5 Honors

GB-ER series. Direct connection to LH70/71 series

Electrospray-MS Charge Deconvolutions without Compromise an Enhanced Data Reconstruction Algorithm utilising Variable Peak Modelling

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Voices From the Deep. Description. Objectives. Essential Questions. Background Information

INFO.* CUNY+ Developments. The Newsletter of the York College Library Fall 1993

Musical Sound: A Mathematical Approach to Timbre

OPERATIVE GUIDE P.I.T. PILE INTEGRITY TEST

REMOTE I/O R30 SERIES. PC CONFIGURATOR SOFTWARE Model: R30CFG Ver Users Manual

TitleVocal Shimmer of the Laryngeal Poly. Citation 音声科学研究 = Studia phonologica (1977),

Amateur TV Receiver By Ian F Bennett G6TVJ

RX40_V1_0 Measurement Report F.Faccio

MP212 Principles of Audio Technology II

THE CONTROL OF SINGING IN VARIED THRUSHES CARL LINN WHITNEY. B.S., Iowa State University, 1970 M.SCi, The University of British Columbia, 1973

11. SUMMARY OF THE BASIC QUANTIFIER TRANSLATION PATTERNS SO FAR EXAMINED

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Coastal Carolina University Faculty Senate Consent Agenda March 4, 2015 COLLEGE OF HUMANITIES AND FINE ARTS

PCIe: EYE DIAGRAM ANALYSIS IN HYPERLYNX

Transcription:

} ihzeucan Juseum PUBLISHED BY THE AMERICAN MUSEUM OF NATURAL HISTORY CENTRAL PARK WEST AT 79TH STREET, NEW YORK 24, N.Y. NUMBER 2I76 APRIL 28, I 964 Spectrographic Analysis of Variation in the Songs of a Population of Blue-winged Warblers (Vermivora pinus) BY WESLEY E. LANYON1 AND FRANK B. GILL2 Recent interest in the variation of avian vocalizations is due to (1) an increasing awareness of the significance of such variation in our understanding of the social and systematic relationships among individuals, populations, and species, and to (2) technological advancements in recording and analytical procedures (Borror, 196; Kellogg, 196). Apart from its intrinsic value, as a basis for comparison with the variation that has been demonstrated in other species, a spectrographic analysis of the songs of the Blue-winged Warbler (Vermivora pinus) is of special interest because of the potential importance of song in the relationships between this species and the Golden-winged Warbler (V. chrysoptera). We have demonstrated through the use of playback techniques that song has an important role in effecting species discrimination in some Blue-winged Warblers (Gill and Lanyon, in press). But, where their ranges overlap, these forms are known to hybridize, and there are numerous field observations of individuals singing "aberrant" patterns and having bivalent repertoires of songs characteristic of both forms (Eisenmann, 1946; Short, 1962). A thorough analysis of variation in song within an area where both species occur on a common breeding ground has not yet 1 Associate Curator, Department of Ornithology, the American Museum of Natural History. 2 Museum of Zoology, the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

2 AMERICAN MUSEUM NOVITATES NO. 2176 been made. Preliminary to such a study we felt it desirable to ascertain, through spectrographic analysis, the limits of individual and intrapopulational variation in the songs recorded from Blue-winged Warblers in a locality where Golden-winged Warblers do not breed. This constituted the objective of a study conducted at the Kalbfleisch Field Research Station of the American Museum of Natural History while Gill was a National Science Foundation Undergraduate Research Fellow in 1961 and a recipient of a Frank M. Chapman Award in 1962 and 1963. FIG. 1. This male Blue-winged Warbler was mist-netted at the Kalbfleisch Station and color-banded to facilitate individual recognition after release. METHODS Our study population consisted of Blue-winged Warblers that breed at the Kalbfleisch Field Research Station, Huntington, Long Island, New York. The Golden-winged Warbler is rarely found as a breeding bird on Long Island and has not bred at the Kalbfleisch Station since the latter was established in 1958. During the summers of 1961, 1962, and 1963, from 12 to 14 pairs of Blue-winged Warblers bred on the 94 acres of the Station, arriving in early May and departing by mid-august. Most of these birds were mist-netted and color-banded to facilitate individual recognition (fig. 1) and exhibited little, if any, evidence of intro-

1964 LANYON AND GILL: WARBLERS 3 gression with V. chrysoptera. The recordings analyzed here were made from five color-banded males in 1962 and 11 color-banded males in 1963. In 1962 we used a Transmagnemite tape recorder that operated at a speed of 15 inches per second. In 1963, a Uher 4 tape recorder that operated at a speed of 71/2 inches per second was used. In both years the recordings were made with an Altec 66B microphone mounted in a parabolic reflector 24 inches in diameter, and a preamplifier. Analyses of all recordings were made with a Kay Electric Company Sonagraph, with the use of the "high-shape" setting. Since the songs of Blue-winged Warblers contain fundamental frequencies above 8 kilocycles, the upper limit of the frequency scale normally registered by our Sonagraph, it was necessary for us to analyze them at one-half of the normal speed. This procedure effectively doubled the frequency scale (16 kilocycles instead of 8 kilocycles) and halved the time scale (1.2 seconds instead of 2.4 seconds) normally associated with a sound spectrogram. The decreased duration of each graph necessitated splicing together two graphs to complete the analysis of most songs. The graph that resulted from this type of analysis, when compared with a graph made from a song played back at normal speed, will appear to be compressed vertically (with a concomitant loss in frequency-resolution) and extended horizontally with a corresponding improvement in time-resolution). We further improved the resolution in time by using the wide band-pass filter. A comparison of graphs made with this filter and the narrow band-pass filter is shown in figure 2. Though the term "song" may offend some readers as being too ambiguous, we feel strongly that more specific terms implying function or connoting motivation should be avoided until more is known of the behavioral significance of these vocalizations. The term "song" is used here in reference to those vocal patterns that are commonly rendered by territorial males and that are more complex in character and of longer duration and greater intensity than other elements within the male's vocal repertoire (call notes, twitters, and so on). There is no "flight song" in this species that is in any way distinct from the vocalizations that we have included here under the general category of "song." BASIC STRUCTURE OF SONGS Four basic components have been recognized in the songs recorded from 16 males in this population, each of which is structurally distinct when viewed spectrographically and audibly distinct when heard separately by the human ear. The first of these we have designated component A (fig. 2A). At normal speed this is heard as a continuous, rasping type

._ -OD =6,A&I ~~~ ~~~~~~~~ o. m c_ k C a g t ;. Q 1. ~ eo 3O * -3 s --~~~~~~~~~C) O~ MA. I- I '-\ ~ ~ ~ ~ -1- -F /\ '1 --' LU] W ~ IT ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ ~~~~~~~~~~~t -2 ;,/ -r- /> o~~~~~~~~~~" * Si3DAD o 33n3s o

1964 LANYON AND GILL: WARBLERS 5 of note, composed of energy distributed over nearly 2 kilocycles but centering roughly upon 6 kilocycles. The narrow filter of the sound spectrograph provides us with a graph of this component that represents the sound much as the human ear perceives it-a continuous, nonpulsating note. But the wide filter, with its greater resolution in the time dimension, clearly indicates that this component actually consists of one or more oscillating elements. These oscillations occur at a rate of more than 2 per second, and cannot be heard by the human ear until the component is slowed down to one-sixteenth of the normal speed. Variation in component A within the repertoire of a single male appears to be limited to subtle variations in the duration and configuration of the note which are not perceptible to the human ear (fig. 6). Variation from male to male within the population is more pronounced. The duration of the component in our recordings varies from brief bursts of.5 second to longer series up to.8 second, though most renditions are just under.5 second in length. An unusually long component A appears in figure 4C, while a short burst appears in figure 4B and at the beginning of the terminal portion of each song pattern in figure 7. The most noticeable variation among males is whether or not the average frequency of the longer renditions remains constant throughout the duration of the component. Eight males rendered component A's which did not change perceptibly in average frequency, as in figure 2A and in all the graphs in figures 3 and 4. But seven males habitually rendered component A's that were noticeably down-slurred, in some instances by as much as 3 kilocycles. Examples of this variation are shown in the three graphs in figure 5. Component B consists of a series of discontinuous elements, delivered at a rate of about 4 per second (varying from 36 to 42 in our series) and is illustrated in figure 2B. Each individual element within this component appears as an inverted V when graphed with the wide filter, with the ascending portion often being longer and of greater intensity. The graphic configuration of the elements in component B is remarkably constant within the individual (fig. 6) but varies considerably from one male to another (as in the terminal portion of those song patterns in figs. 3 to 5 in which component B is represented). When heard at normal speed, the only perceptible variation in the component B's rendered by the various males is in the duration of the component-from.25 to.95 second in our series. Component C is heard by the human ear as a buzzy trill. The wide filter indicates that it consists of a continuously oscillating element, with energy concentrated on the descending portion of each oscillation (fig. 2C). The rate of these oscillations varies from 6 to 1 per second. The

cli t!d rt ~~~C C.)~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~' S3_AO- IA CP ~~~~~~ C; a)a C)< ^' - =:'-m- AON~ nn Ce, -5- ~~~~~o ri) ^ -oc - O~._COS :-~~~ ---)z ~ S3DAOXN S~LL I i yi a. lad3-3r ()C) nt(

1964 LANYON AND GILL: WARBLERS 7 component may be expressed in brief bursts, lasting less than.5 second (as in the central portion of each song pattern in fig. 5 and in the introductory portion of each pattern in fig. 7), or in longer renditions of up to.5 second (as in the terminal portion of each song pattern in figs. 7 and 8). Component D consists of a series of discrete elements, rendered at constant intervals one from another. According to the wide-band filter, these elements are of two types (fig. 2D). One is related to the element that appears as a continuously oscillating note in component C, though in this instance only one-half of each oscillation (usually the upper half) is registered by the sound spectrograph, and this at very low intensity. Its presence in component D is not discernible by the human ear. Alternating with this low-intensity element is a more prominent note which is not found in any other component. Variation in component D among males consisted chiefly of differences in the configuration of this highintensity element (note differences in figs. 7 and 8) and in the delivery rate of this same element-from seven to 12 per second in our series. Duration of this component varied from.25 to 1. second. VARIATION IN SONG PATTERNS Variation in the songs of this population is achieved as a result of change in the sequences and combinations of these four basic components. Our analysis of this variation has confirmed field observations (Saunders, 1951; Ficken and Ficken, 1962) that, in this species as in many wood warblers (Parulidae), there are two distinct types of song. We have arbitrarily designated these as type I and type II. Though their structural differentiation is now clearly understood, we are unable at this time to differentiate them on functional grounds. Both types of song can be heard throughout the three-month period during which these warblers are present at the Kalbfleisch Station, though song type II is heard more prominently during the month that follows the cessation of nesting. We have heard both types of song from territorial males, before and after encounters with intruding birds. In the course of playback experiments in June, the initial vocal response of a territorial male was invariably a rendition of song type I, even in the event that the playback recording was of song type II. Following the cessation of a playback recording, however, a territorial male often gave a number of renditions of song type II, later to be followed by resumption of song type I. We frequently took advantage of this in our efforts to obtain recordings of type II patterns.

C: QC)..^.rt _ -t C) (D~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~C _ : or" ~~~~~~~~~~~. ~ ~~~~~~ _ C- C-~~~~~~~~~c.44_.. z =" s~~~~~~~~~ - e:: = = C-~~~~~~~~~~~~- ct~cr _v o _=7L. _U) <\s --,,. _ ( e _ ST.,O 1I - _ C-~~~~~~b (D~ ~ ~ ~ ~ -(,.g; _,j_ -O.s O~~~~~~~~~~~~C C\j ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~C _._ : Q~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ C- _o _o oc) O N.ON 5 ~~. Oa) tota, cd O s t N O N s t N _~~~CI) 4 (NJ M l4 CJ D3D C)>~4~

1964 LANYON AND GILL: WARBLERS 9 Type I songs in our population consisted of various combinations of components A, B, and C, with component A being given consistently as the introductory phase of each variant pattern. In our series of recordings of type I songs from 15 males, there are only four different combinations of basic components (intrapopulational variation). The distribution of males according to these four combinations is presented in table 1. This intrapopulational variation in song type I is illustrated by the spectrograms in figures 3 through 5. The songs that we have classified as type I are generally described by the field observer as being weak, non-musical, and consisting of two or TABLE 1 DISTRIBUTION OF MALESa ACCORDING TO THE COMBINATIONS OF BASIC COMPONENTS USED IN SONG TYPE I Component Components Components Components A A+B A+C+B A+C XY-W (fig. 3A) RX-BG (fig. 3B) WR-YX (fig. 4C) GW-XR (fig. 5C) XB-RY (fig. 3C) BW-XR (fig. 5A) RX-BW (fig. 4A) XR-R (fig. 5B) GY-XR (fig. 4B) XW-YG BG-XY CX- RW-YX G-RX XRa Identified by symbols representing color-band combinations. more insect-like, buzzing notes (Saunders, 1951). Possibly the only consistent distinction that is made in the field between the variant patterns of song type I illustrated here is the subjective impression of whether the terminal portion of the song rises or falls in pitch (frequency) with respect to the introductory component A. For example, in those songs in which the down-slurred variation of component A is used (fig. 5), the terminal phase generally sounds higher-pitched by way of contrast. No male within our population was known to render more than one of the variant patterns of type I illustrated in figures 3 through 5. Variation within the many renditions of song type I of a particular male (individual variation) was limited to differences in the length of the continuous components, A and C, and to differences in the number of elements included in a series of the discontinuous component B. Individual variation did not include alteration of the combination and sequence of the basic components. This limited variability within an in-

_. cr{ m (( C?i -_i.. - co E, Z mss ' ts~~~zdk; f. 5 a C o - c bj fr 3~, FN8o E. C-~ o-i I~i CD Iko N ),O N-~n ui ~" i

1964 LANYON AND GILL: WARBLERS 11 dividual's renditions of its song type I is illustrated by the series of recordings that we have for male GY-XR. The type I song of this male was unique in that it included a brief burst of component A between the longer components A and B (fig. 4B). Twenty-two renditions of this song were recorded on June 2, 1963, and nine more renditions were recorded on June 5, 1963. The extremes in variation found within this series of recordings from one individual are illustrated in figure 6. The individuality of the song was maintained by the inclusion of the short burst of component A as an intermediate phase in all 31 renditions, whereas variation was limited to alterations in the duration of the components TABLE 2 DISTRIBUrION OF MALESa ACCORDING TO THE COMBINATION OF BASIC COMPONENTS USED IN SONG TYPE II Components Components Components C+D+A+C A+C+D+C D+A+C CX- (fig. 7A) BG-XY (fig. 8B) GY-XR (fig. 8C) XB-RY (fig. 7B) RW-YX (fig. 7C) G-RX (fig. 8A) WR-YX RX-BW RX-BG XGa Identified by symbols representing color-band combinations. and hence the entire song pattern. The 31 renditions varied from 1.12 to 1.53 seconds, and averaged 1.29 seconds in length. In one instance there was a brief interruption in the introductory component (fig. 6A). Type II songs in our population consisted of various combinations of components A, C, and D. They were characterized by the presence of component D and the absence of component B. In our series of type II songs from 1 males, there are only three different combinations of basic components represented (intrapopulational variation). The distribution of males according to these three combinations is presented in table 2. This intrapopulational variation in song type II is illustrated in figures 7 and 8. To the field observer song type II often sounds longer and more variable than song type I. In our series of recordings, however, type II patterns averaged only slightly longer than type I patterns for each of six males for which there were adequate samples (table 3). Intrapopulational

ct~~ ~ ~ _ cm (3._ C- O- V " _F - Ebi īc o -b * al) QD C\ * r\js t - Cj,5 S3-1 JAO 1N I) NI AO N3 n3osi8

1964 LANYON AND GILL: WARBLERS 13 variation appears to be no greater in type II patterns than in type I patterns in our population. On the contrary, there was greater uniformity among males in the combination of basic components used in type II patterns than in type I patterns. This is evidenced by the fact that all but two of the type II patterns in our recordings consisted of the same combination and sequence of basic components (table 2), whereas two distinct combinations of components were prevalent in our recordings of type I patterns (table 1). TABLE 3 AVERAGE DURATION (IN SECONDS) OF SONGS OF SIX MALESa Song Type I Song Type II GY-XR 1.29 (31)b 1.31 (12) RX-BW 1.22 (9) 1.3 (1) CX- 1.38 (1) 1.49 (7) WR-YX 1.53 (1) 1.64 (5) RX-BG 1.27 (6) 1.37 (9) XB-RY 1.42 (6) 1.52 (4) a Identified by symbols representing color-band combinations. b Number of songs analyzed. Individual variation in type II patterns was limited to differences in the length of continuous components, A and C, and to differences in the number of elements included in a series of the discontinuous component D. As in the case of song type I, individual variation in type II patterns did not include alteration of the combination and sequence of the basic components. The graphs in figure 9 illustrate the extent to which variation occurs within a male's renditions of its type II patterns. Each male Blue-winged Warbler in this population had a vocal repertoire that included one type I pattern and one type II pattern. The total size of an individual's repertoire of songs, then, consisted of but two distinct patterns. No male was found to have more than this number. With respect to size of individual repertoires of distinct song patterns, the Blue-winged Warbler would appear to be somewhat less restricted than the Chipping Sparrow (Spizella passerina) and White-crowned Sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys) in which each male is reported to have but one song pattern (Marler and Isaac, 196; Marler and Tamura, 1962). Somewhat larger individual repertoires of from three to nine distinct song patterns have been documented spectrographically for the Great Tit, Parus major (Gompertz, 1961), thrushes of the genus Hylocichla (Borror,

=~~~~~~ S313AD =~~~~ *= 2 tsp -e s - s~~~~~~~~c O Ce) L. t, - r bc) 3-1DA 1 IA LNJ "_) O\ U)O D( O)_so ~~~~~~~~~~~- e~ Er )._ 1MNl ADN3nO3 8S >~~~~~~~~~~~~~~b1

t~~~~~~~~ U _ C~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~\ x -a Cf! -~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ A z~~~~~~~~- )~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~C o - E A ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~C -~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ 'so a.) t -~~~~~~~~~~~~ - 33A C-N D3n3S OD Qo D L \j C) ~- o V S =, K ~~~~~~~~~ k D- IlN? NN N- O no Ai S t

c:~~~om( OA ( ~~~~~~ _ I - (-_ 21E " C CC PC- _ c - o~~~~c) (X1 K c) _ bu) a -~~~~~l " CU cm ) ~ ~~~~~~ x(f) a ~~~~~~~~~ _. ~~ c_c"q A5 --~- I e I >8 -r~ ~~~v -) LU g'j3 ( LU] wc ''k-- O o N C\i > "bo IDCC \ -- n SI]DAOO1I19 NI ASNifloid C

1964 LANYON AND GILL: WARBLERS 17 1961a), and the Western Meadowlark, Sturnella neglecta (Lanyon, 1957). Borror (1961b) provides additional data on intraspecific variation in other species. In view of field observations of individual Blue-winged Warblers having bivalent repertoires of songs characteristic of both V. pinus and V. chrysoptera, in areas where these two species occur together, two points resulting from our study are of special interest and merit repetition here. (1) No male was found to have more than one type I pattern and one type II pattern in its repertoire of song. (2) There was no evidence that a male varies either of these two song patterns by altering the combination and sequence of the basic components. One immediately wonders what factors may be operating to increase this limited individual variability in at least some individuals in areas of sympatry. We have no data at present on the extent of geographical variation in the songs of the Blue-winged Warbler. Above all, detailed spectrographic studies are needed of recordings made from individuals suspected of having bivalent repertoires, as well as information concerning the ontogeny of song in this species. SUMMARY A spectrographic analysis of tape recordings of Blue-winged Warblers at one locality on Long Island, New York, revealed the limits of individual and intrapopulational variation in the song of that species in an area where the closely related Golden-winged Warbler does not breed. Intrapopulational variation consisted of different combinations and sequences of four basic components. Individual variation involved minor alterations in the duration and configuration of these components but did not include changes in their combination or sequence. Each male had a repertoire of only two distinct song patterns. The significance of these findings is discussed with reference to reports that some Blue-winged Warblers have a greater variability in song in areas where the Golden-winged Warbler also occurs. BIBLIOGRAPHY BORROR, D. J. 196. The analysis of animal sounds. In Lanyon, W. E., and W. N. Tavolga (eds.), Animal sounds and communication. Publ. Amer. Inst. Biol. Sci., no. 7. 1961a. Variation in thrush songs. Wheaton Club Bull., vol. 6, pp. 9-15. 1961b. Intraspecific variation in passerine bird songs. Wilson Bull., vol. 73, pp. 57-78.

18 AMERICAN MUSEUM NOVITATES NO. 2176 EISENMANN, E. 1946. Interchange of song between the Blue-winged and Golden-winged Warblers. Proc. Linnaean Soc. New York, nos. 54-57, pp. 53-54. FICKEN, M. S., AND R. W. FICKEN 1962. The comparative ethology of the wood warblers: a review. Living Bird, First Ann. Cornell Lab. Ornith., pp. 13-122. GILL, F. B., AND W. E. LANYON [In press.] Experiments on species discrimination in Blue-winged Warblers. Auk. GOMPERTZ, T. 1961. The vocabulary of the Great Tit. Brit. Birds, vol. 54, pp. 369-394, 49-418. KELLOGG, P. P. 196. Considerations and techniques in recording sound for bio-acoustics studies. In Lanyon, W. E., and W. N. Tavolga, (eds.), Animal sounds and communication. Publ. Amer. Inst. Biol. Sci., no. 7. LANYON, W. E. 1957. The comparative biology of the meadowlarks (Sturnella) in Wisconsin. Publ. Nuttall Ornith. Club, no. 1, 67 pp. MARLER, P., AND D. IsAAc 196. Physical analysis of a simple bird song as exemplified by the Chipping Sparrow. Condor, vol. 62, pp. 124-135. MARLER, P., AND M. TAMURA 1962. Song "dialects" in three populations of White-crowned Sparrows. Condor, vol. 64, pp. 368-377. SAUNDERS, A. 1951. A guide to bird songs. New York, Doubleday and Co., 37 pp. SHORT, L. 1962. The Blue-winged Warbler and Golden-winged Warbler in central New York. Kingbird, vol. 12, pp. 59-67.