DISCRIMINATION BETWEEN REGIONAL SONG FORMS IN THE NORTHERN PARULA

Similar documents
A test for repertoire matching in eastern song sparrows

} ihzeucan Juseum. Warblers (Vermivora pinus) Spectrographic Analysis of Variation in. the Songs of a Population of Blue-winged

song, and the phrase, with the highest frequency); 4, lower part of

Hill Mynah (Bertram 1970), and the White- Shore Canal. Ponds and marshes were interspersed with shrubs, grassy meadows, and forested areas.

Red-winged blackbirds Ageliaus phoeniceus respond differently to song types with different performance levels

Repertoire matching between neighbouring song sparrows

Female Cowbird Song Perception: Evidence for Different Developmental Programs within the same Subspecies

Different Responses to Different Song Types in American Redstarts

Responses of male Red-eyed Vireos (Vireo olivaceus) to song playback varying in rate and cadence. H. Lynn Bradman. University of Nebraska

Preliminary Study in whether land management affects dialect formation in Bobolinks

Experimental evidence that distinct song phrases in the Grey-cheeked Fulvetta Alcippe morrisonia permit species and local dialect recognition

A comparative approach to vocal learning: intraspecific variation in the learning process

CHANGES WITH TIME IN THE SONGS OF A POPULATION OF CHAFFINCHES

LONG-TERM PERSISTENCE OF SONG DIALECTS IN THE MOUNTAIN WHITE-CROWNED SPARROW

Song Dialect of Oriental Magpie-robin (Copsychus saularis) in Northern Thailand

Choral Sight-Singing Practices: Revisiting a Web-Based Survey

Acoustic and neural bases for innate recognition of song

Author's personal copy

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

SINGING ORGANIZATION DURING AGGRESSIVE INTERACTIONS AMONG MALE YELLOW-RUMPED CACIQUES

PATTERNS OF SINGING IN A POPULATION OF THE PLAIN TITMOUSE

Algebra I Module 2 Lessons 1 19

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

WHY DO VEERIES (CATHARUS FUSCESCENS) SING AT DUSK? COMPARING ACOUSTIC COMPETITION DURING TWO PEAKS IN VOCAL ACTIVITY

SONGS OF THE JAPANESE POPULATION OF THE WINTER WREN (TROGLODYTES TROGLODYTES)

Animal Behaviour 77 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Song Types, repertories and son variability in a population of Chestnut-Sided Warblers

The Effects of Study Condition Preference on Memory and Free Recall LIANA, MARISSA, JESSI AND BROOKE

Microdialect and Group Signature in the Song of the Skylark Alauda arvensis

SOUND LABORATORY LING123: SOUND AND COMMUNICATION

Ranging of songs with the song type on use of different cues in Carolina wrens: effects of familiarity

Changes in fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus) song over a forty-four year period in New England waters

Singing Behavior and Geographic Variation in the Songs of the Veery (Catharus Fuscescens) Across the Appalachian Mountains

SINGING BEHAVIOR AND GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN THE SONGS OF THE VEERY (CATHARUS FUSCESCENS) ACROSS THE APPALACHIAN MOUNTAINS COURTNEY L.

FREQUENCY VARIATION IN SONGS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES (PARUS ATRICAPILLUS)

Gryllus texensis n. sp.: A Widely Studied Field Cricket (Orthoptera; Gryllidae) from the Southern United States

ADELAIDE'S WARBLER (DENDROICA ADELAIDAE)

SONG REPERTOIRES AND SONG SHARING BY AMERICAN REDSTARTS

TESTS OF THE FUNCTION OF THE SONG REPERTOIRE IN BOBOLINKS

Song variation in a population of Alder Flycatchers

SONG STRUCTURE, BEHAVIOUR, AND SEQUENCE OF SONG TYPES IN A POPULATION OF VILLAGE INDIGOBIRDS, VIDUA CHALYBEATA

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

In addition, you will receive a separate Excel file with the latest version of the ebird Taxonomy, ML s official reference for bird names.

What exactly is local song in a population of ortolan buntings with a common dialect?

Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees

Song-type sharing and matching in a bird with very large song repertoires, the tropical mockingbird

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Dominance and geographic information contained within black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) song

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

HIGH LEVEL OF SONG SHARING IN AN EASTERN POPULATION OF SONG SPARROW (MELOSPIZA MELODIA)

Monday 15 May 2017 Afternoon Time allowed: 1 hour 30 minutes


Soft song in song sparrows: response of males and females to an enigmatic signal

in the Howard County Public School System and Rocketship Education

WINTER WREN SINGING BEHAVIOR: A PINNACLE OF SONG COMPLEXITY

Running head: FACIAL SYMMETRY AND PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS 1

THE CONTROL OF SINGING IN VARIED THRUSHES CARL LINN WHITNEY. B.S., Iowa State University, 1970 M.SCi, The University of British Columbia, 1973

STAT 113: Statistics and Society Ellen Gundlach, Purdue University. (Chapters refer to Moore and Notz, Statistics: Concepts and Controversies, 8e)

SONG CHARACTERISTICS AND VARIATION IN A POPULATION OF BANANAQUITS ON PUERTO RICO

Black-capped chickadee dawn choruses are interactive communication networks

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

2. Measurements of the sound levels of CMs as well as those of the programs

Behavioral and neural identification of birdsong under several masking conditions

Olga Feher, PhD Dissertation: Chapter 4 (May 2009) Chapter 4. Cumulative cultural evolution in an isolated colony

The Fox News Eect:Media Bias and Voting S. DellaVigna and E. Kaplan (2007)

IBR Review Papers 1. Handouts Avoiding Plagiarism

EFFECTS OF ORFF-SCHULWERK PROCESS OF IMITATION ON ELEMENTARY STUDENTS READING FLUENCY

ADAPTATION TO DISPLACED AND DELAYED VISUAL FEEDBACK FROM THE HAND 1

INTRA- AND INTERSEXUAL FUNCTIONS OF SINGING BY MALE BLUE GROSBEAKS: THE ROLE OF WITHIN-SONG VARIATION

Confidence Intervals for Radio Ratings Estimators

SYMPHONY OF THE RAINFOREST Part 2: Soundscape Saturation

Technical Appendices to: Is Having More Channels Really Better? A Model of Competition Among Commercial Television Broadcasters

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Spatial cognition: Where are we? Cognitive mapping or associative learning?

Linnet. Extent of postjuvenile moult. II III IV V VI VII VIII IX X XI XII Linnet. Summer. Adult. Female (11-VI)

expressed on operational issues are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the U.S. Census Bureau.

Chapter Two: Long-Term Memory for Timbre

Affective response to a set of new musical stimuli W. Trey Hill & Jack A. Palmer Psychological Reports, 106,

Use italic type instead of underlining words to be italicized.

Automatic Analysis of Musical Lyrics

PETER MARLER. 24 february july 2014 BETHANY DANIELS / COLLEGE OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES / UC DAVIS

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

The psychological impact of Laughter Yoga: Findings from a one- month Laughter Yoga program with a Melbourne Business

COMP Test on Psychology 320 Check on Mastery of Prerequisites

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

AP Statistics Sec 5.1: An Exercise in Sampling: The Corn Field

leucophrys, and certain other species, but no juncos. In January 1959 they

Perceiving Differences and Similarities in Music: Melodic Categorization During the First Years of Life

Validity of TV, Video, Video Game Viewing/Usage Diary: Comparison with the Data Measured by a Viewing State Measurement Device

Estimating repertoire size in a songbird: a comparison of three techniques

RF Safety Surveys At Broadcast Sites: A Basic Guide

Topics in Computer Music Instrument Identification. Ioanna Karydi

1. MORTALITY AT ADVANCED AGES IN SPAIN MARIA DELS ÀNGELS FELIPE CHECA 1 COL LEGI D ACTUARIS DE CATALUNYA

Set-Top-Box Pilot and Market Assessment

Analysis Using the OCLC and RLG Bibliographic Databases

Resampling Statistics. Conventional Statistics. Resampling Statistics

Improving music composition through peer feedback: experiment and preliminary results

Effects of Using Graphic Notations. on Creativity in Composing Music. by Australian Secondary School Students. Myung-sook Auh

Simulation Study of the Spectral Capacity Requirements of Switched Digital Broadcast

Transcription:

Wilson Bull., 108(2), 1996, pp. 335-341 DISCRIMINATION BETWEEN REGIONAL SONG FORMS IN THE NORTHERN PARULA DANIEL J. REGELSKI,* AND RALPH R. MOLDENHAUER ABSTRACT.-DiStinCtly different territorial (Type A) song forms characterize western and eastern populations within the breeding range of the Northern Panda (Parula americana). We conducted playback experiments to determine if territorial males respond differentially to the two song forms. Male response is stronger to Type A songs of their own population than to the songs of the other population (two-tailed Wilcoxon test, P < 0.001). The possible basis for this discrimination is discussed. Received 3 Dec. 1993, accepted 15 Oct. 1995. Many oscine species exhibit geographic song variation (Thielcke 1969), and some of these species songs vary microgeographically, with two or more small scale local dialects (Kroodsma 1981, Tomback et al. 1983, Kroodsma et al. 1984). In species such as the Mourning Warbler (Oporornis philudelphia), there is macrogeographical song variation, with two or more distinct and widespread regional song forms or song populations (Pitocchelli 1990). The Northern Parula (Purula umericunu) has two primary song types, Types A and B (Moldenhauer 1992). Spectrographic analysis of Type A songs from throughout the breeding range by Moldenhauer (1992) revealed an eastern and a western song population (Fig. 1) whose songs are characterized by distinctly different terminal notes (Fig. 2). Playback experiments have shown that in species whose song varies geographically, territorial males can often distinguish between different dialects or song forms, as inferred from the intensity of response to playback (Kroodsma et al. 1984, Ritchison 1985). Usually, the response is stronger to local or familiar song forms. In the present study we conducted playback experiments with eastern and western Northern Parulas to determine if territorial males would respond differentially to eastern and western forms of the Type A song. METHODS Our experimental design and data analysis follow Kroodsma et. al. (1984) and Kroodsma (1989), with certain modifications. We obtained over 100 Northern Parula Type A songs from the Texas Bird Sound Library (TBSL) at Sam Houston State Univ. in Huntsville, Texas, the Cornell Library of Natural Sounds (CLNS) at Cornell Univ., and The Borror Laboratory of Bioacoustics (BLB) at The Ohio State Univ. We used REAL TIME SPECTROGRAM software (by Engineering Design, Belmont, MA) to measure the trill portion of each song for two characteristics: trill duration (TD) I Dept. of Biological Sciences, Sam Houston State Univ., Huntsville, Texas 77341. *Present Address: Dept. of Zoology, The Ohio State Univ., 1735 Neil Ave., Columbus, Ohio 43210. 33s

THE WILSON BULLETIN * Vol. 108, No. 2, June 1996 q WESTERN FORM + PLAYBACK LOCATIONS q EASTERN FORM q RECORDING LOCATIONS PIG. 1. Map of the distributions of the western and eastern song populations within the breeding range of the Northern Panda, as well as the recording locations for the songs used to make the playback tapes and the locations of the playback experiments. and trill rate (TR), the number of trill syllables per second. The trill is the portion of the song preceding the last syllable or terminal note. The eastern and western populations differ significantly in the mean values for these two variables (Moldenhauer 1992). Two values were calculated for each song: TD/mean TD and TR/mean TR, using the mean values for the appropriate song population as reported by Moldenhauer (1992). The songs were arranged into pairs of one eastern and one western song that were matched as closely as possible for the values of TD/mean TD and TR/mean TR, to pair songs that were similar to one another, while taking into account the characteristics of each of the two song populations. Using this method, 12 playback tapes were made, using songs recorded at various

Regelski and Moldenhauer * NORTHERN PARULA SONG 8 WESTERN FORM - 0.5 1.0 1.5 TIME IN SECONDS 8 EASTERN FORM 0 0.5 1.o 1.5 TIME IN SECONDS FIG. 2. Representative western and eastern forms of the Northern Parula Type A song. Notice the difference in the terminal note. These songs were used to make playback tape number 10.

338 THE WILSON BULLETIN l Vol. 108, No. 2, June 1996 dates from throughout the ranges of the two song populations (Fig. 1). Each tape consisted of 25 repetitions of one eastern song spaced 12 set apart on the right channel, and the same treatment of one western song on the left channel. The two channels were staggered by 6 set so that a song was broadcast from alternating speakers every 6 sec. On half the tapes an eastern song was broadcast first, on the other half, a western song was broadcast first. From 9 April to 14 May 1993, playback experiments were conducted with 12 western males near Huntsville in Walker County, Texas. From 19 to 24 May 1993, playback experiments were conducted with 12 eastern males in Liberty County, Florida, as well as Bulloch, Camden, Jenkins and Screven Counties, Georgia (Fig. 1). The same 12 tapes were used for the playbacks to both western and eastern males. This reciprocal design is discussed in Kroodsma (1989). Songs were played on a Uher 4200 tape recorder through a pair of Radio Shack Minimus 0.8 self-amplified speakers placed 18 m apart. The playback level was set by ear to approximate a singing bird. Ribbons were placed every 2 m between the speakers, and the location of the bird during the playback period with respect to the measured 18 m was recorded every 6 s. After the first 5 min. playback period with the western song broadcast from position 0 m and the eastern song from position 18 m, the speaker cables were switched and the tape played a second time. For the western trials, the median position of the bird during the first playback period was subtracted from the median position during the second playback period. For the eastern trials, the median position of the bird during the second playback period was subtracted from the median position during the first playback period. Positive values for difference in median positions (measured in meters) indicate a closer approach to and/or more time spent in proximity to the speaker broadcasting the bird s own song form, while negative values indicate a closer approach and/or more time spent in proximity to the speaker broadcasting the other song form. Comparing two positive values, the larger reflects a closer approach and/or more time spent in proximity to the speaker broadcasting the bird s own song form than the smaller value. Kroodsma (1989) suggests that such differences in median positions can be used as an index of response to the two stimuli. The greater the positive difference in median position, the stronger the response to the bird s own song type. The rationale for this method is presented by Kroodsma et. al. (1984). Therefore, we interpret closer approach to, and more time spent in proximity to a speaker broadcasting a song form, as evidenced by the difference in the bird s median position during two playback periods, as a stronger aggressive response to that song form. Hereafter, we will use the terms stronger response and responded more strongly to reflect this interpretation. Trials where the male did not approach and spend time between the speakers were not included in the analysis. RESULTS Ten of twelve western birds responded more strongly to their own song form, and two responded more strongly to the other song form. The bias in response is significant (two-tailed Wilcoxon test, T, = 8, P < 0.05). Nine of twelve eastern birds responded more strongly to their own song form, and three responded more strongly to the other song form. The bias in response is significant (two-tailed Wilcoxon test, T, = 12, P < 0.025). To justify pooling both data sets, we compared the means of the differences in median position, a measure of the average strength of response for all 12 subjects to their own song type. These values, 4.48 m for western birds and 3.78 m for eastern birds, are not statistically different

Regelski and Moldenhauer l NORTHERN PARULA SONG 339 OTHER (-) OWN (+) DIFFERENCE IN MEDIAN POSITIONS (m) FIG. 3. Summary of all the playbacks to Northern Pandas. Each cell represents a playback session to a different male. The letter in each cell indicates whether the playback was to a (W)estern or (E)astern bird, and the number is the playback tape used. The playbacks were conducted in Texas, Florida, and Georgia. The abscissa is the difference between the median positions during the first and second playback periods. Positive values reflect a stronger response to the subject s own song form, a negative value reflects a stronger response to the other song form, and zero (0) indicates no preference. Note: for this figure, the median differences were rounded to the nearest whole number. (two-tailed Mann-Whitney U-test P > 0.20). Applying Bonferroni s Inequality (Lehman 1991), the (Y for the test of the pooled data cannot exceed 0.049. Pooling the data gives the following results: 19 out of 24 birds responded more strongly to their own song form, and five out of 24 responded more strongly to the alternate song form (Fig. 3). The bias in response is significant (two-tailed Wilcoxon test, T, = 35, P < 0.001). These results suggest that the birds respond more strongly to their own song form. DISCUSSION Territorial male Northern Parulas respond to both regional forms of the Type A song but seem to be able to discriminate between the two, as

340 THE WILSON BULLETIN. Vol. 108, No. 2, June 1996 evidenced by stronger response (closer approach, more time spent in proximity) to playback of their own song form. This resembles the pattern of response that has been observed in the Chiffchaff (Phylloscopus collyb&z), a species with macrogeographic song variation comparable to that of the Northern Panda (Thielcke and Linsenmair 1963). In Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia), the strength of response to playback of various songs increases with similarity to the subject s own song (McArthur 1986). There is also evidence that the learned association of song with aggressive behavior or territorial disputes (Payne 1986, Richards 1979) or visual stimuli (Crook 1984, Murray and Gill 1976) is involved in song form discrimination. The two Type A song forms of the Northern Parula are relatively similar (Fig. 2) and the song probably is partially learned (Kroodsma and Baylis 1982). It is unlikely that any of the subjects had been exposed to the other song form on the breeding or wintering grounds, as the playbacks were conducted in areas of allopatry, and preliminary evidence suggests that the two song populations may have separate wintering grounds (Moldenhauer, unpubl. data). Thus, the song discrimination reported in this paper may be a result of (1) stronger response to the more structurally similar song form, (2) association of the bird s own song form with territorial disputes or aggressive interactions, (3) association of the bird s own song form with visual stimuli, such as conspecific plumage, or (4) some combination of the above. Determining how the song discrimination reported here affects gene flow in this species will require further study, especially in the area of female choice between song forms. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Bill and Martha Lovejoy for their hospitality; Andrew Dewees and Cecil Hallum for statistical assistance; Monte Thies for help with the figures; Donald Kroodsma, Frank B. Gill, Robert B. Payne, J. B. Dunning, Richard Bradley, and Tom C. Grubb, Jr. s reading group and an anonymous reviewer for their comments, Frederick Weinzierl for making his property available for playback experiments, and the staff of the A. J. Brown (Parker Creek) Wastewater Treatment Plant for allowing access to Northern Parula habitat. LITERATURE CITED CROOK, J. R. 1984. Song variation and species discrimination in Blue-winged Warblers. Wilson Bull. 96:91-99. KROODSMA, D. E. 1981. Geographical variation and functions of song types in warblers (Parulidae). Auk 98:743-751. -. 1989. Suggested experimental designs for song playbacks. Animal Behavior 37: 600-609. - AND J. R. BAYLIS. 1982. Appendix: a world survey of evidence for vocal learning in birds. Pp. 311-337 in Acoustic communication in birds, Vol. 2. (D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, eds.). Academic Press, New York, New York.

Regelski and Moldenhauer * NORTHERN PARULA SONG 341 -, W. R. MESERVEY, A. L. WHITLOCK, AND W. M. VANDERHAEGEN. 1984. Blue-winged Warblers (Vermivora pinus) recognize dialects in type II but not type I songs. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 15:127-131. LEHMAN, R. S. 1991. Statistics and research design in the behavioral sciences. Wadsworth Publishing, Belmont California. MCARTHUR, l? D. 1986. Similarity of playback songs to self song as a determinant of response strength in song sparrows (Melospiza melodia). Anim. Behav. 34: 199-207. MOLDENHAUER, R. R. 1992. Two song populations of the Northern Parula. Auk 109:215-222. MURRAY, B. G. AND E B. GILL. 1976. Behavioral interactions of Blue-winged and Goldenwinged warblers. Wilson Bull. 88:231-254. PAYNE, R. B. 1986. Bird songs and avian systematics. Current ornithology 3:87-126. PITOCCHELLI, J. 1990. Plumage, morphometric, and song variation in Mourning (Oporornis philadelphiu) and MacGillivray s (0. tolmei) warblers. Auk 107:161-171. RICHARDS, D. G. 1979. Recognition of neighbors by associative learning in Rufous-sided Towhees. Auk 96:688-693. RITCHISON, G. 1985. Responses of neighboring conspecifics to typical and atypical songs of a Rufous-sided Towhee. J. Field Ornith. 56:280-282. THIELCKE, G. A. 1969. Geographic variation in bird vocalizations. Pp. 311-339 in Bird vocalizations (R. A. Hinde, ed.). Cambridge University Press, New York, New York. -AND K. LINSENMAIR. 1963. Zur geographischen Variation des Gesanges des Zilpzalps, Phylloscopus collybita, in Mittel- und Stidwesteuropa mit einem Vergleich des Gesanges des Fitis, Phylloscopus trochilus. J. Ornith. 104:372-402. TOMBACK, D. E, D. B. THOMPSON, AND M. C. BAKER. 1983. Dialect discrimination by Whitecrowned Sparrows: reactions to near and distant dialects. Auk 100:452-460.