FREQUENCY VARIATION IN SONGS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES (PARUS ATRICAPILLUS)

Similar documents
Pitch shifts and song structure indicate male quality in the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees

Different Responses to Different Song Types in American Redstarts

Repertoire matching between neighbouring song sparrows

A test for repertoire matching in eastern song sparrows

WHY DO VEERIES (CATHARUS FUSCESCENS) SING AT DUSK? COMPARING ACOUSTIC COMPETITION DURING TWO PEAKS IN VOCAL ACTIVITY

Black-capped chickadee dawn choruses are interactive communication networks

Dominance and geographic information contained within black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) song

Red-winged blackbirds Ageliaus phoeniceus respond differently to song types with different performance levels

Ranging of songs with the song type on use of different cues in Carolina wrens: effects of familiarity

I. INTRODUCTION. University of California at Davis, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA Electronic mail:

Dawn song of male blue tits as a predictor of competitiveness in midmorning singing interactions

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

TESTS OF THE FUNCTION OF THE SONG REPERTOIRE IN BOBOLINKS

SONG STRUCTURE, BEHAVIOUR, AND SEQUENCE OF SONG TYPES IN A POPULATION OF VILLAGE INDIGOBIRDS, VIDUA CHALYBEATA

DISCRIMINATION BETWEEN REGIONAL SONG FORMS IN THE NORTHERN PARULA

Preliminary Study in whether land management affects dialect formation in Bobolinks

Responses of male Red-eyed Vireos (Vireo olivaceus) to song playback varying in rate and cadence. H. Lynn Bradman. University of Nebraska

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

A Real Word Case Study E- Trap by Bag End Ovasen Studios, New York City

Olga Feher, PhD Dissertation: Chapter 4 (May 2009) Chapter 4. Cumulative cultural evolution in an isolated colony

Loudness and Pitch of Kunqu Opera 1 Li Dong, Johan Sundberg and Jiangping Kong Abstract Equivalent sound level (Leq), sound pressure level (SPL) and f

PATTERNS OF SINGING IN A POPULATION OF THE PLAIN TITMOUSE

Song-type sharing and matching in a bird with very large song repertoires, the tropical mockingbird

Texas Music Education Research

SINGING ORGANIZATION DURING AGGRESSIVE INTERACTIONS AMONG MALE YELLOW-RUMPED CACIQUES

Author's personal copy

SOUND LABORATORY LING123: SOUND AND COMMUNICATION

A Real Word Case Study E- Trap by Bag End Ovasen Studios, New York City

CHANGES WITH TIME IN THE SONGS OF A POPULATION OF CHAFFINCHES

Song Types, repertories and son variability in a population of Chestnut-Sided Warblers

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

song, and the phrase, with the highest frequency); 4, lower part of

ANALYSING DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE INPUT IMPEDANCES OF FIVE CLARINETS OF DIFFERENT MAKES

COMP Test on Psychology 320 Check on Mastery of Prerequisites

Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. I: The chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus)

Versatility from a Single Song: The Case of the Nightingale Wren

ADELAIDE'S WARBLER (DENDROICA ADELAIDAE)

A comparative approach to vocal learning: intraspecific variation in the learning process

Piotr KLECZKOWSKI, Magdalena PLEWA, Grzegorz PYDA

Reconstruction of Ca 2+ dynamics from low frame rate Ca 2+ imaging data CS229 final project. Submitted by: Limor Bursztyn

Vocal-tract Influence in Trombone Performance

Kent Academic Repository

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

Music Perception with Combined Stimulation

Chapter 24. Meeting 24, Dithering and Mastering

Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Song duration mediates responses of territory owner in a songbird species with a small song repertoire

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

Female Cowbird Song Perception: Evidence for Different Developmental Programs within the same Subspecies

Dial A440 for absolute pitch: Absolute pitch memory by non-absolute pitch possessors

Spatial cognition: Where are we? Cognitive mapping or associative learning?

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Soft song in song sparrows: response of males and females to an enigmatic signal

Subjective evaluation of common singing skills using the rank ordering method

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Formant frequency tuning in singing

Behavioral and neural identification of birdsong under several masking conditions

Sexual Selection I. A broad overview

LONG-TERM PERSISTENCE OF SONG DIALECTS IN THE MOUNTAIN WHITE-CROWNED SPARROW

AUD 6306 Speech Science

Convention Paper Presented at the 139th Convention 2015 October 29 November 1 New York, USA

MINIDISC RECORDERS VERSUS AUDIOCASSETTE RECORDERS: A PERFORMANCE COMPARISON

LeCroy Digital Oscilloscopes

Sexual Selection I. A broad overview

A Parametric Autoregressive Model for the Extraction of Electric Network Frequency Fluctuations in Audio Forensic Authentication

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

The Lecture Contains: Frequency Response of the Human Visual System: Temporal Vision: Consequences of persistence of vision: Objectives_template

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

INDIVIDUALITY IN SCOPS OWL OTUS SCOPS VOCALISATIONS

Pitch-Synchronous Spectrogram: Principles and Applications

Timbre blending of wind instruments: acoustics and perception

INTRA- AND INTERSEXUAL FUNCTIONS OF SINGING BY MALE BLUE GROSBEAKS: THE ROLE OF WITHIN-SONG VARIATION

Course Description: Required Texts:

SONG REPERTOIRES AND SONG SHARING BY AMERICAN REDSTARTS

} ihzeucan Juseum. Warblers (Vermivora pinus) Spectrographic Analysis of Variation in. the Songs of a Population of Blue-winged

homework solutions for: Homework #4: Signal-to-Noise Ratio Estimation submitted to: Dr. Joseph Picone ECE 8993 Fundamentals of Speech Recognition

SUBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF THE BEIJING NATIONAL GRAND THEATRE OF CHINA

Changes in fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus) song over a forty-four year period in New England waters

A few white papers on various. Digital Signal Processing algorithms. used in the DAC501 / DAC502 units

Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

Estimating repertoire size in a songbird: a comparison of three techniques

Assessing and Measuring VCR Playback Image Quality, Part 1. Leo Backman/DigiOmmel & Co.

SONG CHARACTERISTICS AND VARIATION IN A POPULATION OF BANANAQUITS ON PUERTO RICO

Differential effects of moving versus stationary territorial intruders on territory defence in a songbird

Experimental evidence that distinct song phrases in the Grey-cheeked Fulvetta Alcippe morrisonia permit species and local dialect recognition

PETER MARLER. 24 february july 2014 BETHANY DANIELS / COLLEGE OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES / UC DAVIS

Neighbor-stranger discrimination by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher, Empidonax alnorum

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

THE CONTROL OF SINGING IN VARIED THRUSHES CARL LINN WHITNEY. B.S., Iowa State University, 1970 M.SCi, The University of British Columbia, 1973

Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Fan Noise

USER S GUIDE DSR-1 DE-ESSER. Plug-in for Mackie Digital Mixers

A Parametric Autoregressive Model for the Extraction of Electric Network Frequency Fluctuations in Audio Forensic Authentication

Transcription:

The Auk 109(4):847-852, 1992 FREQUENCY VARIATION IN SONGS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES (PARUS ATRICAPILLUS) A. G. HORN, M. L. LEONARD, L. RATCLIFFE, S. A. SHACKLETON, AND R. G. WEISMAN 2 Departraent of Biology, and 2Departraent of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6, Canada ABSTRACT.--Recordings of dawn singing by male Black-capped Chickadees (Parus atricapillus) show that each individual sings its fee-bee song at a wide range of frequencies. Males tend to repeat songs at a given frequency but on average after every 41 + SE of 8.8 songs a male shifts the frequency of its song by a statistically significant amount (>-80 Hz). During any given morning, males may appear to shift among a limited set of discrete frequencies, but over longer time periods intermediate frequencies also are sung. These results suggest that chickadees can vary the frequency of their song more or less continuously over the species' frequency range. When songs of one of three widely spaced frequencies (recorded in previous years) were played back, males replied with songs that had approximately the same frequency as the playback song. Thus, frequency shifting appears, at least in part, to be a form of song matching. These results add to a growing body of evidence that some species with single-song repertoires have evolved effective matching strategies through manipulation of the frequency of their song. Received 14 June 1991, accepted 20 January 1992. THE MAJORITY of songbirds possess multisong repertoires, in which song types can be easily distinguished from each other on the basis of several frequency and temporal features. There has been considerable effort to understand the inter- and intrasexual selective pressures favoring the evolution of multisong repertoires (e.g. Kroodsma and Miller 1982, Searcy and Andersson 1986). In contrast, the conditions that predispose species to maintain single-song repertoires have received comparatively little attention. By single-song repertoires, we mean those in which songs may vary in a graded fashion (e.g. longer or shorter), but cannot be divided into groups by discontinuities in any single parameter or combination of parameters. Species with single-song repertoires may show little variation in song structure because of some advantage to stereotypy, for example, because low variation within and between males in a given area aids the recognition of dialects (e.g. Baker and Cunningham 1985). Alternatively, relatively subtle variations in song structure may serve functions that, in a repertoire species, would be served by abrupt changes in song type. For example, variation in the frequency of the songs of Kentucky Warblers (Opornis formosus; Morton and Young 1986) and Harris' Sparrows (Zonotrichia querula; Shackleton et al. 1991) allow males to match the frequency of each other's songs as a signal in ter- ritorial contests. In species with song types, this function is served by males matching each other's song types. We examined the nature and significance of frequency variation in Black-capped Chickadees (Parus atricapillus), a species typically clas- sified as having a single-song repertoire (Hailman 1989). The vocal repertoire of this species is one of the best known of any bird, especially its "chickadee" (Hailman and Ficken 1986, Nowicki 1989) and "gargle"(ficken and Weise 1987) calls. In contrast to these complex calls, chickadee song is remarkably simple, consisting of a two-note whistle (fee bee). The form of this song is relatively invariant; the lengths of the component notes are relatively constant, and the frequency of the first note is higher than that of the second note by a constant ratio (Ficken et al. 1978, Weisman et al. 1990). Males repeat their song at a given frequency with a high degree of stereotypy. However, males sometimes lower or raise the frequency of their song by at least 100 Hz; these are described as "shifted" songs (Ratcliffe and Weisman 1985, Hill and Lein 1987). Chickadee sing little during the day, so previous studies have sampled only one to three "normal" and "shifted" songs from each male. In work preliminary to our study, we observed chickadee singing the fee-bee song at more than two frequencies. We investigated whether 847

848 HORN ET AL. [Auk, Vol. 109 chickadee males sing: (a) just one song type, but potentially at any frequency within a given range (as in Kentucky Warblers; Morton and Young 1986); or (b) a fixed repertoire of song types, which in this case would be songs of essentially the same form at different frequencies. Chickadees, like many passetines, sing persistently at dawn, which enabled us to obtain a large number of songs from each male for analysis. We also conducted a song playback experiment to test whether the pattern of frequency variation we observed might be a consequence of song matching. METHODS standard tone from a pitch pipe was sounded during about 90% of recording sessions, and indicated no measurable variation in tape speed. The frequencies of the two notes in this species' song ("fee" and "bee") are highly correlated (r = 0.95, Weisman et al. 1990). The frequency of the first note, fee, changes by about 200 Hz from beginning to end, but the second note, bee, usually changes less than 20 Hz (Weisman et al. 1990). Therefore, we report measurements of bee. We used the "power spectrum between cursors" function on a Kay Elemetrics DSP Sonograph (Model 5500) to display the average spectrum of the entire note, and measured the frequency at its peak. This method provided nearly 100% repeatability, even in recordings with high background noise. We used a band width of 29 Hz, a transform size of 1,024 points and a Hamming window. The frequency cursor for this display provided frequency measurements at 20-Hz intervals, Our study was conducted at the Queen's University Biological Station at Lake Opinicon, Ontario, about 50 km north of Kingston. Recordings were made from I April to 30 June 1989, and from 19 April to 8 June 1990. Subjects were color banded at winter feeding stations before the study began. Territories were mapped using song playbacks (Fails 1981) and/or obwell above the resolution of the sonograph (_+0.05%, or 1.7 Hz at 3,400 Hz). Song playback experiment.--playback songs were recorded on the study site in 1987 and 1988 using a Sony WM D6C cassette recorder and a Sennheiser MKH816 microphone. Three frequencies were seservations of territorial interactions. lected for playback: the mode of "shifted" songs (3,133 Recording and analysis of dawn singing.--recordings Hz), the mode of all songs (3,381 Hz), and the mode of spontaneous song were made between 0415 and 0600 EST. The songs of one male were recorded on each morning. Selection of the male recorded on a particular morning was opportunistic, but we did not record males that had heard recorded playback songs within the last 24 h, and we avoided recording neighboring males on successive days if possible. We arrived on the target male's territory before he began singing and tried to record his entire dawn perforof "normal" songs (3,628 Hz; Weisman et al. 1990). To ensure that song frequency effects would not be due to the peculiarities of particular tapes or individuals, two songs from different individuals were used to represent each song frequency. Also, each of these songs was made by joining the fee of one individual with the bee of another. Small sample sizes prohibited us from testing whether different tapes within a given treatment elicited different responses. We chose song mance. Identification of males as soon as there was notes that kept the frequency ratio of fee to bee conenough light always indicated that they were singing stant, at approximately the species' mean (1.13), and well within their own territories. the temporal separation of fee and bee constant at 1 I0 In 1989, we analyzed recordings of only one dawn performance from each of 15 males. Unfortunately, males changed perches, and it took us some time to relocate them, so we missed many songs. In 1990, with the help of a field assistant, we recorded complete performances on two different days from each of eight males. Sample sizes vary among analyses because recordings from 1989 were incomplete for some analyses. Most of the analyses presented below are based on the eight males recorded in 1990. We knew the ms. Playback songs were constructed at a work station consisting of a Macintosh SE computer and SoundCap, SoundWave, and MacSpeech Lab software, using 8-bit A/D and D/A conversion. We standardized the amplitudes of the playback tapes at 80 db with an SPL meter (slow setting). This experiment was performed between 1 and 30 April 1989, between 0700 and 1700 (usually between I000 and 1200). Each male received one playback trial. Order of presentation of the playback songs was ranbreeding status of seven of these males; none was domized in blocks of six birds. The male to be tested nesting or excavating nestholes on the first recording date, but by the second recording date four had mates laying eggs. Except where noted each datum represents one dawn performance by one male. Means and standard errors (SE) are reported. Males were recorded using a Sony WM D6 cassette recorder and a Sennheiser MKH816 microphone, or a Panasonic RQ-330 cassette recorder and a Realistic microphone mounted in a Sony PBR330 parabola, at distances ranging from 3 to 50 m (median 10 m). A on a given day was selected randomly, and neighboring males were not tested on the same day. One trial consisted of a l-min playback of one song that was repeated at a rate of 10 songs per min (the mean song rate in our dawn sample). Songs were broadcast from a Sony WM D6C cassette recorder and a Sony SRS-35 speaker. The speaker was placed 2 m above the ground roughly in the center of the subject's territory, and the observer was stationed about I0 to 15 m from the speaker. Responses were recorded with a

October 1992] Frequency Variation in Chickadee Songs 849 TABLE 1. Mean frequencies (+SE) of songs sung in response to playback of three different frequencies. Results of one-sample comparisons of playback with response frequencies also shown. Frequency (Hz) One-sample t-test Playback Response n t P 3,130 3,310 + 64 7 2.80 0.03 3,380 3,410 + 54 7 0.56 0.59 3,630 3,580 _+ 35 10-1.77 0.11 e?oo / A 0 100 200 300 0 100 200 300 400 Sony ECM-155 microphone and WM D6C cassette recorder. Trials were excluded from analysis if the subject did not sing within 30 s of the end of playback, or if a male from a neighboring territory sang first. This yielded a total sample of 24 usable trials (Table 1). We measured the bee of the first song elicited by playback, using the same methods as those described above for naturally occurring songs. Again, we measured the playback song immediately preceding the response song, and used its deviation from the original playback tape to correct the frequencies of response songs for variations in the tape speed in the playback or the recordings of responses. RESULTS Dawn singing.--malespent approximately the first third of their dawn chorus near the center of their territory, and then sang at scattered perches throughout the territory. Because their neighbors were usually singing at the same time, the males we were recording rarely sang solo for more than 20 songs at the beginning or 50 songs at the end of their dawn performance. Figure 1 shows the frequencies of songs sung by four males, comprising their entire dawn performances for a single morning in 1990. These four birds sang the longest dawn choruses of the eight males recorded in 1990. The average length of dawn chorus for all eight males was 284 _+ 59 songs. Individuals usually repeated songs at close to the same frequency before switching to another frequency (Fig. 1). Occasionally, individuals shifted frequency a large amount in small successive steps (e.g. Fig. 1D), but this was uncommon. To quantify the tendency to shift song frequency, we analyzed the distribution of all frequency shifts between successive songs on a particular morning. For each male recorded in 1989, the shift in frequency between successive 0 100 20o 3oo 4oo 5o0 60o 3?00 / D,..._-...., -. c--. ' ".. 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 Number of Songs Fig. 1. Frequencies of all songs sung at dawn by four males. Males recorded on separate mornings, and each panel represents one male. Songs arranged along X-axis in order in which they were sung. songs was less than 80 Hz in 95% of the instances. Figure 2 shows the results of a similar analysis performed on frequency shifts pooled across the eight males recorded in 1990; in this sample, 94.7% of shifts were less than 80 Hz. Using the common practice of regarding the 95% level as indicating a difference unlikely to have arisen by chance (i.e. statistically significant), we focused our analyses on shifts of 80 Hz or more. For the eight males recorded in 1990, the mean number of songs between shifts of 80 Hz or more was 41 + 8.8. It is important to ask whether, when males sing, they shift among a limited set of discrete frequencies (analogous to frequencies playable on a bugle), or among an unlimited set of frequencies (analogous to frequencies playable on a slide trombone). That is, can the mean frequencies of each series of repeated songs occurring after one shift and before the next (e.g. the first 50 songs in Fig. 1A) be sorted into a limited set of significantly different means? To evaluate this query, we treated the first five song

850 HORN ET AL. [Auk, Vol. 109 1200' 1000 600' 400 high, medium, and low playback frequencies, so we assume that males in the playback study could do likewise. A regression analysis showed that the frequency of songs sung by males in response to playback was significantly and positively related to the frequency of the stimulus song (F1.22 = 19.68, P = 0.002), although responses to playback of the low stimulus were significantly higher in frequency than the playback frequency (Table 1). 200 DISCUSSION Frequency Shift (I-Iz) Fig. 2. Distribution of frequency shifts (Hz) in dawn choruses of eight males (n = 2,262 songs) recorded in 1990. measurements after each shift of 80 Hz or more in an individual's morning performance as a separate sample in a Scheffe's multiple-comparisons test, which tests whether means can be sorted into significantly heterogeneous groups (Sokal and Rohlf 1981). Because males tended to repeat each frequency, successive songs are not statistically independent points. However, recall that every tenth song was measured (Methods), which would reduce the effects of such sequential dependence. Also, any effect of nonindependence would bias our resuits against grouping the means together (i.e. towards finding discrete frequencies). Overall, the songs of a male on a given morn- ing did not sort into a few discrete frequencies (i.e. song types). In 7 of 16 dawn choruses, Scheffe's tests revealed four or fewer discrete groups of means (P < 0.05). However, there were significantly fewer shifts of 80 Hz or more in those choruses than in the nine remaining choruses (Mann-Whitney U; day 1, U = 16, n = 8, P = 0.002; day 2, U = 14, n = 7, P = 0.05). That is, the gaps between frequencies fill in the more a bird shifts frequency. For example, recording 90 songs from the male depicted in Figure 1D would make one think he sang only three discrete frequencies, but after recording over 500 songs, one finds he sings most frequencies between 2,960 and 3,500 Hz. Song playback experiment.--all males in the above analysesang songs within 120 Hz of the Species are said to posses song types if discontinuous variation in frequency or temporal characters allows their songs to be divided into discrete types (e.g. McGregor and Krebs 1982, Horn and Fails 1988). In this limited sense, some Black-capped Chickadees, at least within the course of one morning, may be said to possess a repertoire of song types. However, Blackcapped Chickadees differ from species with multisong repertoires in that, on different days and across the population, frequency variation appears to be continuous. Thus, we conclude that variation in Black-capped Chickadee song more nearly approximates that of species with a single fixed song type than a multisong repertoire. Our results add to a growing number of studies (Morton and Young 1986, Shackleton et al. 1991) that suggest there is not a sharp distinction between species with a single song type and those with multisong repertoires. The case of Black-capped Chickadees is especially interesting, because this species displays at least two features once thought to be peculiar to species with song repertoires: (1) repetitive delivery of stereotyped songs punctuated by sudden shifts in song parameters; and (2) a form of song matching (discussed below). The results of our playback experiment suggest that song matching plays an important role in determining what frequencies chickadees sing. In addition, field observation suggesthat neighboring males match each other much of the time (Horn unpubl. data). This evidence suggests that the number of frequencies sung by each male could be determined by the number of neighbors he has and the frequency of interactions he has with each of them. Repetitions at a given frequency might reflect an ongoing vocal interaction with one neighbor, with shifts occurring when an interaction with an-

October 1992] Frequency Variation in Chickadee Songs 851 other neighbor is started. The quality of those interactions among males might also play a role. One way in which this might happen is sugrelatively few examples of continuous variation in species with single-song repertoires (e.g. Morton and Young 1986, Shackleton et al. 1991). gested by the lower rate of matching to our Our results confirm the common idea that lowest-frequency playback song. If songs at lower frequencies convey a stronger territorial message than songs at higher frequencies, as chickadees have a single song type, and at the same time suggest frequency variation has an analogous function to multiple song types. suggested by Hill and Lein (1987), then males might be more reluctant to match a lower song. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS However, there may also be individual dif- L. James, D. Syme, and I. Johnsrude provided inferences in singing behavior. Although the pre- valuable assistance in the field. M. Ficken, J. Hailman, cise frequencies sung by each male may vary S. Nowicki, K. Teather and D. Weary provided helpful from one day to another, the total range of fre- comments on the manuscript. A.G.H. was supported quencies sung appears to be consistent within by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Natural Scimales (Spearman rank correlation of day 1 val- ences and Engineering Research Council of Canada ues with day 2 values by male: rs [corrected for ties] = 0.81, n = 8, P < 0.05). Also, the number (NSERC). Financial support for this project was provided by NSERC grants awarded to L.R and R.G.W., of significantly different subsets of frequencies, by a Queen's University Advisory Research Commitas identified by Scheffe's tests, is consistent tee grant to L.R., and by the A. B. Kelly Memorial within males (rs = 0.87, n = 8, P -< 0.05). If Fund (Province of Quebec Society for the Protection of Birds) to S.A.S. singing patterns among neighbors are relatively constant, then matching might account for this consistency. Individual differences in song structure other than repertoire size have received little attention, so this possibility deserves further study. Differences in song struc- LITERATURE CITED BAKER, M. C., AND M. A. CUNNINGHAM. 1985. The biology of bird-song dialects. Behav. Brain Sci. 8:85-100. CUTHILL, I. C., AND W. A. McDONALD. 1990. Experture in the related Great Tit (Parus major) have imental manipulation of the dawn and dusk chobeen related to various measures of male quality rus in the Blackbird Turdus merula. Behav. Ecol. (Lambrechts and Dhondt 1986). Sociobiol. 26:209-216. Studies of the dawn chorus in other species FALLS, J.B. 1981. Mapping territories with playback: suggesthat dawn singing is at least partly di- An accurate census method for songbirds. Stud. rected to the singer's mate (Mace 1987, Cuthill Avian Biol. 6:86-91. and McDonald 1990). The different frequencies sung by Black-capped Chickadees, therefore, might be used in intersexual communication, even though the behaviors of song matching FICKEN, M. S. 1981. What is the song of the Blackcapped Chickadee? Condor 83:384-386. FICKEN, M. S., FICatEN, R. W., AND S. R. WITKIN. 1978. Vocal repertoire of the Black-capped Chickadee. Auk 95:34-48. and of patrolling territory boundaries during FICKEN, M. S., AND C. M. WEISE. 1987. A complex the chorus in this species suggest an intrasexual vocalization of the Black-capped Chickadee. II. function as well. Recordings of complete dawn Repertoires, dominance and dialects. Condor 89: performances from different nesting stages 500-509. would be valuable; changes in the length of the HAILMAN, J.P. 1989. The organization of major vodawn chorus in Great Tits in relation to laying calizations in the Paridae: A study in the evodate have been attributed to an increase in the lution of communication. Wilson Bull. 101:305- risk of cuckoldry (Mace 1987). We found little difference in singing patterns between prelaying and laying stages, but our sample size was small. The song of the Black-capped Chickadee was thought to be one of the simplest among songbirds (e.g. Ficken 1981, Hailman 1989). Although the structure of individual songs is simple, we have now shown that each male sings several frequencies and can match the frequencies of playback songs. To date there have been 343. HAILMAN, J.P., AND M. S. FICKEN. 1986. Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as 'language' by structural linguistics. Anim. Behav. 34:1899-1901. HILL, B. G., AND M. R. LEIN. 1987. Function of frequency-shifted songs of Black-capped Chickadees. Condor 89:914-915. HORN, A., AND J. B. FALLS. 1988. Structure of Western Meadowlark song repertoires. Can. J. Zool. 66: 284-288.

852 HORN ET AL. [Auk, Vol. 109 KROODSMA, D. E., AND E. H. MILLER. 1982. Acoustic communication in birds, vols. I and II. Academic Press, New York. LAMBRECHTS, M., AND A. A. DHONDT. 1986. Male quality, reproduction, and survival in the Great Tit (Parus major). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19:57-63. MACE, R. 1987. The dawn chorus in the Great Tit Parus major is directly related to female fertility. Nature (Lond.) 330:745-746. MCGREGOR, P. K., AND J. R. KREBS. 1982. Song types in a population of Great Tits (Parus major): Their distribution, abundance and acquisition by individuals. Behaviour 79:126-152. MORTON, E. $., AND K. YOUNG. 1986. A previously undescribed method of song matching in a species with a single song "type," the Kentucky Warbler (Opornis formosus). Ethology 73:334-342. NOWlCKI, S. 1989. Vocal plasticity in captive Black- capped Chickadees: The acoustic basis and rate of call convergence. Anim. Behav. 37:64-73. RATCLIFFE, L., AND R. G. WEISMAN. 1985. Frequency shift in the fee bee song of the Black-capped Chickadee. Condor 87:555-556. SEARCY, W. A., AND g. ANDERSSON. 1986. Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 17:507-533. SHACKLETON, S. A., L. RATCLIFFE, A. G. HORN, AND C. T. NAUGLER. 1991. Song repertoires of Harris' Sparrows (Zonotrichia querula). Can. J. Zool. 69: 1867-1874. SOKAn, R. R., AND F. J. ROHLF. 1981. Biometry, 2nd ed. W. H. Freeman and Co., New York. WEISMAN, R., L. RATCLIFFE, L JOHNSRUDE, AND T. A. HURLEY. 1990. Absolute and relative pitch production in the song of the Black-capped Chickadee. Condor 92:118-124.