Perceptual differences between cellos PERCEPTUAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CELLOS: A SUBJECTIVE/OBJECTIVE STUDY

Similar documents
A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

STUDY OF THE PERCEIVED QUALITY OF SAXOPHONE REEDS BY A PANEL OF MUSICIANS

The Brassiness Potential of Chromatic Instruments

Perceptual and physical evaluation of differences among a large panel of loudspeakers

Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs

A study of the influence of room acoustics on piano performance

Effect of task constraints on the perceptual. evaluation of violins

Experimental Study of Attack Transients in Flute-like Instruments

ANALYSING DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE INPUT IMPEDANCES OF FIVE CLARINETS OF DIFFERENT MAKES

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

STUDY OF VIOLIN BOW QUALITY

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

Shock waves in trombones A. Hirschberg Eindhoven University of Technology, W&S, P.O. Box 513, 5600 MB Eindhoven, The Netherlands

NOVEL DESIGNER PLASTIC TRUMPET BELLS FOR BRASS INSTRUMENTS: EXPERIMENTAL COMPARISONS

Timbre blending of wind instruments: acoustics and perception

However, in studies of expressive timing, the aim is to investigate production rather than perception of timing, that is, independently of the listene

Transient behaviour in the motion of the brass player s lips

Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Shock waves in trombones

An acoustic and perceptual evaluation of saxophone pad resonators

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Sound Quality Analysis of Electric Parking Brake

Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures


On perceptual evaluation of musical instruments: The case of the violin put into perspective

Noise. CHEM 411L Instrumental Analysis Laboratory Revision 2.0

A Need for Universal Audio Terminologies and Improved Knowledge Transfer to the Consumer

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Analysis, Synthesis, and Perception of Musical Sounds

Perception and Sound Design

Room acoustics computer modelling: Study of the effect of source directivity on auralizations

Regularity and irregularity in wind instruments with toneholes or bells

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

Vocal-tract Influence in Trombone Performance

Instrument Recognition in Polyphonic Mixtures Using Spectral Envelopes

Perceptual thresholds for detecting modifications applied to the acoustical properties of a violin

INTRODUCTION. I. EXPERIMENTAL SETUPS AND PROCEDURES A. Artificial mouth and buzzing lips

Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

THE VIRTUAL BOEHM FLUTE - A WEB SERVICE THAT PREDICTS MULTIPHONICS, MICROTONES AND ALTERNATIVE FINGERINGS

GCT535- Sound Technology for Multimedia Timbre Analysis. Graduate School of Culture Technology KAIST Juhan Nam

A FUNCTIONAL CLASSIFICATION OF ONE INSTRUMENT S TIMBRES

Mechanical response characterization of saxophone reeds

Real-time Granular Sampling Using the IRCAM Signal Processing Workstation. Cort Lippe IRCAM, 31 rue St-Merri, Paris, 75004, France

ONLINE ACTIVITIES FOR MUSIC INFORMATION AND ACOUSTICS EDUCATION AND PSYCHOACOUSTIC DATA COLLECTION

Animating Timbre - A User Study

EFFECTS OF REVERBERATION TIME AND SOUND SOURCE CHARACTERISTIC TO AUDITORY LOCALIZATION IN AN INDOOR SOUND FIELD. Chiung Yao Chen

Music Representations

Timbral description of musical instruments

Computational Modelling of Harmony

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Music Performance Panel: NICI / MMM Position Statement

Psychophysical quantification of individual differences in timbre perception

Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Fan Noise

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

TOWARDS VIDEO QUALITY METRICS FOR HDTV. Stéphane Péchard, Sylvain Tourancheau, Patrick Le Callet, Mathieu Carnec, Dominique Barba

JOURNAL OF BUILDING ACOUSTICS. Volume 20 Number

TYING SEMANTIC LABELS TO COMPUTATIONAL DESCRIPTORS OF SIMILAR TIMBRES

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

Automatic meter extraction from MIDI files (Extraction automatique de mètres à partir de fichiers MIDI)

USING PULSE REFLECTOMETRY TO COMPARE THE EVOLUTION OF THE CORNET AND THE TRUMPET IN THE 19TH AND 20TH CENTURIES

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Music Representations

Correlating differences in the playing properties of five student model clarinets with physical differences between them

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Understanding PQR, DMOS, and PSNR Measurements

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Robert Alexandru Dobre, Cristian Negrescu

ISOMET. Compensation look-up-table (LUT) and Scan Uniformity

NAA ENHANCING THE QUALITY OF MARKING PROJECT: THE EFFECT OF SAMPLE SIZE ON INCREASED PRECISION IN DETECTING ERRANT MARKING

DERIVING A TIMBRE SPACE FOR THREE TYPES OF COMPLEX TONES VARYING IN SPECTRAL ROLL-OFF

An Overview of ATIAM International Master Program Sorbonne Université Ircam Télécom ParisTech. Journée d Informatique Musicale May 2018

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Skip Length and Inter-Starvation Distance as a Combined Metric to Assess the Quality of Transmitted Video

REBUILDING OF AN ORCHESTRA REHEARSAL ROOM: COMPARISON BETWEEN OBJECTIVE AND PERCEPTIVE MEASUREMENTS FOR ROOM ACOUSTIC PREDICTIONS

Assessing and Measuring VCR Playback Image Quality, Part 1. Leo Backman/DigiOmmel & Co.

Analysis of car door closing sound quality

Timbre Variations as an Attribute of Naturalness in Clarinet Play

MOTIVATION AGENDA MUSIC, EMOTION, AND TIMBRE CHARACTERIZING THE EMOTION OF INDIVIDUAL PIANO AND OTHER MUSICAL INSTRUMENT SOUNDS

OBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF A MELODY EXTRACTOR FOR NORTH INDIAN CLASSICAL VOCAL PERFORMANCES

Hong Kong University of Science and Technology 2 The Information Systems Technology and Design Pillar,

Syllabus: PHYS 1300 Introduction to Musical Acoustics Fall 20XX

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

Colour Reproduction Performance of JPEG and JPEG2000 Codecs

CTP431- Music and Audio Computing Musical Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology KAIST Juhan Nam

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

TOWARDS IMPROVING ONSET DETECTION ACCURACY IN NON- PERCUSSIVE SOUNDS USING MULTIMODAL FUSION

Effects of headphone transfer function scattering on sound perception

Evaluation of the Technical Level of Saxophone Performers by Considering the Evolution of Spectral Parameters of the Sound

DESIGNING OPTIMIZED MICROPHONE BEAMFORMERS

Music Complexity Descriptors. Matt Stabile June 6 th, 2008

Semi-automated extraction of expressive performance information from acoustic recordings of piano music. Andrew Earis

Acoustic enhancement in the Aylesbury theatre with the CARMEN electroacoustic system

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Transcription:

PERCEPTUAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CELLOS: A SUBJECTIVE/OBJECTIVE STUDY Jean-François PETIOT 1), René CAUSSE 2) 1) Institut de Recherche en Communications et Cybernétique de Nantes (UMR CNRS 6597) - 1 rue de la Noë, BP 92101, 44321 Nantes Cedex 3 France, petiot@irccyn.ec-nantes.fr 2) Institut de Recherche et Coordination Acoustique Musique (UMR CNRS 9912) 1 place Igor Stravinsky, 75004 Paris France, Rene.Causse@ircam.fr Abstract This paper addresses the characterisation of cello sounds. In order to study to which extent a jury perceives differences between two cellos in playing conditions, we carried out two blindfolded hearing tests, involving two instruments and two professional musicians: (1) An evaluation test of the couple musician-instrument on a structured scale, for 5 different attributes defined by adjective-pairs. The assessment of the jury was based on the same musical sequences, played by the musicians; (2) A comparative test, based on the ranking of the couple musician-instrument on 4 different attributes. The same short musical fragment was played successively by the musicians. All the played musical sequences have been recorded, and metrics based on the acoustic signal (playing frequency, spectral centroid, signal/noise ratio) were calculated in order to interpret the perceived differences. The results show that for the evaluation test, the inter-subject differences of the jury are too large and do not allow the definition of a significant instrument effect. For the comparative test, the agreement between the subjects is better and significant differences between the instruments and the musicians can be observed, explained by the signal processing of the played sounds. INTRODUCTION The study of the quality of musical instruments is particularly interesting to help their development and to improve their design. In the literature, we distinguish two kinds of studies which address this goal: (1) subjective studies, where the quality is assessed by listeners or players [Pratt & Bowsher, 1978] during evaluation tests; (2) objective studies, where the quality is evaluated by physical measurements on the instruments [Pratt & Bowsher, 1979]. Most of the time, links are proposed between these two studies, in order to explain a posteriori subjective attributes by physical measurements. In order to propose a model for predicting certain qualities of an instrument, the approach consists in discovering correlations, for a set of instruments, between the subjective response (given by the subject) and measurements (made on the signal of the sounds, or directly on the instrument itself) [Plitnik & al., 1999]. This approach has been used for example for the study of loudspeakers [Lavandier, 2005], the study of guitars [Wright, 1996] or trumpets [Poirson et al., 2007] and is classical in room acoustics.

The main difficulty with this approach is to get reliable subjective data. Indeed, the subjective assessments are most of the time dependent on cultural and training aspects of the subject, and subjected to inherent inter-individual differences. This work is in this context. On request of two professional musicians, we wanted to know if what can be called a Stradivarius effect is perceptible by listeners. In other words, our aim was to study in which extend a jury of subjects perceive consensual and/or reliable differences between two high-end cellos. More precisely, the objectives of this work are: To characterise, by the way of hearing tests with a jury, the perceived differences between two high-end cellos To try to explain the differences and to link them to attributes of the acoustical signal In this paper, we present firstly the experimental protocol we designed for the assessments of the instruments. The different tests and the design of experiments are detailed. Secondly, the results of the subjective tests are presented. Finally, the main results of the signal analysis are given, in relation with the conclusions of the subjective tests. Jury and room EXPERIMENTAL PROTOCOL The jury was made of 6 participants, each of them being involved in the musical acoustics or instrument making sector. The jury was blindfolded, all the instrument have been played behind a curtain (figure 1). The tests were carried out in the ESPRO room of the IRCAM, the duration of the session was approximately 2 hours. Instruments and musicians Figure 1 : picture of the assessment session with the jury Two professional musicians, denoted X and Y, played the instruments for the tests. Two instruments (their own instruments) have been considered for the study: Instrument R, French maker, modern, 2005 Instrument C, Italian maker, ancient, Venice, 1680 Assessment tests Given that we wished to asses the instrument effect, the musician effect, and eventually the interaction musician*instrument, we tested all the combinations

musician*instrument, and we introduced repetition in the presentation. The full factorial design is made of 2 2=4 cells (all the possible combinations). It has to be noticed that the musicians used their own bow whatever instrument they played. In collaboration with the two musicians, two listening tests were designed: Test n 1: evaluation test: assessment on a bipolar structured scale, for a set of 6 attributes concerning the sound and the radiation of the instrument Test n 2: comparative test : ranking of the instruments, for a set of 4 attributes Test n 1: evaluation test Six attributes were proposed for the characterisation of the instruments. Concerning the sound, 5 attributes, defined by a bipolar scale: neutral-rich; not resonant resonant; nasal-round; magnitude neutral-rich; timber homogeneity. Concerning the radiation, 1 attribute: narrow-large. In order to test the repeatability of the subjects, 3 repetitions of the configurations were introduced in the presentation. The factorial design proposed to the jury was finally made of 2 2 3=12 configurations. The stimuli of the test n 1 have been proposed by the musicians. It was a musical sequence of around 2 minutes, mixing various style and dynamics, repeated for all the configurations. For the evaluations, the musicians played the stimuli, and the jury assessed progressively the configuration during the listening. The assessment sheet for test N 1 is given figure 2. It is made of 6 structured scales on which the jury had to indicate his/her assessment. LE SON (timbre de l instrument) neutre riche ni neutre Très neutre Assez neutre ni riche Assez riche Très riche Très peu résonnant Epreuve 1 : cotation peu résonant résonant Très résonant Assez peu ni résonant ni Assez résonant peu résonant résonant Très nasal Assez nasal nasal rond ni nasal ni rond Assez rond Très rond Amplitude neutre riche faible Assez faible moyenne Assez grande Très grande Très peu homogène Assez peu homogène Homogénéité du timbre ni / ni Assez homogène LE RAYONNEMENT DU SON étroit large Très homogène ni étroit Très étroit Assez étroit ni large Assez large Très large Figure 2 : assessment sheet for the test n 1

Test n 2: comparative test Four attributes were proposed for the ranking of the instruments: powerfulbright-full-directive. In order to test the repeatability of the subjects, 2 repetitions of the configurations were introduced in the presentation. The factorial design proposed to the jury was finally made of 2 2 2=8 configurations. The stimuli of the test n 2 have been proposed by the musicians. It consists of a short musical fragment of around 5 seconds, specific for each attribute. The two musicians were asked to play successively this fragment, and the jury had to rank the instruments just after, according to the attribute. The assessment sheet for the test n 2 is given figure 3. Epreuve 2 : classement Classer les instruments du plus puissant (1) au moins puissant (2) Instrument A Instrument B Classer les instruments du plus clair (1) au moins clair (2) Instrument A Instrument B Classer les instruments du plus ample (1) au moins ample (2) Instrument A Instrument B Classer les instruments du plus directif (1) au moins directif (2) Instrument A Instrument B Recordings and signal processing Figure 3 : answer sheet for the test n 2 All the stimuli were recorded during the session. Specifically, the stimuli for the test n 1 comprised the open strings of the cello, C2, G2, D3, A3 (figure 4), played at a dynamic mf. Figure 4 : notes of the cello recorded for each configuration, basis for the signal processing

For each configuration (musician*instrument), three repetitions of each note were available. For each note, the playing frequency, the spectral centroid Sc, and the signal-noise ratio R sn (ratio of the total intensity of the signal on the intensity of the nonharmonic part of the signal), were computed. The synchronous detection method was used to calculate the level of each harmonic of the note. Test n 1: assessment tests RESULTS OF THE SUBJECTIVE ASSESSMENTS For all the attributes, the box-plot of the assessments of the two instruments R and C (whatever musician is playing) by the 6 subjects are given figure 5. These raw data don t show a clear instrument effect (the average value of the evaluations, represented by the red cross, is rather similar for all the attributes, except for timber homogeneity, for which the instrument R seems to be more homogeneous than C). The inter-subjects difference and/or the error of repeatability of the subjects seem to be rather high. Box plots of the assessments narrow-large R timber homo. R narrow-large C timber homo.c mag.neutral--rich R mag.neutral--rich C nasal-round R not resonant - resonant R neutral rich R nasal-round C not resonant - resonant C neutral rich C 0 6 Figure 5 : Box-plots of the assessments for the test n 1. A two-way ANOVA with interaction (factors instrument and musician) shows that the only significant effect (p-value<5%) is the instrument effect for the attribute timber homogeneity (p-value = 4%). Table 1 gives the p-values of the ANOVA for all the factors and all the attributes. p-value not resonant resonant magnitude neutral-rich timber homogeneity. neutralrich nasalround narrowlarge instrument effect 0,170 0,479 0,846 1,000 0,040 0,481 musician effect 0,894 0,550 0,928 0,700 0,436 0,467 Interaction musician*instrument 0,756 0,081 0,741 0,785 0,541 0,279 Table 1: p-values of the two-ways ANOVA

This analysis confirms the examination of the raw data: the differences between the instruments are weak and/or the subjects are not in agreement for their evaluations and/or the repeatability of the subjects is weak. In conclusions, after a debriefing with the subjects, the test n 1 seems to be rather difficult for the subjects, they had not a clear idea of what they had to evaluate. Comparative tests For all the attributes, each subject provided a rank of the 2 instruments (they had also the possibility to rank them placed equal). For the instrument R (arbitrarily), we computed the number of times it has been ranked first (1), second (2) or placed equal (1,5) for all the evaluations. Figure 6 shows the results of the ranking by all the subjects for the four attributes. The ranking of instrument L can be directly deducted of this figure 6, by inverting the rank 1 by 2. powerful - instrument R ranking % bright - instrument R ranking % 1 1,5 2 1 1,5 2 full - instrument R ranking % directive - instrument R ranking % 1 1,5 2 1 1,5 2 Figure 6 : ranking % for the test n 2. The raw data (percentage of rank) shows that instrument R seems to be more powerful, more bright, more full and directive than instrument C (the rate of rank 1 is higher than 50%). A Friedman test showed that the following propositions are significant (p-value<0.1%): R is more powerful and directive than C The musician X plays more powerful and directive than Y For the musician X, he played more full with C than with R (C is his own instrument), and more directive with R than with C For the musician Y, he played more powerful and more full with R than with C (R is his own instrument) A debriefing of the test n 2 with the subjects showed that this test was very intuitive and simple. This test n 2 gives more consistent results than the test n 1.

RESULTS OF THE SIGNAL ANALYSIS Globally for the four notes studied, a one-way ANOVA with the factor instrument shows first that the effect of the instrument on the signal-noise ratio R sn and on the spectral centroid Sc is not significant. A study note by note is necessary. Table 2 gives, for each note, the average value of the spectral centroid, and the p-value of the instrument effect of the ANOVA. C2 G2 D3 A3 R C R C R C R C Sc 8,912 8,666 8,534 7,060 6,435 6,062 6,024 7,018 Instrument effect p-value = 3.8% p-value = 0.5% p-value = 6.8% p-value = 0.2 % Table 2: value of the average Spectral centroid Sc and p-values of the ANOVA For the notes C2, G2, D3, the spectral centroid is higher for instrument R than for instrument C For the note A3, the spectral centroid is higher for instrument C Furthermore, the absolute value of the spectral centroid for the note A3 and instrument C is not really consistent with the S c for the notes C2, G2, D3: the string A3 seems to stand out. A debriefing with the musicians and the instrument maker after the session confirmed this fact. The results of this analysis are in accordance with the conclusions of the test n 1: the timber of instrument R is more homogeneous than those of instrument C. No effect of the musician on the spectral centroid or on the signal- noise ratio was significant, neither globally nor note by note: the effect of the instrument is always more important than those of the musician. CONCLUSIONS In this paper, we presented a study of the perceived differences between to highend cellos during instrument playing. Two kinds of tests were carried out with a blindfolded jury of six participants: an evaluation test and a comparative test. For the evaluation test, large differences between subjects and few significant differences between instruments were noticed. The comparative test provided fairly consensual results: it seems possible to evaluate certain attributes of sound quality in this way. The effect of the instrument was always greater than the effect of the musician. In order to explain the perceived differences between the instruments, metrics based on the signal of the open strings were calculated. The value of the spectral centroid of the sounds was consistent with the conclusions of the listening tests. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The authors wish to thank the musicians Etienne CARDOZE and Raphaël PIDOUX, the jury Matthias DEMOUCRON, Nicolas RASAMIMANANA, Pierre CARADOT, Michel ORIANO, Cécile LENOIR and Emilie POIRSON for their substantive input.

REFERENCES Lavandier M. (2005). Différences entre enceintes acoustiques : une evaluation physique et perceptive. Thèse de doctorat de l université d Aix Marseille II, 19 décembre 2005. Plitnik G. R, Lawson B.A. (1999). An investigation of correlations between geometry, acoustic variables, and psychoacoustic parameters for French horn mouthpieces. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106, 1111-1125. Poirson E., Depincé P., Petiot J-F. (2007). User-centered design by genetic algorithms: Application to brass musical instrument optimization. Engineering Applications of Artificial Intelligence, 20 (2007) 511 518. Pratt R.L., Bowsher J.M. (1978) The subjective assessment of trombone quality. Journal of Sound and Vibration 57, 425-435. Pratt R.L., Bowsher J.M. (1979). The objective assessment of trombone quality. Journal of Sound and Vibration 65(4), 521-547. Wright H. The acoustics and psychoacoustics of the guitar (1996). PhD Thesis, University of Wales, Cardiff, (1996).