Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Is the musical retard an allusion to physical motion?

Similar documents
Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. On the anatomy of the retard. A study of timing in music

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Musicians and nonmusicians sensitivity to differences in music performance

v end for the final velocity and tempo value, respectively. A listening experiment was carried out INTRODUCTION

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Replicability and accuracy of pitch patterns in professional singers

About Giovanni De Poli. What is Model. Introduction. di Poli: Methodologies for Expressive Modeling of/for Music Performance

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

6.5 Percussion scalograms and musical rhythm

DELTA MODULATION AND DPCM CODING OF COLOR SIGNALS

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Perceiving temporal regularity in music

On time: the influence of tempo, structure and style on the timing of grace notes in skilled musical performance

Director Musices: The KTH Performance Rules System

The Effect of Time-Domain Interpolation on Response Spectral Calculations. David M. Boore

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

The Final Ritard: On Music, Motion and Kinematic Models i

A prototype system for rule-based expressive modifications of audio recordings

Tempo and Beat Analysis

Human Preferences for Tempo Smoothness

The influence of musical context on tempo rubato. Renee Timmers, Richard Ashley, Peter Desain, Hank Heijink

Machine Learning of Expressive Microtiming in Brazilian and Reggae Drumming Matt Wright (Music) and Edgar Berdahl (EE), CS229, 16 December 2005

AskDrCallahan Calculus 1 Teacher s Guide

Measuring & Modeling Musical Expression

Smooth Rhythms as Probes of Entrainment. Music Perception 10 (1993): ABSTRACT

> f. > œœœœ >œ œ œ œ œ œ œ

Beethoven, Bach, and Billions of Bytes

WHO IS WHO IN THE END? RECOGNIZING PIANISTS BY THEIR FINAL RITARDANDI

Chapter 27. Inferences for Regression. Remembering Regression. An Example: Body Fat and Waist Size. Remembering Regression (cont.)

Igaluk To Scare the Moon with its own Shadow Technical requirements

Automatic characterization of ornamentation from bassoon recordings for expressive synthesis

ECE438 - Laboratory 4: Sampling and Reconstruction of Continuous-Time Signals

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

THE OPERATION OF A CATHODE RAY TUBE

Timing In Expressive Performance

NEXT ION OPTICS SIMULATION VIA ffx

EE373B Project Report Can we predict general public s response by studying published sales data? A Statistical and adaptive approach

Composer Identification of Digital Audio Modeling Content Specific Features Through Markov Models

Music Representations

transcends any direct musical culture. 1 Then there are bands, like would be Reunion from the Live at Blue Note Tokyo recording 2.

On music performance, theories, measurement and diversity 1

Pseudorandom Stimuli Following Stimulus Presentation

University of Tennessee at Chattanooga Steady State and Step Response for Filter Wash Station ENGR 3280L By. Jonathan Cain. (Emily Stark, Jared Baker)

A Case Based Approach to the Generation of Musical Expression

Trends in preference, programming and design of concert halls for symphonic music

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

An Integrated Music Chromaticism Model

Lecture 17 Microwave Tubes: Part I

Experiment on adjustment of piano performance to room acoustics: Analysis of performance coded into MIDI data.

THE OPERATION OF A CATHODE RAY TUBE

Structure and Interpretation of Rhythm and Timing 1

A Beat Tracking System for Audio Signals

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Music communication as studied by means of performance

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen


Simple motion control implementation

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Formant frequency tuning in singing

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using vocabulary and language of music.

Introductions to Music Information Retrieval

On the contextual appropriateness of performance rules

Hidden Markov Model based dance recognition

Overview of Pitch and Time Organization in Stockhausen's Klavierstück N.9

Lab 5 Linear Predictive Coding

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Matching the rule parameters of PHRASE ARCH to performances of Träumerei : a preliminary study

Robert Alexandru Dobre, Cristian Negrescu

PCM-16 Phase Synchronization Controller Operators Manual

Calibrate, Characterize and Emulate Systems Using RFXpress in AWG Series

Effects of Tempo on the Timing of Simple Musical Rhythms

Controlling Musical Tempo from Dance Movement in Real-Time: A Possible Approach

OCTAVE C 3 D 3 E 3 F 3 G 3 A 3 B 3 C 4 D 4 E 4 F 4 G 4 A 4 B 4 C 5 D 5 E 5 F 5 G 5 A 5 B 5. Middle-C A-440

TOWARDS IMPROVING ONSET DETECTION ACCURACY IN NON- PERCUSSIVE SOUNDS USING MULTIMODAL FUSION

CHAPTER 2 SUBCHANNEL POWER CONTROL THROUGH WEIGHTING COEFFICIENT METHOD

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

CS229 Project Report Polyphonic Piano Transcription

LE062XF DCC Decoder for Atlas N Scale Locomotives

Physical Modelling of Musical Instruments Using Digital Waveguides: History, Theory, Practice

Analysis of WFS Measurements from first half of 2004

A cross-cultural comparison study of the production of simple rhythmic patterns

DAAB DB409 INSTRUCTION MANUAL FOR THE VFD-EL FREQUENCY CONVERTER. For the DAAB EP104 automatic control system with software version 4.

Lab P-6: Synthesis of Sinusoidal Signals A Music Illusion. A k cos.! k t C k / (1)

The Definition of 'db' and 'dbm'

Quarterly Progress and Status Report

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

Making music with voice. Distinguished lecture, CIRMMT Jan 2009, Copyright Johan Sundberg

CLASSIFICATION OF MUSICAL METRE WITH AUTOCORRELATION AND DISCRIMINANT FUNCTIONS

A Bayesian Network for Real-Time Musical Accompaniment

A STATISTICAL VIEW ON THE EXPRESSIVE TIMING OF PIANO ROLLED CHORDS

RHYTHM. Simple Meters; The Beat and Its Division into Two Parts

Drunken Sailor The Melody

Playing the Accent - Comparing Striking Velocity and Timing in an Ostinato Rhythm Performed by Four Drummers

SMS Composer and SMS Conductor: Applications for Spectral Modeling Synthesis Composition and Performance

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

Sources of Error in Determining Countermovement Jump Height With the Impulse Method

Music Fundamentals. All the Technical Stuff

Can the Computer Learn to Play Music Expressively? Christopher Raphael Department of Mathematics and Statistics, University of Massachusetts at Amhers

6.UAP Project. FunPlayer: A Real-Time Speed-Adjusting Music Accompaniment System. Daryl Neubieser. May 12, 2016

Goebl, Pampalk, Widmer: Exploring Expressive Performance Trajectories. Werner Goebl, Elias Pampalk and Gerhard Widmer (2004) Introduction

Swing Ratios and Ensemble Timing in Jazz Performance: Evidence for a Common Rhythmic Pattern

Transcription:

Dept. for Speech, Music and Hearing Quarterly Progress and Status Report Is the musical retard an allusion to physical motion? Kronman, U. and Sundberg, J. journal: STLQPSR volume: 25 number: 23 year: 1984 pages: 126141 http://www.speech.kth.se/qpsr

STGQPSR 23/1984 128 A. me1 for deceleration of physical motion We will limit our study to the types of motion which can be classif ied as motorhythmic, i.e., "rhythmic" in a motoric sense. A motorhythmic motion is any physical motion which generates impulses whose density in time has a direct relationship to the velocity of the motoric movement. Examples of such motorhythmic motion is, for instance, human or animal walking or running, train running or other motions where the steplength, i.e., the distance moved for each impulse, can be considered to be independent of the velocity and, thus, introduced as a constant (c). For such motorhythmic motion the relation between velocity (v) ard pulsation rate (T) can be written: v=c*t (1) (V = velocity (m/sec), T = tempo (impulses/sec), and c = distanceperimpulseconstant (m/ impulse) ). In analogy with this, the distance (x) as a function of number of impulses (n) will be: x=c*n (x = distance (m) and n = number of pulses) If a motion with the initial velocity vo is to come to a complete halt (v = 0) in the distance S by a constant (negative) acceleration (a) the acceleration is given by: The instant velocity (v) as a function of distance (x) from initiation of deceleration is given by the same equation: Cunbining (4) and (5) gives: In order to get an expression of the instant pulsetempo (T) as a function of the preretard tempo (T~) and the distance (x) from beginning of deceleration we combinine (1) and (7):

To complete the analogy with music, we substitute the distances x and S with the nu~nber of pulses according to relation (2), thus getting: where n is the number of pulses since the beginning of retardation and N is the total nurnber of pulses in the entire deceleration process. Finally, expressing the tempo as a function of number of pulses (P) left to the final pulse, where the velocity is zero, we get the "generalized retardat ion function" : The result can be seen in Fig. 1. It constitutes the simple model for retardation of physical motion which henceforth will be referred to as a "retardation curve" in contrast with the measured musical "retard curves" to be presented further down. Given this model of rhythrnic deceleration we need to know more of the retards we are going to compare it with. B. Anatomy of the musical retard In an attempt to describe an aspect of musical timing, Sundberg and Verrillo analyzed 24 recorded final retards in motor music, i.e., music dominated by long sequences of sbrt and equal note values. Most of the music was composed by J.S. Bach (whose preludes and fugues are good examples of such motor music) and played mainly on the hapicord. In the analysis of the retards the instant tempo (T) was defined as the inverse of tone duration. The length of the slnrtest note value was chosen as duration unit. The durations of the shortest note values were measured over a longer period of time to provide information on the preretard mean tempo (T~) in which I3e piece was played The retard length was defined as the number of shortest note values from the beginning of the final sequence, in which all notes were played slower than the preretard mean tempo, to the onset of the final chord. The inverse of the tone durations (representing the instant tempo) was plotted according to their distance from the final chord, measured in number of shortest note values. An example of such a "retard curve" obtained by this procedure is shown in Fig. 2, together with the notation of the corresponding last three measures. The retard curves were normalized with respect to retard length (N) and preretard mean tempo (T,). An "average retard" was calculated from the 24 recordings by linear interpolation of tempo and computation of averages and standard deviations at each tenth of the retard. The result can be seen in Fig. 3. The retard curves were found to exhibit the following characteristics :

INSTANT TEMPO

the beat tempo can be assumed to be zero at this point; how would a harpsichord player know when to take the hands off the keyboard, if he had not a feel of (beat) tempo during the final chord? Against this background it seems practical to reformulate the question above: What is the beat tempo at the onset of the final chord? This tempo can be estimated using the S&V article: the last part of the retards was found to exhibit a linear decrease in tempo and could, therefore, be approximated as a straight line in all except four of the 24 retards. Its length was defined as the part of the curve where the data points fell close to this line; in half of the cases it included just three data points, but in some cases up to seven points. If we temporarily accept a straight line to approximate the last retard points of the normalized average retard curve, the function TN = PN + 0.30 offers a good linear approximation of the three last points. Extrapolation of this line to the point where PN = 0 suggests the tempo to be 0.30 at the onset of the final chord, or, in other words, 30% of the preretard average value. This extrapolated function can be seen in Fig. 5. According to the reasoning above, a curve describing the decrease of beat tempo must be extended beyond the onset of the final chord and assume the value of.3 at the onset of this chord. If we modify the theoretical retardation curve accordingly, the retardation time will increase by 10 %. This modified retardation curve offers a good approximation of the retard curve. Not only does the theoretical curve fall within the onestandarddeviation bar at the last point, ht also do all predicted values fall within f0.5 standard deviation of the corresponding mean values. The function suitable to describe this extended retardation is given by: TN = J (pn + e)/(l +Lj (e = extension = 0.1) and can be studied in Fig. 5 together with the mean values and the standard deviation bars from Fig. 4. As the lengthening by 10% refers to an average curve, it must be regarded as a mean value itself; this means that the actual lengthening in the individual case could range from 4 to 14%, and still lie within the limits of one standard deviation. This, of course, would depend on factors such as individual mean tempo, retard length, construction of specific retard, etc. Before continuing further, it is appropriate to consider the question what the "average retard" in fact represents. One may argue that in reality there is no such thing as an average retard; presumably every musically acceptable retard has to be individually designed taking into consideration musical context, instrument properties, room acoustics and so on. An averaging process obviously disregards such individual con

Fig. 5. NORMALIZED RETARD TIME Retardation curve, extended by lo%, canpared with average retard values (open circles), the last three points of which have been approximated also by a straight line; bars as in Fig. 3. I BEGINNING OF RETARD ' I I I I I I 1 I 1 I I 1 I 1 I I I BAR LINE I N =15 1 \ \ I 1 I I I I I I I I 1 I I I I II IS 10 S 0 P.. DISTANCE TO FINAL CHORD (SV) Fig. 6. Performed retard capred with 10% prolonged retardation curve divided into two parts according to the values given in the figure. I

applicable to a retard situation. In any event, such rules prove the need for introducing some sort of measure of expressive deviation. A reasonable assumption would be that the performer uses at least as big margins for expressive deviations during the retard as during the preretard performance. This means that such margins skrould be added to the retardation curve, and we would then expect that the measures pertaining to individual retards should lie within these margins, if the retardation is a good model of the retards. To get a realistic estimate of margins applicable to each case, the performance of the last few preretard bars was studied and the measured deviations were expressed in relation to the computed mean tempo. The maximum duration deviations were found to be of the order of magnitude of 1070 msec with an average of about 40 msec; this results in tempo variations from 4 to 27% of the individual average tempo, with 10% as a mean value. Another factor of importance is the possibility of unintended deviations, or, in other words, playing errors. Presumably, even the most skilled musician makes small unintended variations due to technical and psychological factors tut it is difficult to separate intended from unintended deviations because of the present lack of tools for predicting what is intended. However, both unintended and intended variations are evidently included in the value of the preretard expressive deviations mentioned above. A modified model, including appropriate margins for expressive deviations, shduld explain the main part of the performed durations in single retards. The method for adapting the beat retard model to single tone retard measurements will thus be: I. Chooseanoptional length of the retardation by placing the endpoint 025% beyond the onset of the final chord. (Use the divided retard model shown in Fig. 6, if implicated by performance and notation.) 11. Add margins for expressive deviation according to the maximum deviations indicated by prior execution of nonretard performance of the same piece. An example of such a curve can be seen in Fig. 7. Applying the above mentioned procedure, the 24 retards in the S&V study were compared with the model including the expressive deviations. The result showed that the model could explain 82% of the performed durations. In two cases the divided retard model was used. In order to get an idea of the significance of this result, a simpler, alternative model comprising a linear decrease of tempo was also tested, applying the same conditions as for the previous model. An example is shown in the same Fig. 7. The results showed that this straight line model could explain no less than 83% of the measured durations. However, this required that in many cases the postulated endpoint of the retard had to be placed 50100% beyond the onset of the final chord. This implies that, for example, the final chord of the retard, shown in Fig. 7, should be sounding in 3.9 sec, which seems unrealistic in view of the fast decay of a harpsichdd tone. Moreover, a

I I I I I I I I I I I 1 1 I I I I I PRE RETARD P I I I I RETARD MARGINS FOR EXPRESSIVE DEVIATION Fig. 7. NORMALIZED RETARD TIME Predicted curve for beat retard and straight line retard applied to a normalized performed retard. Margins are added for expressive deviation (hatched area and dashed lines, respectively).

The authors are indepted to Anders Askenfelt for valuable discussions. References Bengtsson, I. & Gabrielsson, A. (1983): "Analysis and synthesis of musical rhythm", in (J. Sundberg, ed.) Studies of Music Performance, Issued by the Eloyal Swedish Academy of Music, Stockholm. Brown, P. (1979): "An enquiry into the origins and nature of tempo behaviour", Psychology of Music 7:l. Fraisse, P. (1978): "Time and rhythm perception", In Handbook of Perception, Vol. 8, pp. 203253. Lee, D.N. (1976): "A theory of visual control of braking used on information about time to collision", Perception 5, pp. 437459. Lishman J.R. (1981): "Vision and the optic flow field", Nature 293, pp 263264. Sundberg, J. & ~errillo, V. (1980): "On the anatomy of the retard: A study of timing in music", J.Acoust.SocAn. 68:3, pp 772779. Sundberg, J*, Frydgn, L. & Askenfelt, A. (1983): "What tells you the player is musical? An analysisbysynthesis study of musical performance", in (J. Sundberg, ed.) Studies of Music Performance, Issued by the 1 Swedish Academy of Music, Stockholm.