Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

Similar documents
arxiv: v1 [physics.soc-ph] 24 Mar 2009

Experimental Study of Attack Transients in Flute-like Instruments

Vocal-tract Influence in Trombone Performance

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

JOURNAL OF BUILDING ACOUSTICS. Volume 20 Number

Valuing Noise Impacts Using Hedonic Pricing and Stated Preference Methods: What does the Evidence Tell us? M. Morrison Full Paper

THE VIRTUAL BOEHM FLUTE - A WEB SERVICE THAT PREDICTS MULTIPHONICS, MICROTONES AND ALTERNATIVE FINGERINGS

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

Correlating differences in the playing properties of five student model clarinets with physical differences between them

ANALYSING DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE INPUT IMPEDANCES OF FIVE CLARINETS OF DIFFERENT MAKES

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

CHILDREN S CONCEPTUALISATION OF MUSIC

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

Texas Bandmasters Association 2017 Convention/Clinic

Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

Coimisiún na Scrúduithe Stáit State Examinations Commission LEAVING CERTIFICATE EXAMINATION 2003 MUSIC

Multidimensional analysis of interdependence in a string quartet

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

Creating a Successful Audition CD

Simple Harmonic Motion: What is a Sound Spectrum?

However, in studies of expressive timing, the aim is to investigate production rather than perception of timing, that is, independently of the listene

PulseCounter Neutron & Gamma Spectrometry Software Manual

SUBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF THE BEIJING NATIONAL GRAND THEATRE OF CHINA

Interacting with a Virtual Conductor

More About Regression

STEVE TADD WOODWIND REPAIRS (.co.uk)

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

Zooming into saxophone performance: Tongue and finger coordination

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

How players use their vocal tracts in advanced clarinet and saxophone performance

From quantitative empirï to musical performology: Experience in performance measurements and analyses

Praxis Music: Content Knowledge (5113) Study Plan Description of content

Early and Late Support over various distances: rehearsal rooms for wind orchestras

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

Finger motion in piano performance: Touch and tempo

Semi-automated extraction of expressive performance information from acoustic recordings of piano music. Andrew Earis

About Giovanni De Poli. What is Model. Introduction. di Poli: Methodologies for Expressive Modeling of/for Music Performance

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

MHSIB.5 Composing and arranging music within specified guidelines a. Creates music incorporating expressive elements.

From child to musician: skill development during the beginning stages of learning an instrument

The interaction between room and musical instruments studied by multi-channel auralization

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

BitWise (V2.1 and later) includes features for determining AP240 settings and measuring the Single Ion Area.

Pre-processing of revolution speed data in ArtemiS SUITE 1

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

Colour-influences on loudness judgements

C r o s s c u r r e n t s (revised 2003)

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

Making Progress With Sounds - The Design & Evaluation Of An Audio Progress Bar

White Paper Measuring and Optimizing Sound Systems: An introduction to JBL Smaart

6.UAP Project. FunPlayer: A Real-Time Speed-Adjusting Music Accompaniment System. Daryl Neubieser. May 12, 2016

Characterization and improvement of unpatterned wafer defect review on SEMs

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

WRAP-AROUND ACOUSTIC SCREEN FIELD TRIAL QUEENSLAND SYMPHONY ORCHESTRA 8-9 APRIL 2011

Capstone Experiment Setups & Procedures PHYS 1111L/2211L

A FUNCTIONAL CLASSIFICATION OF ONE INSTRUMENT S TIMBRES

The acoustics of the Concert Hall and the Chinese Theatre in the Beijing National Grand Theatre of China

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

Piotr KLECZKOWSKI, Magdalena PLEWA, Grzegorz PYDA

SIDRA INTERSECTION 8.0 UPDATE HISTORY

UTTR BEST TELEMETRY SOURCE SELECTOR

PRACTICAL APPLICATION OF THE PHASED-ARRAY TECHNOLOGY WITH PAINT-BRUSH EVALUATION FOR SEAMLESS-TUBE TESTING

Klages Philosophy of Trumpet Pedagogy

Precision testing methods of Event Timer A032-ET

Application Note AN-708 Vibration Measurements with the Vibration Synchronization Module

Bassoon Fingering Issues By Michael Burns

Modbus for SKF IMx and Analyst

Acoustic Echo Canceling: Echo Equality Index

about half the spacing of its modern counterpart when played in their normal ranges? 6)

Physics HomeWork 4 Spring 2015

about half the spacing of its modern counterpart when played in their normal ranges? 6)

CLASSROOM ACOUSTICS OF MCNEESE STATE UNIVER- SITY

Laser Beam Analyser Laser Diagnos c System. If you can measure it, you can control it!

ALGORHYTHM. User Manual. Version 1.0

Noise Tools 1U Manual. Noise Tools 1U. Clock, Random Pulse, Analog Noise, Sample & Hold, and Slew. Manual Revision:

A consideration on acoustic properties on concert-hall stages

Introduction to Performance Fundamentals

Application of a Musical-based Interaction System to the Waseda Flutist Robot WF-4RIV: Development Results and Performance Experiments

EMI/EMC diagnostic and debugging

Chapter 4. The Chording Glove Experiment

Sample Analysis Design. Element2 - Basic Software Concepts (cont d)

A HIGHLY INTERACTIVE SYSTEM FOR PROCESSING LARGE VOLUMES OF ULTRASONIC TESTING DATA. H. L. Grothues, R. H. Peterson, D. R. Hamlin, K. s.

Getting Started. Connect green audio output of SpikerBox/SpikerShield using green cable to your headphones input on iphone/ipad.

CURRICULUM COURSE GUIDE

Training Note TR-06RD. Schedules. Schedule types

Toward a Computationally-Enhanced Acoustic Grand Piano

Automatic Rhythmic Notation from Single Voice Audio Sources

Inter-Player Variability of a Roll Performance on a Snare-Drum Performance

Improved Synchronization System for Thermal Power Station

The Human Features of Music.

MU Class Woodwinds Syllabus Spring 2012

Work Package 9. Deliverable 32. Statistical Comparison of Islamic and Byzantine chant in the Worship Spaces

PITCH & INTONATION. Baritone T.C. The American Band College of Sam Houston State University. By Kendra R. Schwartz. A Beginner s Guide to:

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Observations and Thoughts on the Opening Phrase of Webern's Symphony Op.21. Mvt. I. by Glen Charles Halls. (for teaching purposes)

Special Studies for the Tuba by Arnold Jacobs

Habits of a Successful STRING ORCHESTRA. Teaching Concert Music and. Christopher R. Selby. GIA Publications, Inc. Chicago

PRESCOTT UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT District Instructional Guide January 2016

The influence of Room Acoustic Aspects on the Noise Exposure of Symphonic Orchestra Musicians

Transcription:

Proceedings of the International Symposium on Music Acoustics (Associated Meeting of the International Congress on Acoustics) 25-31 August 2010, Sydney and Katoomba, Australia Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers Andre Almeida (1), Marie Devillers (2), Renee Chow (2), John Smith (2), Joe Wolfe (2) (1) Laboratoire d'acoustique de l'université du Maine, Le Mans, France (2) School of Physics, The University of New South Wales, Sydney Australia PACS: 43.75.EF ABSTRACT The key systems of woodwind modern instruments minimise the need for cross fingering in lower registers. Nevertheless, performance often requires near-simultaneous movements of several fingers, often with some digits rising while others fall, especially in performance in the high registers. We measured the individual finger movements of a group of amateur and professional flutists as they played an original piece unseen before the experiment. They played a modified flute with a position detector mounted below each key. The detectors, via an interface and computer, gave the timing and speed of each key, as reported in an earlier study (Almeida et al., 2009). Here we report the changes in speed and coordination between sight-reading and performance after a short session of practice. We also report the variability among players within each group, and the differences between amateurs and professionals INTRODUCTION The long process of learning a musical instrument implies the acquisition of many different actions or gestures that are implied in a fine control of the sound produced by the instrument. In many cases, lengthy repetition of gestures that are commonly used in a particular style of music is required. For instance, in many tonal instruments in classical European music, musical scales and arpeggi are repeated a large number of times over the whole career of the musician, even after he or she becomes a professional player. This training is necessary to maintain the coordination and reflexes necessary to play note sequences that appear often in their repertoire. In a woodwind instrument such as the flute, among the main gestures implied in the performance we can cite: adjustments of the velocity, direction and width of the jet (Coltman, 1976) and speed and coordination of finger motions. Achieving appropriate control of the jet is one aim of beginners on the flute as they try to master the intonation, intensity and timbre of each note using control variables that must be adjusted for each note in order to produce a homogeneous sound throughout the playing range of the instrument (de la Cuadra et al., 2005). Fingering control is another important aim. Good coordination of the fingers is essential to play fast musical passages, and scales and arpeggi are usually rehearsed to prepare sequences that will later be performed in a musical context. Even in instruments, such as the piano, on which a note is produced by the motion of a single finger, the accuracy and speed of a musical phrase are determined by the freshness of coordination and reflexes (Bresin & Batel, 2000). Woodwinds have an added complication: they rely on the opening of lateral holes to change the effective length of the instrument and thus the note the instrument plays. These holes are open or closed by the direct or indirect action of the fingers. In many cases, two or more fingers must move to pass from one note to another. Since each combination of open and closed holes corresponds to an acoustic configuration of the resonator, it is easy to imagine that a poor coordination of finger movements has consequences on the transient sound between notes. Previous studies have considered the acoustical aspects of every possible fingering in the flute (Botros et al. 2002) and of the acoustical effects of non-simultaneous finger motions (Almeida et al. 2009). The latter includes a study on the speed and coordination of players in note transitions involving the motion of different numbers of fingers and in short musical pieces. In this work, we analyse the performance of amateur and professional musicians in more common musical contexts, including sight-reading for a musical performance. These two contexts are compared, and we propose reasons for different finger coordination performance in the two cases. MATERIALS AND METHODS This study used a modified flute that is described by Almeida et al. (2009). (Other methods of key motion monitoring for ISMA 2010, associated meeting of ICA 2010 1

25-31 August 2010, Sydney and Katoomba, Australia Proceedings of the International Symposium on Music Acoustics, ISMA 2010 musical performance have been reported, for example, by Ystad and Voinier, 2001 and Palacio-Quintin, 2003). players have different levels, based on accuracy and performance speed. Here, reflected infrared sensors were installed under each key that is operated directly by a finger. These continuously measure the (analog) position of each key as functions of time. The sensors are sufficiently light and small that they do not disturb the normal playing of the flute. The cables that bring the signal to the recording hardware, however, increase the mass noticeably and can change the feeling of holding the flute. However, most of the players that participated on the study reported feeling comfortable after a few minutes getting used to the instrument. A group of 5 musicians, divided into 2 professionals (concert player or teacher) and 3 of intermediate level participated in this study. They were asked to play an original musical piece as a first-sight reading, then were allowed 15 minutes of practice and finally to perform the piece as if in a concert. The piece (see appendix) is of no musical interest: it was specifically written to require several note transitions in which multiple finger movements are involved, sometimes with fingers in opposite directions. It also included some syncopation and accidentals so that it was not a trivial exercise in sight-reading. They had no prior knowledge of the score. They were asked to use, as much as possible, standard fingerings, and in some cases, given the exact fingering expected for one note. With these constraints, a 'maximum comfortable tempo' was requested for each trial. The full performance was recorded in a small room treated to reduce noise and reverberation. Two microphones were used, one in a stand in front of the musician, above the score, the other clipped to the flute, about 10 cm from the embouchure. Simultaneously, the 15 signals for the positions of the keys acted on by a finger were digitised and recorded in a computer via a MOTU analog-digital interface. The key signals were than analysed, using the procedure described by Almeida et al. (2009), to detect key opening and closing times. From the sound track, the durations of note transitions were extracted, and played notes compared to the musical score. Key transition data were aligned with the sound track in order to associate key movements to each note transition. Figure 1: Percentage of expected fingerings for each performance. The total number of note transitions for each performance is 117. An idea of the overall performances of the player can be obtained using a compact view of a few diagnostics performed on the measurements. Figure 2: Duration of each trial The errors are shown explicitly in the matrix below, where rows represent individual performances in the same order (top first) as figures 1 and 2. The 117 columns correspond to the sequence number of the note in the musical score. The colour code is that used in figure 1, where a white square again represents a mistake both in note and fingering. RESULTS Overall analysis over a performance We first analyse the performances in terms of correct notes and fingerings. In Figure 1, we represent the number of notes that match the original score for each trial. For each intermediate (I) or professional (P) musician, statistics for the two trials (1 st Sight or Final) are shown. In some cases, additional pitches are sounded very briefly between two expected notes. These were not counted as mistakes, but are seen as transients produced by the fingering transition. We then divide correct notes into the ones that were produced using the 'expected fingering' and those that were produced using an alternative fingering. Correct fingerings with incorrect notes represent very few cases where the note was produced using the wrong resonance of the instrument. Figure 2 shows the time used for completing the performance in each trial. Different interpretations seem to have been adopted by each musician, in particular for the two professional musicians. In particular P1 preferred speed to accuracy in the requested fingerings, whereas P2 concentrated on the requested fingerings, compromising slightly the speed. It is also apparent that the intermediate The next figure, which has the same column and line configuration, represents the number of keys moved as a colour code for each note. These range from 0 (black) to 7 (white). The first line, separated from the others, represents the expected number of fingers moved for each note in the score. A third matrix represents, for each note transition, the delay between the first moved key and the last moved key in each transition. The grey scale ranges from 0 (black) to 50 ms (white). Beyond 50 ms, the duration is always represented in white. 24% of the total number of recorded transitions had delays greater than 50 ms. 2 ISMA 2010, associated meeting of ICA 2010

23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 The fingering delays in the first sight reading and the final performance for each player are compared by plotting (Figure 5), for each transition, the transition time for the final performance against that for the same transition in the first sight-reading. The figure distinguishes note transitions in which all fingers move in one direction from those in which some fingers move in opposition to others. transitions in each category) and the middle line is the average delay for each category. The error bars represent the total extent of the delays considered for the statistics, and the few crosses the outliers that were not taken into account. Only 2 and 3 finger transitions are represented. Transitions with more fingers were less common in the piece, and they do not show clear differences between first-sight and final trials. When a small number of fingers are involved, consistent differences are observed between the first and final trials. A regular and natural pattern is that note transitions that involve fingers moving in opposite directions are performed substantially faster in the final trials. This difference is not so clear-cut in the first-sight readings even though averages follow roughly the same trend. Figure 5: Duration in seconds of fingering transitions on first sight trials plotted against the corresponding in the final performance. Plot separates the transitions having fingers moving in opposite directions from those in which all fingers move together. Figure 5 shows that, as expected, transitions with unidirectional movement are usually faster. Figure 5 also shows a correlation between transition times in the first sightreading and final performance. However, this correlation is by no means perfect. The scatter suggests that the transition time does not depend very strongly on the context of the transition in the piece. How to explain the non-zero correlations, which is typically 0.5? It is possible that some transitions may be difficult because of their context, either because of the series of fingerings that form that context, or because the player is, at that point, distracted by other difficulties. Figure 6 represents a statistical analysis of the transition time as a function of the numbers of fingers moved and whether or not fingers moved in opposite directions. The boxes correspond to quartile durations (transition durations for which the delay ranges between 25% and 75% of the total Figure 6: Statistical view of the finger motion delays for transitions involving 2 or 3 fingers moving in the same direction (t) or in opposite directions (o). Boxes represent quartile positions and the central line the average. Crosses represent outliers not taken into account for the statistics (see text). In general, the average delays are slightly shorter in final performances, and the spread around the average is smaller, indicating that the players are more accurate than in first sight-readings, especially the intermediate level players. This may be related to a higher concentration on reading in the first-sight trial. The fact that the difference is higher in the intermediate group gives a hint that the finger transitions are less automated by years of practice in this group than in the professionals. ISMA 2010, associated meeting of ICA 2010 3

25-31 August 2010, Sydney and Katoomba, Australia Proceedings of the International Symposium on Music Acoustics, ISMA 2010 Detailed analysis of a finger transition In a previous study (Almeida et al., 2009) it was shown that some non-instantaneous finger movements are 'unsafe', meaning that they may produce unwanted transient effects in the sound, depending on the order of movement of the fingerings. One case is the transition between F6 and F#6, involving the right index and ring fingers. When an intermediate configuration has both of these fingers raised, the oscillation in the resonator is harder to maintain due to an absence of a strong bore resonance close to the frequencies of F6 or F#6. It was shown that intermediate players particularly tended to avoid this situation on musical exercises, and that professionals tend to be more synchronous, staying within 10-20 ms of simultaneity. In this section we compare the performance regarding these two fingers in the first-sight and the final trials of the musical piece studied in the previous section. Here the time delay between the depressing a key and releasing the other is analysed. A positive value for the interval means that both keys are depressed during that interval (safe transition), and a negative value means that both keys are open during that interval (unsafe). We also represent other note transitions that involve the same keys moving in opposite direction but do not have the same 'safety' concern, either alone (for instance F4-F#4 or F5-F#5) or together with other keys. CONCLUSIONS During this work, an automatic method was developed for associating an analysis of recorded sound to finger action during a musical performance. In a first stage, differences were identified in the level of the performances. A comparison of speed and mistakes allows one to rank players on this aspect of playing. In some cases a better performance in terms of transition time represents more mistakes in terms of the requested fingerings. The durations of fingering transitions along the score are usually somewhat correlated between first-sight to final readings. In general, the average and standard deviation of fingering durations is reduced from the first to the final reading. Musicians may compromise accurate finger motion in favour of a higher concentration on the reading and interpretation process. Musicians tend to avoid the unsafe transitions, i.e. those in which multiphonics, broad band noise or wrong notes may briefly sound during the transition, in what is probably an acquired gesture. However, it was shown previously (Almeida et Al., 2009) that in most cases the unsafety in these transitions can be avoided by using a correct embouchure. This allows musicians to use more simultaneous transitions once they have a correct embouchure. A study combining finger analysis to embouchure analysis could test this hypothesis. Figure 7: For F6, the index and little fingers of the right hand are down, the others up. To pass to F#6, the index finger rises and the ring finger is pushed down. For this transition, the top row in each figure plots the time at which the ring finger moves minus that at which the index finger moves. The same finger motion is used in other note transitions, alone or with fingers. These data are shown in the next two rows. REFERENCES 1 A. Almeida, R. Chow, J. Smith and J. Wolfe, "The kinetics and acoustics of fingering and note transitions on the flute" J. Acoust. Soc. America, 126, 1521-1529 (2009) 2 A. Botros, J. Smith and J. Wolfe J. "The Virtual Boehm Flute-A Web Service that Predicts Multiphonics, Microtones and Alternative Fingerings" Acoust. Australia. 30, 61-66 (2002) 3 R. Bresin and G.U. Battel, "Articulation strategies in expressive piano performance" J. New Music Research. 29, 211 224 (2000) 4 J.W. Coltman, "Jet drive mechanisms in edge tones and organ pipes" J. Acoust. Soc. America. 60, 725-733 (1976) 5 P. de la Cuadra, B. Fabre, N. Montgermont and L. De Ryck, "Analysis of flute control parameters: A comparison between a novice and an experienced flautist" In Proc. Forum Acusticum, Budapest, Hungary (2005) 6 C. Palacio-Quintin "The hyper-flute", In Proc 2003 Conf. New Interfaces for Musical Expression, Montreal, Quebec, Canada, Nat Univ. Singapore pp. 206-207 7 S. Ystad and T. Voinier, "A Virtually Real Flute" Comp. Mus. J. 25, 13-24 (2001) Here, the differences between trials are small. The figure shows however that the tendency to use the 'safe' fingering does not depend considerably on the actual note transition involved. This may be an indication that musicians have learned (whether consciously or subconsciously) that having both keys closed rather than open is a safer option. 4 ISMA 2010, associated meeting of ICA 2010

23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 APPENDIX Score ISMA 2010, associated meeting of ICA 2010 5