Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

Similar documents
Multidimensional analysis of interdependence in a string quartet

However, in studies of expressive timing, the aim is to investigate production rather than perception of timing, that is, independently of the listene

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

The Relationship Between Auditory Imagery and Musical Synchronization Abilities in Musicians

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

On time: the influence of tempo, structure and style on the timing of grace notes in skilled musical performance

Tempo and Beat Analysis

Finger motion in piano performance: Touch and tempo

Zooming into saxophone performance: Tongue and finger coordination

Measuring & Modeling Musical Expression

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Object selectivity of local field potentials and spikes in the macaque inferior temporal cortex

Beating time: How ensemble musicians cueing gestures communicate beat position and tempo

Temporal Coordination and Adaptation to Rate Change in Music Performance

TEMPO AND BEAT are well-defined concepts in the PERCEPTUAL SMOOTHNESS OF TEMPO IN EXPRESSIVELY PERFORMED MUSIC

Collaboration in the choral context: The contribution of conductor and choir to collective confidence

Getting Started. Connect green audio output of SpikerBox/SpikerShield using green cable to your headphones input on iphone/ipad.

MOST FORMS OF ENSEMBLE PERFORMANCE SYNCHRONIZATION OF TIMING AND MOTION AMONG PERFORMING MUSICIANS

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

Speech and Speaker Recognition for the Command of an Industrial Robot

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

2 Autocorrelation verses Strobed Temporal Integration

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

The roles of expertise and partnership in collaborative rehearsal

CS 591 S1 Computational Audio

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Brain-Computer Interface (BCI)

Quantitative multidimensional approach of technical pianistic level

Smooth Rhythms as Probes of Entrainment. Music Perception 10 (1993): ABSTRACT

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Rhythm analysis. Martin Clayton, Barış Bozkurt

Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

Automatic music transcription

JOURNAL OF BUILDING ACOUSTICS. Volume 20 Number

Tempo and Beat Tracking

The Beat Alignment Test (BAT): Surveying beat processing abilities in the general population

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

The role of texture and musicians interpretation in understanding atonal music: Two behavioral studies

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

CONCERT HALL STAGE ACOUSTICS FROM THE PERSP- ECTIVE OF THE PERFORMERS AND PHYSICAL REALITY

An exploration of the pianist s multiple roles within the duo chamber ensemble

A Computational Model for Discriminating Music Performers

THE MAGALOFF CORPUS: AN EMPIRICAL ERROR STUDY

Perceptual Smoothness of Tempo in Expressively Performed Music

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

Analysis, Synthesis, and Perception of Musical Sounds

performance may vary as a function of piece, genre, socio- cultural context, and performers

An Investigation of Musicians Synchronization with Traditional Conducting Beat Patterns

Maintaining skill across the life span: Magaloff s entire Chopin at age 77

Detecting Audio-Video Tempo Discrepancies between Conductor and Orchestra

Chamber Orchestra Course Syllabus: Orchestra Proficient Joli Brooks, Jacksonville High School, Revised August 2016

Preferred acoustical conditions for musicians on stage with orchestra shell in multi-purpose halls

Audio-Based Video Editing with Two-Channel Microphone

LEVEL ONE Provider Reference

Sensorimotor synchronization with chords containing tone-onset asynchronies

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

ISCEV SINGLE CHANNEL ERG PROTOCOL DESIGN

Maintaining skill across the life span: Magaloff s entire Chopin at age 77

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

TOWARD AN INTELLIGENT EDITOR FOR JAZZ MUSIC

LISTENERS RESPONSE TO STRING QUARTET PERFORMANCES RECORDED IN VIRTUAL ACOUSTICS

HUMAN PERCEPTION AND COMPUTER EXTRACTION OF MUSICAL BEAT STRENGTH

CLASSIFICATION OF MUSICAL METRE WITH AUTOCORRELATION AND DISCRIMINANT FUNCTIONS

SPECTRAL LEARNING FOR EXPRESSIVE INTERACTIVE ENSEMBLE MUSIC PERFORMANCE

ECE 4220 Real Time Embedded Systems Final Project Spectrum Analyzer

Investigations of Between-Hand Synchronization in Magaloff s Chopin

Supplemental Material for Gamma-band Synchronization in the Macaque Hippocampus and Memory Formation

Estimation of inter-rater reliability

Timing In Expressive Performance

Video compression principles. Color Space Conversion. Sub-sampling of Chrominance Information. Video: moving pictures and the terms frame and

Automatic Rhythmic Notation from Single Voice Audio Sources

A STATISTICAL VIEW ON THE EXPRESSIVE TIMING OF PIANO ROLLED CHORDS

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

Chamber Orchestra Course Syllabus: Orchestra Advanced Joli Brooks, Jacksonville High School, Revised August 2016

Ferenc, Szani, László Pitlik, Anikó Balogh, Apertus Nonprofit Ltd.

A prototype system for rule-based expressive modifications of audio recordings

Experiment on adjustment of piano performance to room acoustics: Analysis of performance coded into MIDI data.

COMPUTATIONAL INVESTIGATIONS INTO BETWEEN-HAND SYNCHRONIZATION IN PIANO PLAYING: MAGALOFF S COMPLETE CHOPIN

Music Representations

Auditory Feedback in Music Performance: The Role of Melodic Structure and Musical Skill

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Paired plot designs experience and recommendations for in field product evaluation at Syngenta

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Spatial-frequency masking with briefly pulsed patterns

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

T ips in measuring and reducing monitor jitter

Temporal control mechanism of repetitive tapping with simple rhythmic patterns

AN IMPROVED ERROR CONCEALMENT STRATEGY DRIVEN BY SCENE MOTION PROPERTIES FOR H.264/AVC DECODERS

SWING, SWING ONCE MORE: RELATING TIMING AND TEMPO IN EXPERT JAZZ DRUMMING

Digital Signal Processing Detailed Course Outline

1aAA14. The audibility of direct sound as a key to measuring the clarity of speech and music

Processes for the Intersection

AUDIOVISUAL COMMUNICATION

Transcription:

International Symposium on Performance Science ISBN 978-2-9601378-0-4 The Author 2013, Published by the AEC All rights reserved Temporal coordination in string quartet performance Renee Timmers 1, Satoshi Endo 1, and Alan M. Wing 2 1 Department of Music, University of Sheffield, UK 2 School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK Temporal coordination between members of a string quartet performing an excerpt of a Haydn string quartet was characterized in terms of patterns of dependence between player note onset times estimated from acoustic data, and compared to self-reported patterns of dependence between players. Audio onsets revealed temporal dependencies indicative of a leading-following relationship between the first and second violin and a relationship of mutual adaptation between the first violin and both the viola and cello. This relationship of mutual adaptation was not reflected in the self-reported dependencies, which predominantly ascribed a leadership role to the first violin. Keywords: timing; synchronization; string quartet; self-report; measurement Investigations of group dynamics in chamber music ensembles have suggested the relevance of leadership as well as democracy for the successful operation of such groups (Murnighan and Conlon 1991). Within string quartets, artistic leadership is often attributed to the first violin, while other members may take up other roles, organizational or social, or may function as deputy leader (King 2006). The second violin may seem to have the least significant role by primarily supporting the melody; it is nevertheless essential to the success of the group, a phenomenon known as the paradox of the second violin (Mullighan and Conlon 1991). These dynamics between ensemble members concern their musical roles and social relationships, and may be indicative of processes of decision-making. It is likely, however, that similar patterns of interaction operate during performance and influence the way in which string quartet members coordinate with each other over time.

570 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG There may be a single leader of an ensemble who the other members follow to assure ensemble synchronization, or synchronization may be a more reciprocal process in which timing adaptation is bidirectional across ensemble members. For duo performances, evidence for reciprocal rather than unidirectional adaptation has been found (Goebl and Palmer 2009). Evidence also exists that accurate prediction of a partner is more beneficial to temporal synchronization than leadership, with optimal results being obtained if a duo consists of two predictors (Pecenka and Keller 2011). The case may be different, however, for larger ensembles. As Rasch (1979) demonstrated, larger ensembles may need a clearly uniting point of reference, such as a conductor, for successful synchronization. Even if ensembles have a leader who provides a primary reference for temporal coordination, it is still likely that individuals distribute attention and respond to and correct for asynchronies with other members of the ensemble. The degree of allocation of attention to timing across players is of particular interest (Keller 2001) and may vary depending on such factors as the perceptual salience of the instrument or the similarity in musical function between players. The aim of this study was to investigate how members of a string quartet adjust their timing to each other and, in particular, to investigate patterns of uni-directional or bi-directional dependencies. We estimated timing dependencies from inter-response interval data (intervals between note onsets) and compared these with self-reported dependencies between pairs of performers in order to evaluate the usefulness of self-report in exploring synchronization strategies in string quartets. Participants METHOD An existing string quartet of professional musicians participated who had played together for 5 years at the time of the study. Materials The musicians performed the first eight bars of the String Quartet in G Major, Op. 77 No. 1 by Joseph Haydn (see Figure 1). This excerpt was selected because of the relatively high proportion of synchronous notes across the two lower instruments. Violin I states a simple ornamented theme, echoed by Violin II, while Viola and Cello provide steady accompanying pulses.

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 571 Figure 1. The excerpt and a single trial example of audio data of the quartet. The vertical dotted lines indicate the note onset detected from their respective audio signal. Procedure The quartet was seated in a circle of radius approximately 2 m with, from stage right to left, Violin I, Violin II, Viola, and Cello. They performed the musical excerpt 15 times, endeavoring to make each repeat an individual performance with some variation in interpretation. At the end of the block, participants indicated their subjective estimates of temporal dependencies between pairs of performers using a questionnaire: firstly, the players reported the dependence on each player including him/herself in a percentage (the sum of the score across the quartet summed to 100%). Secondly, they indicated the dependence that they expected each of the other players would report. Audio data were recorded at 41 khz using an omnidirectional miniature condenser microphone attached below the strings between bridge and tailpiece using a rubber clip. The audio data for each instrument were rectified and then smoothed using a bi-directional 2nd-order Butterworth low-pass filter with a cut-off frequency of 50 Hz. Local maxima of the signal, corresponding to successive notes were detected (see Figure 1), and note onsets were determined using an adaptive threshold applied to the valley preced-

572 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG ing each maximum. The inter-response intervals (IRI) of note onsets were then calculated for each player. RESULTS The timing dependency between pairs of instruments was calculated by crosscorrelating IRI variability between players after removal of changes in tempo estimated from the average of the 15 repetitions. The correlation was calculated at the bar level to allow for missing onsets due to rests in Violin I. Figure 2 shows an overview of the correlations, including auto-correlations along the diagonal. Within each box, correlations at different lags are given from a negative to a positive lag of four positions. In case of negative lags, the voice in rows is shifted 1, 2, 3, or 4 bars backwards with respect to the voice in columns. In case of positive lags, the voice in rows is shifted forwards. Positive cross-correlations at lag 1 indicate that the instrument of the column follows variations in IRI of the instrument in rows. This is the case for the Viola (third column) and Cello (right column) adapting to the second Violin (second row). The second Violin (second column) followed the first Violin (top row). Mutual adaptation (positive correlations at lag 1 and lag -1) can be seen between the first Violin and Cello at lag 1, and between the first Violin and the Viola at lag 2. Negative values at lag 0 support the idea of a 1st order linear correction between instruments. Interestingly, all cells have a negative coefficient at lag 0, although the coefficients are especially strong for correlations with Violin I, and for the correlation between the Cello and Violin II. Turning from cross-correlations to autocorrelations, these were negative at lag 1, which is consistent with the Wing and Kristofferson (1973) model of internal timing control. Figure 3 shows the self-reported dependencies between pairs of instruments. The left panel shows the indications of players of the extent to which their own timing depends on the timing of others or on themselves. The right panel shows the means of the ratings by others of the timing dependency between voices other than themselves. The left panel shows that ratings of dependence were highest for self in the case of Violin I and II. The Viola and Cello indicated to depend in particular on Violin I and II. The right panel shows that expected ratings of dependence by other players were highest for self with the expected rating for Violin I next highest. The inter-rater reliability in terms of Pearson s coefficient was r=0.63 (±0.163).

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 573 Figure 2. A matrix of cross-correlation coefficients of IRIs between players. In the diagonal axis (bold squares), the autocorrelations are shown. Shaded areas indicate one standard error. Figure 3. Subjective ratings for timing dependency of own performance and the averaged ratings by the other players. The error bar represents one standard deviation. DISCUSSION The measured timing dependencies (see Figure 2) suggested several leadingfollowing relationships with Violin II following Violin I and the Viola and Cello following Violin II. Mutual adaptation was observed between Violin I

574 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG and the Viola and Cello. In contrast, the expected dependence patterns (see Figure 2, right panel) emphasized the first violin as the leader. Although the data is limited and the results preliminary, the apparent discrepancy between self-report and observed timing dependence may not be a surprise. Correction of timing errors operates at a subconscious level (Repp 2001) and may not be accessible for reflection. While this may cast doubt on the usefulness of self-report in uncovering strategies of ensemble synchronization, a combination of methods may nevertheless prove most informative in uncovering explicit and implicit strategies of temporal coordination. Address for correspondence Renee Timmers, Department of Music, University of Sheffield, 34 Leavygreave Road, Sheffield S3 7RD, UK; Email: r.timmers@sheffield.ac.uk References Goebl W. and Palmer C. (2009). Synchronization of timing and motion among performing musicians. Music Perception, 26, pp. 427-438. Keller P. E. (2001). Attentional resource allocation in musical ensemble performance. Psychology of Music, 29, pp. 20-38. King E. C. (2006). The roles of student musicians in quartet rehearsals. Psychology of Music, 34, pp. 262-282. Murnighan J. K. and Conlon D. E. (1991). The dynamics of intense work groups: A study of British string quartets. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, pp. 165-186. Pecenka N. and Keller P. E. (2011). The role of temporal prediction abilities in interpersonal sensorimotor synchronization. Experimental Brain Research, 211, pp. 505-515. Repp B. H. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, pp. 600-621. Rasch R. (1979). Synchronization in performed ensemble music. Acustica, 43, pp. 121-131. Wing A. and Kristofferson A. (1973). Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, pp. 5-12.