Children s recognition of their musical performance

Similar documents
The role of texture and musicians interpretation in understanding atonal music: Two behavioral studies

Construction of a harmonic phrase

Chapter Five: The Elements of Music

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Effects of Using Graphic Notations. on Creativity in Composing Music. by Australian Secondary School Students. Myung-sook Auh

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Musicians and nonmusicians sensitivity to differences in music performance

Perceiving Differences and Similarities in Music: Melodic Categorization During the First Years of Life

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

Chords not required: Incorporating horizontal and vertical aspects independently in a computer improvisation algorithm

The assessment of creativity in children's musical improvisations and compositions

Subjective evaluation of common singing skills using the rank ordering method

The Human Features of Music.

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

K-12 Performing Arts - Music Standards Lincoln Community School Sources: ArtsEdge - National Standards for Arts Education

6 th Grade Instrumental Music Curriculum Essentials Document

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

Hidden Markov Model based dance recognition

You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

Music Curriculum Kindergarten

Music Curriculum. Rationale. Grades 1 8

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

Listening and Thinking: A Pilot Study

Grade-Level Academic Standards for General Music

MEMORY & TIMBRE MEMT 463

Years 3 and 4 standard elaborations Australian Curriculum: Music

Praxis Music: Content Knowledge (5113) Study Plan Description of content

A Computational Model for Discriminating Music Performers

AUD 6306 Speech Science

A COMPARISON OF COMPOSITIONAL TEACHING METHODS: PAPER AND PENCIL VERSUS COMPUTER HARDWARE AND SOFTWARE

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

The Relationship Between Auditory Imagery and Musical Synchronization Abilities in Musicians

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

Modeling memory for melodies

CALIFORNIA Music Education - Content Standards

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

MUSIC CURRICULM MAP: KEY STAGE THREE:

Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription

From quantitative empirï to musical performology: Experience in performance measurements and analyses

13. ENHANCING MUSIC LISTENING IN EDUCATIONAL CONTEXT

Affective response to a set of new musical stimuli W. Trey Hill & Jack A. Palmer Psychological Reports, 106,

River Dell Regional School District. Visual and Performing Arts Curriculum Music

Music. Last Updated: May 28, 2015, 11:49 am NORTH CAROLINA ESSENTIAL STANDARDS

Toward an analysis of polyphonic music in the textual symbolic segmentation

STRATEGIES FOR WORKING WITH INACCURATE SINGERS

EXPECTANCY AND ATTENTION IN MELODY PERCEPTION

Modeling perceived relationships between melody, harmony, and key

THE EFFECTS OF COMPUTER MUSIC LEARNING ACTIVITIES ON THE TONAL APTITUDES OF CANADIAN STUDENTS ALLAN F. ANDERSON

GENERAL MUSIC Grade 3

Chapter Two: Long-Term Memory for Timbre

PRESCOTT UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT District Instructional Guide January 2016

Third Grade Music Curriculum

Connecticut State Department of Education Music Standards Middle School Grades 6-8

Music Performance Panel: NICI / MMM Position Statement

Articulation Clarity and distinct rendition in musical performance.

A description of intonation for violin

Test Design and Item Analysis

However, in studies of expressive timing, the aim is to investigate production rather than perception of timing, that is, independently of the listene

SUBMITTED TO MUSIC, TECHNOLOGY AND EDUCATION, 2 ND JUNE 2009

ScienceDirect. Humor styles, self-efficacy and prosocial tendencies in middle adolescents

THE SONIC ENHANCEMENT OF GRAPHICAL BUTTONS

Texas Music Education Research

Computational Modelling of Harmony

Director Musices: The KTH Performance Rules System

The Investigation and Analysis of College Students Dressing Aesthetic Values

Real-time composition of image and sound in the (re)habilitation of children with special needs: a case study of a child with cerebral palsy

Pitch Perception. Roger Shepard

EXPLAINING AND PREDICTING THE PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL STRUCTURE

About Giovanni De Poli. What is Model. Introduction. di Poli: Methodologies for Expressive Modeling of/for Music Performance

The effect of male timbre vocal modeling in falsetto and non-falsetto on the singing and pitch accuracy of second grade students

WESTFIELD PUBLIC SCHOOLS Westfield, New Jersey

SMCPS Course Syllabus

Curriculum Development In the Fairfield Public Schools FAIRFIELD PUBLIC SCHOOLS FAIRFIELD, CONNECTICUT MUSIC THEORY I

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Music Education (Bachelor's) Program Requirements

Instrumental Music Curriculum

The effect of focused instruction on young children s singing accuracy

Tapping to Uneven Beats

VALLEY CENTRAL HIGH SCHOOL

CHILDREN S CONCEPTUALISATION OF MUSIC

A perceptual assessment of sound in distant genres of today s experimental music

Ensemble Novice DISPOSITIONS. Skills: Collaboration. Flexibility. Goal Setting. Inquisitiveness. Openness and respect for the ideas and work of others

Chapter 117. Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills for Fine Arts Subchapter A. Elementary

Activation of learned action sequences by auditory feedback

HINSDALE MUSIC CURRICULUM

Identifying the Importance of Types of Music Information among Music Students

Music in Practice SAS 2015

Standard 1: Singing, alone and with others, a varied repertoire of music

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Music and Brain Symposium 2013: Hearing Voices. Acoustics of Imaginary Sound Chris Chafe

PRESCOTT UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT District Instructional Guide January 2016

Chapter 117. Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills for Fine Arts. Subchapter A. Elementary

BRICK TOWNSHIP PUBLIC SCHOOLS (SUBJECT) CURRICULUM

Transcription:

Children s recognition of their musical performance FRANCO DELOGU, Department of Psychology, University of Rome "La Sapienza" Marta OLIVETTI BELARDINELLI, Department of Psychology, University of Rome "La Sapienza" ECONA (Systems) Interuniversity Centre for Research on Cognitive Processing in Natural and Artificial

Introduction Children show non specialized musical abilities even before musical instruction. Such capacities, resulting from both acculturation and innate factors (Sloboda, 1988), are often manifested in musically expressive behaviours. Research concerning musical productions by children prior to the onset of formal musical instruction examined the processes underlying unrehearsed performances. Moog (1976) assessed how children changed their own spontaneous singing improvisations with advancing age level; Dowling (1984) determined the nature of musical schemata in children s singing performances; Olivetti Belardinelli et al. (1984) detected the factors implied in children s spontaneous piano productions; Mialaret (1994) studied the emergence of regularities in children s instrumental improvisations. All these studies suggest an implicit comprehension of the constraints and characteristics of the musical language by children. Although the importance of procedural abilities in unskilled children is evident, historically most research concerning musical aptitude used only discrimination and preference tasks particularly focusing attention on perceptive aspects, while the procedural and expressive ones were underestimated. More «active» tasks involving a higher level of musical cognitive processing, such as performance tasks and problem solving tasks, are used only in a few studies, (Karma, 1984; Webster, 2000), probably due to the difficulty of assessing performance abilities in untrained subjects. In spite of these difficulties Skille (1977) defined musicality as a primary human function in a framework of general functions, which are necessary to give musical expression to musicality. Recently Webster (2000) affirmed that music aptitude represents a set of constructs that relate not only to the ability to "audiate" tonal and rhythmic patterns and to make simple preference choices but also the ability to think with and to manipulate larger musical wholes. Although Olivetti Belardinelli (1996) found a good correlation between untrained children s scores to Wing Standardized Tests of Musical Intelligence (Wing, 1948) scoring subject s answers to auditory items formed by musical material and to Skille s Musical Behaviour Scale (MUBS) scoring subject s active music productions, we are convinced that, as musical aptitude refers to the whole behaviour of a person, musical experience cannot be assessed entirely by a task in which subjects analyse music. Moreover tasks demanding subjects to produce music could give information about the ways they implement their own knowledge. As Rodriguez (1998) pointed out, expression is one of the main aspect of musical aptitudes. The importance of expression in children s musicality is evident, but only a little work concerning this aspect of musical aptitude has been done until now. Musically expressive behavior is often regarded as the domain of the mature artist and by this way children's instinctive use of music for a variety of interpretative purposes is underestimated.

In the field of melodic memory research, the literature shows that even young children are able to discriminate among melodies that are identical or very similar to an initial melody over an extended period of time (Madsen, 2000). Although there is considerable evidence of relationships between performance and memory skills in musical development (Snyder, 2000; Temperly, 2001), too few studies with untrained children as subjects have been run. It is time to look for new ways of assessing musical aptitude procedurally, thanks to new digital technology that could provide different assessment tools. Several commercial software programs already exist that offer children a chance to make music without any previous experience. These programs could afford new means for assessing musical aptitude with performance-based tasks. By computer recording of children s responses to musical tasks it is possible to analyse their musical behaviour for both products and processes variables. Hickey (1995), Daignault (1997), and Younker (1997) have already used MIDI equipment and computer software to study children's composition in terms of product and process. In this work a computer allowed our children to try and re-try, hear and re-hear and to make judgments about their musical products. Aims Method This research is aimed at investigating the mutual influence of perception, production, and memory strategies in children s music recognition ability. We intend to verify children s recognition capability in recognizing their own musical products among other similar ones. In this study we used Rodriguez s paradigm (Rodriguez, 2001) on a sample of Italian children, maintaining his stimuli and procedures, while varying the retention interval between performance tasks and identification tasks. A subsidiary aim is to detect eventual influences of different cultural environments. Therefore we tested the Italian sample answers to Rodriguez s following questions: - Are children able to recognize their own musical performances? - Are there age-related tendencies in children s recognition ability of their own musical performances? - Which are the cognitive strategies used by children in this recognition task? Are there agerelated tendencies in using these strategies? Moreover, following the variation of the retention interval specifically introduced in this study, we will verify if there is an influence of the retention interval in children s recognition ability of their own musical performances.

Subjects 238 elementary students from two towns in Southern Italy participated; ages ranged from 6 to 12 with a mean age of 8.67. The sample consisted of 72 first-grade students (38 girls and 34 boys), 82 third-grade students (44 girls and 38 boys) and 85 fifty-grade students (37 girls and 48 boys). See table 1. SEX Total male female male female CLASS 1 34 38 72 CLASS 1 34 38 72 3 38 44 82 3 38 44 82 5 48 37 85 5 48 37 85 Total 120 119 239 Total 120 119 239 Table 1: Cross-table between Class and Sex Apparatus Equipment used included a Macintosh PowerBook computer, a Roland XP10 digital synthesizer with built-in MIDI interface, a set of two computer speakers and a portable audiocassette recorder. For practicing, recording and storing performances by children we used Instant Pleasure music performance software. Procedure Performance Task Subjects were presented with the familiar children s song «Twinkle, Twinkle, Little Star». The MIDI file contained both the melody and a chordal accompaniment of the song; the timbre used was a grand piano. Each subject listened to the song at least two times, but the children were allowed to listen as many times as they needed. Subsequently, using the Instant Pleasure software, the subjects «practiced» the song by simply pressing any key on the computer keyboard. Each key depression corresponded to the listening of a event by the «Twinkle, Twinkle» MIDI file.

The subjects were able to change the song s time by when the computer keys were depressed (slow, fast, regular, irregular,) and dynamics by where the computer keys were depressed (only considering the letters rows of keys, the upper one for louder, middle one for the mezzoforte and the bottom one for the softer). After the practice period, each subject was asked to make a recording of his performance. Subjects were told: «Now that you have practiced in playing this song, I will record it so that I can listen to it later. Any questions? Let s begin recording.» The subjects were allowed to re-record as many times as they preferred until they were satisfied. -Recognition task In order to control the influence of the retention intervals on recognition we varied the time between performance and recognition task. Therefore, after 4 or 6 or 8 days (according to experimental group) each subject was presented with three different interpretations of the target song. The participants were asked to recognize which melody was the one they produced among the distractors. Distractor melodies were randomly selected from the list of melodies performed by other same age subjects. The subjects were allowed to listen to each performance as many times as needed for their identification. -Interview After the recognition task each subject was asked how he could recognize his own performance. Subjects answers were recorded with an audiocassette recorder, and then transcribed and stored in a text file. -Judging task Three expert judges examined subjects answers; they were instructed to code the verbal responses to detect the cognitive strategies used by the subjects. These strategies are belonging to eight categories, subsequently grouped in three macro-categories: Sensory : Tactile, Aural Cognitive : Error detection, Elimination strategy Product : Structure, Tempo, Duration, Dynamics In order to determine the level of agreement between judges in assigning these categories to the verbal responses we used Cohen s K statistic.

Results and analysis Recognition task Similar to the American sample (Rodriguez, 2001), the Italian sample showed a good rate of correct recognition, 165 correct responses, (69%), versus 74 wrong responses, (31%). In order to verify an eventual age-related improvement of correct recognitions, we compared the frequencies of correct responses between the three age groups (Table 2). The differences are not significant (Chi 2 =.101) but there is an interesting trend of improvement in the frequencies of correct recognitions as the age increases (59,7% in first class, 70,7% in third class, 75,3% in fifth class). Recognition Total Wrong Correct Wrong Correct CLASS 1 29 43 72 CLASS 1 29 43 72 3 24 58 82 3 24 58 82 5 21 64 85 5 21 64 85 Total 74 165 239 Total 74 165 239 Table 2: Cross-table between Class and Recognition There is no difference between male and female subjects concerning the recognition task (68,3% males and 69,7% females). As regards the influences of retention intervals (table 3) the recognition performance is not impaired following an increase in the time intervals between performance and recognition (4, 6, 8 days). Recognition Total Wrong Correct Wrong Correct Retention interval 4 days 45 87 132 Retention interval 4 days 45 87 132 % 34,09 65,91

6 days 14 40 54 % 25,93 74,07 8 days 15 38 53 % 28,30 71,70 Total 74 165 239 Table 3. Cross-table between retention interval and recognition We can glean the following consideration by the data: memory of their own musical performances in elementary school children is stable, considering a maximum retention interval of eight days from the moment of performance. The verbal responses: Children s recognition strategies An important aspect of this research is the analysis of the children s verbal responses. The Kappa rates of agreement among judges are generally good, ranging from 0,37 to 0,80, but they seems rather heterogeneous among categories (table 4). Response Categories Presence Judgment Absence Judgment Kappa Tactile 66 651 0,46323 Aural 83 634 0,60171 Elimination strategy 150 567 0,53735 Error detection 95 622 0,87793 Structure 110 607 0,37671 Tempo 68 649 0,56642 Duration 40 677 0,45374 Dynamics 331 386 0,80962 Table 4. Kappa Statistics for Judges. Analyzing the two first columns of the above table, we can see that children used some categories more than others. The categories more used are: Elimination Strategy, Structure and above all Dynamics. The last result may be a consequence of the prominence of dynamic changes as a performance option in the MIDI software (three rows of keys with three different loudness). For the same reason it is strange that categories like tempo and duration are not very frequently considered. In order to verify a possible influence of different cultural environments we compared our data to the data obtained by Rodriguez (2001) from American children (table 5).

50% 40% Italian children American children percentual value 30% 20% 10% 0% Strategies tactile aural elimination strategy error detection structure tempo duration dynamics Table 5. Cross-cultural comparison between cognitive strategies. The graph shows primarily a more balanced distribution of the strategies in the American sample. Dynamics responses are the most used by both samples, but in Italian children this strategy seems to be predominant in comparison to other strategies. In order to detect age-related tendencies in the verbal responses, the Chi square test was used. Three categories showed a significant difference in the distribution of frequencies among the three age groups: in particular Error detection [Chi 2 (df=2) = 29,6 p<.001], and Tempo [Chi 2 (df=2) = 7,8 p<.05], show an improvement between the first and the fifth grade while Dynamics [Chi 2 (df=2) = 21,6 p<.001], differently from all other categories reaches a significantly higher level in the third class. In the following figure we noticed the age-related tendencies in the verbal responses (table 6): 60% tactile Judge composite percentual scores 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% Aural Elimination strategy Error Detection Structure Tempo Duration

Table 6. Age-related tendencies of response categories. The general trend of the answers categories is similar to the American one and in our case too in quite generally non linear. The increased presence of Error detection and Tempo responses in function of age, could be interpreted as a consequence of the attainment of higher level cognitive processes by the older group. Vice versa, the peculiar trend of the Dynamics category and in particular the maximum level attained in the third class, could be interpreted as an attempt to meet, by emotional contrast, cognitive requests that eight years old children can understand but are not ready to satisfy by means of cognitively abstract strategies. Discussion Our results confirm that children in all age groups succeed in recognizing their own performances and the only influence of the cultural environment seems to be limited to the recognition strategies. These data can provide us with several indications of the development of musical abilities in children. Although differences between the three age groups are not statistically significant there is a trend of improvement in recognition performances with advancing age level. Children s recognition abilities are not impaired with increasing retention interval: this result suggest that children s memory for their own musical performance is «strong» and stable. Children used prevalently the Dynamics strategy to discriminate their performance among the other ones but the recourse to higher level strategies gradually improves with age. Both a qualitative and quantitative analysis of data suggest an age-related improvement of «precision» responses, in fact older children provide more «technical» responses than the younger ones. Conclusion By means of performance tasks we can arrive at a better understanding of musical potential in children. These kind of procedures seems to be more direct and punctual than the traditional ones in estimating musical aptitudes and expressive musical performances in unskilled children and involve important implications for music teaching and learning. In this perspective we will extend the present research in assessing relations between quantitative characteristics of products (analysis of MIDI files containing children interpretations), recognition performances and verbal responses.

References Daignault, L. (1997). Children's creative musical thinking within the context of a computersupported improvisational approach to composition. Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 4681. Dowling, W. J. (1984). Development of musical schemata in children's spontaneous singing. In W.R. Crozier, & A.J. Chapman (Eds.), Cognitive processes in the perception of art. New York: Elsevier. Hickey, M. (1995). Qualitative and quantitative relationships between children's creative musical thinking processes and products. Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 145. Karma, K. (1984). Musical aptitude as the ability to structure acoustic material. International Journal of Music Education, 3, 27-30. Madsen, C.K. (2000). Discrimination and interference in the recall of melodic stimuli: musically trained and untrained adults versus children. In Sixth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Keele, UK, August 6-10, 2000. Mialaret, J.P. (1994) Régularités au cours de tâtonnements musicaux exploratoires chez le jeune enfant. In I. Deliège (Ed.), Proceedings of 3th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition. Liege, Belgium. Moog, H. (1976). The musical experience of the pre-school child. London: Schott & Co. Olivetti Belardinelli, M. (1996). Musical Intelligence and Musical Behavior in Naif and Espert School Students, Creativity and Culture. In European Council for High Ability, Wien October 19-22, 1996. Olivetti Belardinelli, M., Rossetti, M., & Capirci, C. (1985) La produzione musicale spontanea: contributo all analisi psicometrica. Comunicazioni scientifiche di psicologia generale /Scientific contributions to general psychology, 13, 95-127. Rodriguez, C.X. (1998). Children's perception, production, and description of musical expression. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46, 48-61. Rodriguez, C.X. (2001). Children's recognition of their musical performances. Contributions to Musical Education, 28, 37-50. Sloboda, J.A. (1988). La mente musicale. Psicologia cognitivista della musica. Bologna: Il Mulino.

Snyder, B. (2000). Music and memory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Bradford Books Temperly, D. (2001). The cognition of basic musical structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Bradford Books Webster, P. (2000). Broadening the concept of music aptitude: New approaches to theory and practice. In Sixth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Keele, UK, August 6-10, 2000. Younker, B. (1998). Thought processes and strategies of eight, eleven, and fourteen-year old students while engaged in music composition. Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 4217.