Semiotic modelling of biological processes: semiotic systems João Queiroz a,b,c & Charbel El-Hani a,b

Similar documents
44 Iconicity in Peircean situated cognitive Semiotics

Daniella Aguiar and Joao Queiroz Semiosis and intersemiotic translation

C. S. Peirce s formal science of signs provides an analytic framework

Author Query Form. Iconic semiosis and representational efficiency in the London Underground Diagram

Priscila Lena Farias* and João Queiroz On Peirce s diagrammatic models for ten classes of signs

Intersemiotic translation: The Peircean basis

Peircean concept of sign. How many concepts of normative sign are needed. How to clarify the meaning of the Peircean concept of sign?

Is Genetic Epistemology of Any Interest for Semiotics?

Chapter 7 C. S. Peirce and Intersemiotic Translation

Pragmatism, Semiotic mind and Cognitivism

Terminology. - Semantics: Relation between signs and the things to which they refer; their denotata, or meaning

Supplemento n. 6 a «Illuminazioni» n. 18 (ottobre-dicembre 2011) Alessandra Anastasi THE SINGING OF PRIMATES

Lecture (0) Introduction

PETER MARLER. 24 february july 2014 BETHANY DANIELS / COLLEGE OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES / UC DAVIS

Peirce and Semiotic an Introduction

Conclusion. One way of characterizing the project Kant undertakes in the Critique of Pure Reason is by

Embodied Experience and the Semiosis of Abductive Reasoning. Donna E. West State University of New York at Cortland

Habit, Semeiotic Naturalism, and Unity among the Sciences Aaron Wilson

Semiosis as an Emergent Process

Transduction and Meaning Making Issues Within Multimodal Messages

Semiotics and the Philosophy of Language UMBERTO ECO INDIANA. University Press. Bloomington & Indianapolis

Keywords: semiotic; pragmatism; space; embodiment; habit, social practice.

Space, Time, and Interpretation

MISSING FUNDAMENTAL STRATUM OF THE CURRENT FORMS OF THE REPRESENTATION OF CONCEPTS IN CONSTRUCTION

Creativity and self-organization: contributions from cognitive science and semiotics

Current Issues in Pictorial Semiotics

In Pursuit of Objectivity. Chiara Ambrosio Department of Science and Technology Studies University College London

that would join theoretical philosophy (metaphysics) and practical philosophy (ethics)?

Is there a Future for AI without Representation?

The meaning of meaning in biology and cognitive science: A semiotic reconstruction

PEIRCE ON PRACTICAL REASONING

THE PROBLEM OF NOVELTY IN C.S. PEIRCE'S AND A.N. WHITEHEAD'S THOUGHT

Necessary Connections Between Distinct Existences: A Peircean Challenge to Humeanism

An introduction to biological essentialism. John Wilkins Biohumanities Project University of Queensland

PHI6500: seminar times to be arranged early in the course. Short Essay deadline: Thursday 29th November (Thursday week 10)

Pre-conference workshop with Lars Elleström (Linneaus University) Daniella Aguiar & João Queiroz (orgs.)

Theories and Activities of Conceptual Artists: An Aesthetic Inquiry

A new aspect of sign and its implications for the Theory of Communication

RAFAEL BOMBELLI S ALGEBRA (1572) AND A NEW MATHEMATICAL OBJECT : A SEMIOTIC ANALYSIS

Cognitive poetics as a literary theory for analyzing Khayyam's poetry

SYSTEM-PURPOSE METHOD: THEORETICAL AND PRACTICAL ASPECTS Ramil Dursunov PhD in Law University of Fribourg, Faculty of Law ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION


Toward a New Comparative Musicology. Steven Brown, McMaster University

WHITEHEAD'S PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE AND METAPHYSICS

Robert Rowe MACHINE MUSICIANSHIP

Biosemiotics: To Know, What Life Knows. Kalevi Kull 1. Whether biology has studied what organisms know?

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A.

Psychology PSY 312 BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR. (3)

How Semantics is Embodied through Visual Representation: Image Schemas in the Art of Chinese Calligraphy *

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

Semiosis and pragmatism: Toward a dynamic concept of meaning

Diagrams, Iconicity, and Abductive Discovery

C. S. Peirce s Evolutionary Sign: an Analysis of Depth and Complexity within Peircean Sign Types and Peircean Evolution Theory.

Edusemiotics To Date, an Introduction of

STYLE GUIDELINES. Dear Afterimage Contributor,

Information in Biosemiotics: Introduction to the Special Issue

Natika Newton, Foundations of Understanding. (John Benjamins, 1996). 210 pages, $34.95.

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF PRAGMATISM AND AMERICAN PHILOSOPHY. The History of Reception of Charles S. Peirce in Greece 1

The erratically fine-grained metaphysics of functional kinds in technology and biology

Semiotics of culture. Some general considerations

Notes on Semiotics: Introduction

On actual semantic and aesthetic interaction with design objects

Lecture 16 Thinking about schemas Ontology [and Semiotics] and the Web

Reviews. Structures of Experience and Dispositions of Being

Natural Kinds and Concepts: A Pragmatist and Methodologically Naturalistic Account

KINDS (NATURAL KINDS VS. HUMAN KINDS)

CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW. This study should has a theory to cut, to know and to help analyze the object

Università della Svizzera italiana. Faculty of Communication Sciences. Master of Arts in Philosophy 2017/18

Pragmatism and Idealism

Design is the conscious and intuitive effort to impose meaningful order.

Jacek Surzyn University of Silesia Kant s Political Philosophy

PARTICIPATION IN ALTERNATIVE REALITIES: RITUAL, CONSCIOUSNESS, AND ONTOLOGICAL TURN

Symposium on Semiotics and Mathematics with the Special Theme 'Peirce, the Mathematician', June 11 13

SYNTHESE LIBRARY STUDIES IN EPISTEMOLOGY, LOGrC, METHODOLOGY, AND PHILOSOPHY OF SCIENCE. Managing Editor: Editors:

On Recanati s Mental Files

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

Deleuze, Thompson and Jonas: Toward a New Transcendental Aesthetic and a New Question of Panpsychism

Chapter 10 - Non-verbal Information and Artistic Expression in the Symbolosphere and Its Emergence through Secondary Perception

Data, information, and knowledge a semiotic view of phenomena of organization

THE ECOLOGICAL MEANING OF EMBODIMENT

Formalizing Irony with Doxastic Logic

International conference on William James and Pragmatism

Monday August 25. Göran Sonesson: Phenomenological semiotics.

Kalevi Kull 1. [...] and since knowledge is habit [...]. (1906, CP 4.531) 4

Narrative Dimensions of Philosophy

Peirce s Final Account of Signs and The Philosophy of Language ALBERT ATKIN

By Tetsushi Hirano. PHENOMENOLOGY at the University College of Dublin on June 21 st 2013)

Journal of Nonlocality Round Table Series Colloquium #4

Guide to the Republic as it sets up Plato s discussion of education in the Allegory of the Cave.

INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON ENGINEERING DESIGN ICED 05 MELBOURNE, AUGUST 15-18, 2005 GENERAL DESIGN THEORY AND GENETIC EPISTEMOLOGY

Theory and Practice of Tangible User Interfaces. Tuesday Week 5: Ambient Media. week. Ambient Media. At the periphery of our awareness

Undercutting the Realism-Irrealism Debate: John Dewey and the Neo-Pragmatists

Embodied music cognition and mediation technology

Semiotics of Terminology: A Semiotic Knowledge Profile

Darwinian populations and natural selection, by Peter Godfrey-Smith, New York, Oxford University Press, Pp. viii+207.

Architecture as the Psyche of a Culture

Foundations in Data Semantics. Chapter 4

Keywords: Peirce, index, degenerate, genuine, development

Ontology as Meta-Theory: A Perspective

Transcription:

Semiotic modelling of biological processes: semiotic systems João Queiroz a,b,c & Charbel El-Hani a,b a. Graduate Studies Program in History, Philosophy, and Science Teaching, Federal University of Bahia/State University of Feira de Santana, Brazil. b. Research Group in History, Philosophy, and Biology Teaching, Institute of Biology, Federal University of Bahia, Brazil. c. Institute of Arts and Design, Federal University of Juiz de Fora, Brazil. General abstract: Here we introduce biosemiotics as a field of research that develops models of life processes focusing on their informational aspects. Peirce s general concept of semiosis can be used to analyze such processes, and provide a powerful basis for understanding the emergence of meaning in living systems, by contributing to the construction of a theory of biological information. Peirce s theory of sign action is introduced, and the relation between information processing and sign processes is discussed, in fact, a semiotic definition of information is introduced. Three biosemiotic models of informational processes, at the behavioral and molecular levels, are developed, first, a model of genetic information processing in protein synthesis; second, a model of signal transduction in Bcell activation in the immune system; and, finally, a model of symbolic nonhuman primate communication. We also address some perspectives for the development of applied semiotic research in fields such as Artificial life, cognitive ethology, cognitive robotics, theoretical biology, and education. The self-corrective behavior exhibited by semiotic systems depends on the capability of using signs of different kinds as media for the communication of forms from objects to interpretants so as to constrain their own behavior. For James Fetzer, who has defined cognition as semiosis, cognitive systems develop three radically distinct modalities of semiotic behavior, corresponding to the capacity to be causally affected by signs of qualities, events and laws, respectively iconic, indexical and symbolic semiotic systems -- What makes a system semiotic thus becomes that the behavior of the system is causally affected by the presence of signals because it stands for something else iconically, indexically, or symbolically, for that system (Fetzer 1997, p.358; also 1988). In this lecture we introduce Fetzer s notion of semiotic system and explore some biological examples of his conception. Semiotic systems and the most fundamental division of signs Systems that produce, communicate, receive, compute, and interpret signs of different kinds can be classified as semiotic systems. As Fetzer writes: What makes a system semiotic thus becomes that the behavior of the system is causally affected by the presence of a sign because that sign stands for something else iconically, indexically, or symbolically, for that system (Fetzer 1997, p.358). By interpreting signs, semiotic systems can show selfcorrective behavior (Ransdell 1977, p.162). Semiosis can be defined as a selfcorrective process involving cooperative interaction between its three components. Such a self-corrective behavior depends on the capability of semiotic systems of using signs as media for the communication of forms from objects to interpretants so as to constrain their own behavior.

Peirce suggested that semiotic systems could be treated as the embodiment of semiotic processes (CP 5.314). Surely, this blurs a distinction between entities and processes which characterized Western thinking for most of its history. Peirce was, however, a process thinker, i.e., a representative of a philosophical tendency of treating processes as being more fundamental than entities as ontological categories (Rescher, 1996). A process philosophy can address entities, as Peirce does, as relatively stable bunches of processes. A semiotic system can be understood in these terms as a relatively stable (both spatially and temporally) cluster of semiotic processes. If we wish to understand how semiotic systems instantiate sign processes, we have to refine our understanding of the types of signs involved in particular semiotic processes. Particularly, we should explore in a more technical way the fundamental differences between iconic, indexical, and symbolic processes, and the Peircean framework is a proper place to pursue such an understanding (Fetzer 1988, 1997). In his most fundamental division of signs (CP 2.275), Peirce characterized icons, indexes, and symbols as matching, respectively, relations of similarity, contiguity, and law between S and O (sign-object relation) in the triad S-O-I. Iconic semiotic systems Icons are signs which stand for their objects through similarity or resemblance (CP 2.276), irrespective of any spatio-temporal physical correlation that S may have with an existent O (CP 2.299). If a determinative relation of the sign (S) by the object (O) is a relation of analogy, that is, if S is a sign of O in virtue of a certain quality that S and O share, then S is an icon of O. If S is an icon, then S communicates to I a quality of O: An Icon is a sign which refers to the Object that it denotes merely by virtue of characters of its own, and which it possesses, just the same, whether any such Object actually exists or not (CP 2.247). Among the examples of icons mentioned by Peirce, we find images, diagrams, metaphors, pictures, maps, ideographs, hieroglyphics. In terms of cognitive processes, icons are associated with sensory tasks. They are present in the sensorial recognition of external stimuli of any modality, and in the cognitive relation of analogy. According to Sebeok (1989, p.121), iconic signs are found throughout the phylogenetic series, in all modalities as circumscribed by the sense organs by which members of a given species are able to inform themselves about their environment. Signal forgery, viz., the phenomenon of mimicry, in fact, all deceptive maneuvering by plants and animals, as well as humans, often crucially depends on iconicity. Figure 3 shows an example of an iconic sign, a structure in a thorn bug which mimics a real thorn.

Figure 3: Mimicry offers an example of iconicity. In the picture, a thorn bug (Membracid). (Photo from Chip Clark, Smithsonian Institution, Copyright 1993. Reproduced under permission). Figure 4 shows a semiotic interpretation of this case of mimicry. The interpretant is the effect of the thorn on a potential predator of the bug, namely, that the latter will not try to eat the bug. In this iconic sign process, the form which is communicated from the object to the interpretant through the sign is a general similarity between the thorn in the bug and the thorn in the plant. Generally speaking, an iconic sign communicates a habit embodied in an object to the interpretant, so as to constrain the interpreter s behavior, as a result of a certain quality that the sign and the object share. Figure 4: a semiotic interpretation of mimicry in the thorn bug Indexical semiotic systems In contrast, if S is a sign of O by reason of a direct physical connection between them (CP 1.372), then S is said to be an index of O. In that case, S is really determined by O, and both must exist as events: An Index is a sign which refers to the Object that it denotes by virtue of being really affected by that Object (CP 2.248). The notion of spatio-temporal co-variation is the most characteristic property of indexical processes. The examples range from a pronoun demonstrative or relative, which forces the attention to the

particular object intended without describing it (CP 1.369), to physical symptoms of diseases, photographs, weathercocks, thermometers. In a Peircean analysis, small red spots in a child s skin, for instance, can be treated as a sign (S) which stands for a disease, say, measles, its object (O), so as to constrain its interpretant, the effect the red spots have on an interpreter, say, a doctor performing a diagnosis. The sign processes involved are indexical. The small red spots operate as signs because they are physically correlated with the disease, which is the primary constraining factor in the process, the form of which will end up producing an effect on the interpreter (Figure 5). Figure 5: a semiotic interpretation of the diagnosis of measles. In this indexical sign process, the form which is communicated from the object to the interpretant through the sign is a general physical correlation between measles and the small red spots in the skin. Generally speaking, an indexical sign communicates a habit embodied in an object to the interpretant as a result of a direct physical connection between sign and object. Symbolic semiotic systems Finally, in a symbolic relation, the interpretant stands for the object through the sign by a determinative relation of law, rule or convention (CP 2.276). According to Peirce (CP 2.307), a symbol is a Sign (q.v.) which is constituted a sign merely or mainly by the fact that it is used and understood as such, whether the habit is natural or conventional, and without regard to the motives which originally governed its selection. We have claimed elsewhere that the alarm-call system used by African vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops), a well-known case of vocal communication in non-human primates, logically satisfies the Peircean definition of symbol (Queiroz 2004, 2003; Queiroz & Ribeiro 2002). These primates possess a sophisticated repertoire of vocal signs used for intraspecific alarm purposes regarding imminent predation on the group (Seyfarth et al., 1980). Field studies have revealed three main kinds of alarm-calls, used to warn about the presence of (a) terrestrial stalking predators such as leopards, (b) aerial raptors such as eagles, and (c) ground predators such as snakes (Figure 6). Adult vervets produce these calls only in reference to the

presence of specific predators. Such calls motivate whole-group escape reactions that are specific to predator type (Figure 6). For instance, when a terrestrial predator call is uttered, vervets escape to the top of nearby trees; aerial predator calls cause vervets to hide under trees; and ground predator calls elicit rearing on the hind paws and careful scrutiny of the surrounding terrain. These are the interpretants of the calls, as signs. Figure 6: Vervet monkeys alarm-calls, their referents, and their effects on the monkeys as interpreters (interpretants). While adults share a code for predator reference, infant vervet monkeys babble these calls in response to a variety of animals (predators and nonpredators), as well as to inanimate objects. Thus, adults pay little attention to infant calls (Cheney and Seyfarth 1990). The progressive specificity of alarm-call production as vervets grow older indicates that they should learn how to use the calls in the proper context. Field experiments in which predator-specific alarm-calls were played from loudspeakers to groups of wild vervet monkeys showed that adult individuals first responded to playbacks of alarm-calls by looking around in search of a referent (predator). Remarkably, even though this referent was always absent, adult animals consistently fled away to nearby refuges according to the specific type of alarm-call played. Infant monkeys, in turn, responded poorly to playbacks, and teenagers displayed intermediate behaviors. According to the Peircean classification of signs, if the alarm-call operates in a specific way even in the absence of the external referent, it must be interpreted as a symbol of a predator class. The transition from a sensory scan behavior after the alarm auditory perception to an escape reaction motivated solely by the alarm-call corresponds to the transition from indexical semiosis (interpretation by spatio-temporal coincidence) to symbolic semiosis (interpretation mediated by law or convention) (Figure 7). The object of the sign, in the latter case, is not an object-token but rather a class of objects,

i.e., an object-type, and therefore does not need to exist as a singular event. To say that an alarm-call is a symbol of a type of predator is equivalent to say that this call evokes a brain representation (of any modality) that stands for the class of predators represented in a law-like and specific way. Figure 7: a semiotic interpretation of communication in vervet monkeys. In this symbolic sign process, the form which is communicated from the object to the interpretant through the sign is a lawful relationship between a given kind of alarm-call and a given type of predator. Generally speaking, a symbolic sign communicates a habit embodied in an object to the interpretant as a result of a regularity in the relationship between sign and object. Acknowledgement: João Queiroz and Charbel El-Hani are indebted to the Brazilian National Research Council (CNPq) and the State of Bahia Foundation for Research Support (FAPESB). Next lecture (Genetic information system)> In the next lecture we will employ semiotic concepts to propose a semantic and pragmatic account of biological information. In particular, we propose a model of genetic information system as semiosis, grounded in Peirce s theory of signs. REFERENCES Cheney, D.L., & Seyfarth, R. (1990). How Monkeys See the World. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Fetzer, J. H. 1988. Signs and Minds: An Introduction to the Theory of Semiotic Systems in J. Fetzer (ed.), Aspects of Artificial Intelligence, Dordrecht, The Netherlands. (pp. 133 161).. 1997. Thinking and Computing: Computers as Special Kinds of Signs, Minds and Machines 7: 345 364. Peirce, C. S. (EP1, 1992; EP2, 1998.). The Essential Peirce. Selected Philosophical Writings. (Vol. 1 ed. by N. Houser & C. Kloesel; Vol 2 ed. by the Peirce Edition Project). Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. (quoted as EP, followed by volume and paragraph). Peirce, C.S. 1931 1935. The Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Electronic edition reproducing Vols. I VI [C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss (eds.), Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1931 1935]; Vols. VII VIII [A. W. Burks

(ed.), same publisher, 1958]. Charlottesville: Intelex Corporation. (quoted as CP, followed by volume and paragraph.) Peirce, C.S. 1967. Annotated Catalogue the Papers of Charles S. Peirce. (ed.) R.S. Robin. Massachusetts: The University of Massachusetts Press. [quoted as MS, followed by the number of the manuscript]. Queiroz, J. & Ribeiro, S. 2002. The biological substrate of icons, indexes, and symbols in animal communication, In: The Peirce Seminar Papers The State of the Art. Vol. V (Ed.) M. Shapiro. Berghan Books. pp. 69-78. Queiroz, J. 2003. Comunicação simbólica em primatas não-humanos: uma análise baseada na semiótica de C.S.Peirce. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 25 (Supl II): 2-5. (available at http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php? script=sci_arttext&pid=s1516-44462003000600002 2004. Semiose segundo Peirce. EDUC-FAPESP. Queiroz, J. & El-Hani, C. N. 2004. Towards a Multi-level Approach to the Emergence of Semiosis in Semiotic Systems. Technical Reports DCA-FEEC- UNICAMP, DCA07-04, (pp.1-21). (available at http://www.dca.fee.unicamp.br/research/docs/techrep). Ransdell, J. 1977. Some Leading Ideas of Peirce s Semiotic, Semiotica 19 (3/4):157-178. Sebeok, T. (1989). The Sign and its Masters. University Press of America, New York.