Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones

Similar documents
Vibratory and Acoustical Factors in Multimodal Reproduction of Concert DVDs

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations

Experiments on tone adjustments

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

The quality of potato chip sounds and crispness impression

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

HUMAN PERCEPTION OF SINUSOIDAL ROTATIONAL STEERING WHEEL VIBRATION.

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

EFFECTS OF REVERBERATION TIME AND SOUND SOURCE CHARACTERISTIC TO AUDITORY LOCALIZATION IN AN INDOOR SOUND FIELD. Chiung Yao Chen

Loudness of pink noise and stationary technical sounds

JOURNAL OF BUILDING ACOUSTICS. Volume 20 Number

Colour-influences on loudness judgements

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

Loudness and Sharpness Calculation

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

ADVANCED PROCEDURES FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC NOISE EVALUATION

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

The Cocktail Party Effect. Binaural Masking. The Precedence Effect. Music 175: Time and Space

T ips in measuring and reducing monitor jitter

Electrical and Electronic Laboratory Faculty of Engineering Chulalongkorn University. Cathode-Ray Oscilloscope (CRO)

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

Psychoacoustics. lecturer:

DIFFERENCES IN TRAFFIC NOISE MEASUREMENTS WITH SLM AND BINAURAL RECORDING HEAD

THE EFFECT OF PERFORMANCE STAGES ON SUBWOOFER POLAR AND FREQUENCY RESPONSES

Rhona Hellman and the Munich School of Psychoacoustics

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

Methods to measure stage acoustic parameters: overview and future research

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

ACTIVE SOUND DESIGN: VACUUM CLEANER

Loudness of transmitted speech signals for SWB and FB applications

Preferred acoustical conditions for musicians on stage with orchestra shell in multi-purpose halls

BBN ANG 141 Foundations of phonology Phonetics 3: Acoustic phonetics 1

2. Measurements of the sound levels of CMs as well as those of the programs

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

Effect of room acoustic conditions on masking efficiency

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Do Zwicker Tones Evoke a Musical Pitch?

The Lecture Contains: Frequency Response of the Human Visual System: Temporal Vision: Consequences of persistence of vision: Objectives_template

S. S. Stevens papers,

Progress in calculating tonality of technical sounds

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Music Representations

Room acoustics computer modelling: Study of the effect of source directivity on auralizations

Dimensions of Music *

REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England

MIE 402: WORKSHOP ON DATA ACQUISITION AND SIGNAL PROCESSING Spring 2003

ANALYSING DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE INPUT IMPEDANCES OF FIVE CLARINETS OF DIFFERENT MAKES

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

Binaural Measurement, Analysis and Playback

Analysis of WFS Measurements from first half of 2004

CATHODE RAY OSCILLOSCOPE. Basic block diagrams Principle of operation Measurement of voltage, current and frequency

Human Vibration Meter

The Fox News Eect:Media Bias and Voting S. DellaVigna and E. Kaplan (2007)

BACKGROUND NOISE LEVEL MEASUREMENTS WITH AND WITHOUT AUDIENCE IN A CONCERT HALL

Applied Acoustics 73 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Applied Acoustics

CLASSROOM ACOUSTICS OF MCNEESE STATE UNIVER- SITY

THE DIGITAL DELAY ADVANTAGE A guide to using Digital Delays. Synchronize loudspeakers Eliminate comb filter distortion Align acoustic image.

RECORDING AND REPRODUCING CONCERT HALL ACOUSTICS FOR SUBJECTIVE EVALUATION

OCTAVE C 3 D 3 E 3 F 3 G 3 A 3 B 3 C 4 D 4 E 4 F 4 G 4 A 4 B 4 C 5 D 5 E 5 F 5 G 5 A 5 B 5. Middle-C A-440

THE ACOUSTICS OF THE MUNICIPAL THEATRE IN MODENA

Interior and Motorbay sound quality evaluation of full electric and hybrid-electric vehicles based on psychoacoustics

Lab P-6: Synthesis of Sinusoidal Signals A Music Illusion. A k cos.! k t C k / (1)

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Modeling memory for melodies

Musical Signal Processing with LabVIEW Introduction to Audio and Musical Signals. By: Ed Doering

Largeness and shape of sound images captured by sketch-drawing experiments: Effects of bandwidth and center frequency of broadband noise

A 5 Hz limit for the detection of temporal synchrony in vision

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

Getting Started with the LabVIEW Sound and Vibration Toolkit

MASTER S THESIS. Sound Quality Evaluation of Floor Impact Noise Generated by Walking. Payman Roonasi

REPORT NO CRT-001h SOUND OUTPUT MEASUREMENTS ON SIX INVERTER GENERATOR SAMPLES RENDERED TO

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Generating the Noise Field for Ambient Noise Rejection Tests Application Note

Calibration of auralisation presentations through loudspeakers

Auto-Tune. Collection Editors: Navaneeth Ravindranath Tanner Songkakul Andrew Tam

Hybrid active noise barrier with sound masking

SUBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF THE BEIJING NATIONAL GRAND THEATRE OF CHINA

Trends in preference, programming and design of concert halls for symphonic music

Airborne sound insulation between two cinemas

Implementing sharpness using specific loudness calculated from the Procedure for the Computation of Loudness of Steady Sounds

Acoustical design of Shenzhen Concert Hall, Shenzhen China

Shock and Vibration Tests on SmartScan Interrogators to ISO :

Transcription:

Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones Sebastian Merchel, M. Ercan Altinsoy and Maik Stamm Chair of Communication Acoustics, Dresden University of Technology, Germany sebastian.merchel@tu-dresden.de Abstract. In this study, two experiments were conducted to determine the curves of equal intensity perception for sinusoidal vertical whole-body vibrations (WBV) of seated subjects over the frequency range from 0 Hz to 250 Hz. Vibrations were presented to subjects using a at hard seat. In total, 0 participants were asked to match the intensity of dierent vibrations, using a method of adjustment. The obtained contours were compared with the threshold of vibration and to vibrations cross-modally matched to tones from isophones. The shapes of the equal intensity contours in the present study show reasonable agreement with the contours from other studies despite the use of dierent methodologies and experimental questions. The contours show a characteristic similar to the perception threshold. No dependency of vibration magnitude on the shape of the contours was found in the applied dynamic range. However, large inter-individual variations were observed. The results imply that vibration curves that are cross-modally matched to isophones show similar characteristics. Keywords: Equal Intensity Contour, Whole-Body Vibration, Isophone, Audio- Tactile Perception, Cross-Modality Matching Introduction In a previous study [], pure tones from isophones were cross-modally matched to vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations. It was hypothesized that the resulting vibrations are perceived with equal intensity. The present study investigates whether those contours coincide with curves of equal intensity perception for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations. Earlier studies have determined equivalent comfort contours for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations mainly in the context of health risk estimation. Thus, only high vibration magnitudes and low frequencies have been investigated. A review can be found in Grin [2]. The overall shape of the equivalent comfort contours is somewhat consistent between most of these studies: above

2 5 Hz the contours rise with increasing frequency. However, these contours represent discomfort or annoyance from whole-body vibrations. It is questionable if (dis-)comfort or annoyance can be compared to perceived vibration intensity. Few studies exist that measure vibrations close to the perception threshold or for frequencies above Hz. Therefore, equal intensity contours will be measured for low magnitudes between 0 Hz and 250 Hz. In an aim to compare the equal intensity contours with the perception threshold for sinusoidal vertical whole-body vibrations, threshold curves from various laboratories [38] were obtained and are summarized in Figure. The shapes of the threshold contours are similar. An overall trend of increasing threshold with increasing frequency is found over the range from 5 Hz to 300 Hz. However, Miwa [3] and Parson & Grin [4] measured lower thresholds, particularly at lower frequencies. Dierent body postures or body support might explain some variability between studies in this frequency range. For example, the surface of the seat used by Morioka & Grin [5] was contoured (to provide contact with the ischial tuberosities) and approximately half the size of the at seats used in other studies, which themselves also provided contact with the thighs. The absence of contact with the thighs might reduce sensitivity to low-frequency vibrations. Dierences between studies might also be partially explained by different psycho-physical methods. For example, Miwa [3], Bellmann [6], Stamm et al. [7] and Merchel & Leppin [8] used dierent adaptive n-interval forced choice methods, whereas Parsons & Grin [4] and Morioka & Grin [5] employed a 'yes-no' method. Two frequency regions can be separated and tted by st order regressions. At lower frequencies, the perception threshold increases slightly by approximately.3 db per octave. Above 50 Hz, the increase climbs up to 6.5 db per octave. No prediction can be made outside of the shown frequency range. The regression curves must be interpreted carefully since few measurement points are available at higher frequencies. However, Morioka and Grin [5] reported a signicant increase for frequencies above 200 Hz. It can be seen that the threshold curve from ISO 263:989 [9], which was removed from the revised ISO 263:2003 [0], does not represent the data well. Thus, the tted curves will be used for comparison later in this study. 2 Experiment Two experiments were conducted to determine the curves of equal intensity perception of sinusoidal vertical whole-body vibrations for seated subjects. The subjects were asked to compare the intensity of a reference vibration with a test stimulus using a method of adjustment. In the rst experiment, two curves determined using dierent reference frequencies were obtained. One reference frequency was selected and used in the second experiment to determine equal intensity curves over a wider dynamic range.

3 Stamm, Altinsoy & Merchel (200) Merchel & Leppin (2009) Morioka & Griffin (2008) Bellmann (2000) Parsons & Griffin (988) Miwa (967) st order regressions ISO 263 2 (989) 0.0 70 60 0.00 Fig.. Perception threshold for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations from various laboratories in comparison to the threshold from ISO 263:989 [9]. st order regressions were tted to the data below and above 50 Hz. 2. Setup Whole-body vibrations were generated vertically using an electro-dynamic shaker. The subject was seated on a at hard wooden seat (460 mm x 460 mm) with both feet on the ground. There was no backrest. The transfer characteristic of the vibrating chair is strongly dependent on the individual person []. This phenomenon is referred to as the body related transfer function (BRTF). The BRTF of each subject was individually monitored using a vibration pad (B&K Type 455B) and a Sinus Harmonie quadro measuring board and individually compensated using inverse lters in Matlab. For higher frequencies, the vibration chair can emit acoustic noise. Pink noise was presented at 74 db(a) to acoustically mask the noises emitted by the chair. In addition, unwanted auditory feedback possibly delivered through bone conduction will be masked as well. The audio signal was delivered through an external Hammerfall DSP Multiface sound card, amplied by a Phone-Amp G93 and reproduced through a set of Sennheiser HDA 200 closed dynamic headphones. The participant was able to control the amplitude of the vibration using a rotary knob that was innitely adjustable and did not possess any visual indicators (Grin Technology, PowerMate).

4 2.2 Subjects Ten subjects (6 male and 4 female) voluntarily participated in both experiments. Most of the participants were students between 9 years and 27 years old (mean 23 years). The participants had masses between 62 kg and 85 kg (mean 7 kg) and indicated to have no hearing or spinal damage. The subjects were instructed to sit upright with comfortable posture, their hands on their thighs and both feet at on the ground. Additional plates were used to adjust the height of the feet until the thighs were approximately horizontal and level with the seat. 2.3 Stimuli and Experimental Design Seven vibration frequencies over a wide frequency range (0 Hz, 20 Hz, 50 Hz, Hz, 50 Hz, 200 Hz and 250 Hz) were selected for the experiments. In the rst experiment, two frequencies were selected as reference frequencies (20 Hz and Hz) with a xed acceleration level of db. The reference vibration was presented for one second followed by a 0.5 s break. Afterwards, one of the test frequencies was reproduced for one second. The order of the test frequencies was completely randomized. All vibration signals were faded in and out using half a hanning window of 50 ms anks. Reference and test stimuli were marked visually using the experimental interface controlled by Matlab. The task of the subject was to adaptively adjust the perceived intensity of the test vibration to the perceived intensity of the reference vibration using the rotary knob. The test frequency was adjustable with a minimum step size of 0.25 db. The initial acceleration level of the test whole-body vibration was ±5 db (a random oset was used for each trial). A low initial acceleration was necessary because the dynamic range of vibration perception is small and a high level of vibration may cause discomfort. This sequence was automatically repeated until the participant was satised with his/her match. The subject was free to take as much time as necessary to make the proper adjustments. The total duration of the experiment varied between participants and took between 25 and 35 minutes to match both reference vibrations with all 7 test vibrations (including a familiarization phase in the beginning). For the Hz reference condition, each match was repeated three times for all participants to check intra-individual repeatability. In the second experiment, a reference frequency of 20 Hz was used with various acceleration levels ranging close to the perceptual threshold until moderately strong values ( db, 05 db, db, 5 db and db). The experimental design was identical to the rst experiment. The total duration of the experiment varied between 35 minutes and 45 minutes, depending on the individual subject.

5 3 Results and Discussion 3. Experiment - Dierent Reference Frequencies Figure 2 shows the individual equal intensity contours for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations for all subjects using a reference frequency of Hz with an acceleration level of db. As expected, the results show small deviations at the reference frequency. The individual contours match well toward higher frequencies, but increasing inter-individual dierences are found toward lower frequencies. The intra-individual standard deviations (not plotted here) are comparatively small, in the range between db and 3 db independent of frequency, which indicates good repeatability. In Figure 3, individual equal intensity contours for a reference frequency of 20 Hz with an acceleration level of db are plotted. The measured individual contours agree for low frequencies but show considerable dierences toward higher frequencies. The amount of variation is comparable to the data shown in Figure 2. It can be concluded that contours of equally perceived intensity are highly subject dependent. This dependency could be explained with strong inter-individual dierences in the perception threshold as found in [8]. However, no threshold data exist for the participants in the present study. To compare the results, the mean was calculated for both conditions. The averaged intensity contours are plotted in Figure 4 with inter-individual standard deviations. The mean values increase depending on the frequency from 0 Hz to 250 Hz. The inter-individual standard deviations increase with increasing distance from the reference frequencies. However, the averaged contours show reasonable agreement. Thus, the 20 Hz reference stimulus was used with various levels in the following experiment. The averaged contours should be interpreted and used carefully because of the high inter-individual deviations. 3.2 Experiment 2 - Dierent Magnitudes Figure 5 shows the averaged equal intensity contours for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations using a reference frequency of 20 Hz with acceleration levels of db, 05 db, db, 5 db and db. The contours are almost parallel at lower frequencies and show some deviations at higher frequencies. This nding can be explained by the increasing distance to the reference stimulus. Two subjects in particular showed inconsistencies for higher test frequencies (see Figure 3). The inter-individual standard deviations increase with increasing frequency. The variations are independent of vibration magnitude and comparable to the results shown in Figure 4; they will not be plotted here for a better overview. The averaged contours increase depending on the frequency from 0 Hz to 250 Hz. The slope is shallow up to Hz and steepens for higher frequencies

6 60 50 40 30 0 0.0 Fig. 2. Individual equal intensity contours for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations for all subjects using a reference frequency of Hz with an acceleration level of db. 60 50 40 30 0 0.0 Fig. 3. Individual equal intensity contours for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations for all subjects using a reference frequency of 20 Hz with an acceleration level of db.

7 60 50 Equal intensity contour relative db at Hz Equal intensity contour relative db at 20 Hz 40 30 0 0.0 Fig. 4. Comparison between averaged equal intensity contours for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations using a reference frequency of 20 Hz and Hz with an acceleration level of db. up to 250 Hz. The curves increase above Hz with approximately 9 db per octave. The general shape is comparable with the averaged perception threshold, which is also plotted in Figure 5 for comparison. Few studies have investigated equal perception contours for low magnitudes of vibration and/or a broad frequency range. Bellmann [6] measured an equal intensity contour between 5 Hz and Hz using an adaptive two-interval forced choice procedure. A reference frequency of 20 Hz at db was used, which corresponds to the weakest reference stimuli of the present study. The resulting contour is plotted in Figure 5 for comparison and shows reasonable agreement. Morioka & Grin [2] used magnitude estimation to determine judgements of discomfort caused by vertical vibrations over a wide frequency and dynamic range. Equal (dis-)comfort contours were then calculated from the data. The resulting curves are plotted in Figure 5 for comparison. Note that the bottom and top contour in the plot do not predominantly represent measured vibrations but are determined by extrapolation. The contours show a similar tendency as in the present study, but no pronounced break at Hz. The data from Morioka & Grin [2] conrm that the contours are similar to the perception threshold for low sensation magnitudes. However, Morioka & Grin [2] found a dependence of the magnitude on the shape of the equal comfort contours. This result diers somewhat from the present ndings but might be explained by the dierent experimental question (discomfort versus intensity) and the solely low

8 60 50 40 30 Equal intensity contours Merchel (this study) Equal annoyance contours Howarth & Griffin (988) Equal intensity contour Bellmann (2002) Equal comfort contours Morioka & Griffin (2006) Perception threshold 0 0.0 Fig. 5. Comparison of equal perception contours for sinusoidal vertical whole-body vibrations from dierent laboratories. acceleration levels used in the present study. The results at low magnitudes in the present study deviate in general from literature using higher accelerations (e.g., health risk, annoyance or discomfort curves; see Grin [2] for an overview), which show steeper slopes. Earlier data can be found in Howarth & Grin [3], who used an acoustical reference stimulus (/3 octave band noise centered at khz) to measure annoyance caused by vibration using a method of magnitude estimation. Only frequencies up to 63 Hz were measured with a small dynamic range. Interestingly, the resulting annoyance contours are consistent with the equal intensity contours from the present study as illustrated in Figure 5. Merchel et al. [] hypothesized that cross-modally matched vibrations with tones from equal loudness contours might be similar to equal intensity contours for whole-body vibrations like the ones measured in the present study. Figure 6 shows the results from both studies for comparison. The data is consistent between experiments. This nding suggests that equal intensity contours could be used to predict cross-modal matching between the loudness of sounds and the perceived intensity of whole-body vibrations. Most participants reported that intensity matching within a modality, which reduces experimental time and complexity, is a much easier task than matching across modalities. Merchel et al. [] found that an increase of 20 phon in the loudness level resulted in a 5 db - 6 db increase in the matched acceleration level (at loudness levels greater than 40 phon). A 20 phon increase in the loudness level corresponds to

9 60 50 40 30 Equal intensity contours Merchel (this study) Cross modal matched vibration with 60 phon tones Cross modal matched vibration with phon tones Cross modal matched vibration with phon tone Perception threshold 0 0.0 Fig. 6. Comparison of equal intensity contours for sinusoidal vertical whole-body vibrations (present study) with cross-modally matched vibrations with tones from equal loudness contours []. a quadruplication of perceived loudness. It might thus be assumed that the perceived intensity of sinusoidal vertical whole-body vibrations is approximately doubled through a 2.5 db - 3 db increase in acceleration level. This trend may only be true for the investigated frequency and dynamic range and could be tested with a magnitude estimation experiment in a further study. 4 Summary and Outlook In this study, contours of equally perceived intensity were determined for vertical sinusoidal whole-body vibrations. The following results were obtained: - The shapes of the equal intensity contours in the present study show reasonable agreement with the contours from other studies despite the use of dierent methodologies and experimental questions. - Small intra-individual and large inter-individual variations were observed. - Contours from cross-modally matched vibrations with tones from equal loudness contours show a similar shape if compared to the measured equal intensity contours for whole-body vibrations of the present study.

0 Future research should investigate even lower vibration magnitudes than the present study. Another open question is the variability between subjects, which remains to be explained. 5 Acknowledgements The authors wish to thank Prof. U. Jekosch for her support and informative discussions. References. Merchel, S. and Altinsoy, M.E.: Cross-Modality Matching of Loudness and Perceived Intensity of Whole-Body Vibrations, Proceedings of HAID'0, Copenhagen, Denmark (200) 2. Grin, M.J.: Handbook of Human Vibration, Academic Press, London (9) 3. Miwa, T.: Evaluation Methods for Vibration Eect, Part : Measurements of Threshold and Equal Sensation Contours of Whole-Body for Vertical and Horizontal Vibrations, Industrial Health 5, pp. 83-205 (967) 4. Parsons, K.C. and Grin, M.J.: Whole-Body Vibration Perception Thresholds, J. Sound Vib. 2, pp. 237-258 (988) 5. Morioka, M., Grin, M.J.: Absolute Thresholds for the Perception of Fore-and-Aft, Lateral, and Vertical Vibration at the Hand, the Seat, and the Foot, J. Sound Vib. 34, pp. 357-370 (2008) 6. Bellmann, M.: Perception of Whole-Body Vibrations: From Basic Experiments to Eects of Seat and Steering-Wheel Vibrations on the Passenger's Comfort Inside Vehicles, Ph.D. Thesis, Carl von Ossietzky Universitt Oldenburg (2002) 7. Stamm, M., Altinsoy, M.E. and Merchel, S.: Frequenzwahrnehmung von Ganzkrperschwingungen im Vergleich zur auditiven Wahrnehmung I und II, Proceedings of DAGA'0, Berlin, Germany (200) 8. Merchel, S., Leppin, A. and Altinsoy, M.E.: The Inuence of Whole Body Vibrations on Loudness Perception, ICSV 6, Krakw, Poland (2009) 9. International Organization of Standardisation: ISO 263-2:989 Evaluation of Human Exposure to Whole-Body Vibration Part 2: Continuous and Shock-Induced Vibrations in Buildings ( Hz to Hz), Geneva (989) 0. International Organization of Standardisation: ISO 263-2:2003 Evaluation of Human Exposure to Whole-Body Vibration Part 2: Vibrations in Buildings ( Hz to Hz), Geneva (2003). Altinsoy, M.E. and Merchel, S.: BRTF - Body Related Transfer Functions for Whole-Body Vibration Reproduction Systems, DAGA, Rotterdam, Netherlands (2009) 2. Morioka, M., Grin, M.J.: Magnitude-Dependence of Equivalent Comfort Contours for Fore-and-Aft, Lateral and Vertical Whole-Body Vibration, J. Sound Vib. 298, pp. 755-772 (2006) 3. Howarth, H.V.C., Grin, M.J.: The Frequency Dependence of Subjective Reaction to Vertical and Horizontal Whole-Body Vibration at Low Magnitudes, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 83, (4), pp. 406-43 (988)