Comparative Study made on Piano Timbre Perception

Similar documents
Topics in Computer Music Instrument Identification. Ioanna Karydi

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

A perceptual assessment of sound in distant genres of today s experimental music

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

Perceptual and physical evaluation of differences among a large panel of loudspeakers

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

What is music as a cognitive ability?

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

GCT535- Sound Technology for Multimedia Timbre Analysis. Graduate School of Culture Technology KAIST Juhan Nam

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

Perceptual Evaluation of Automatically Extracted Musical Motives

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

Pitch and Timing Abilities in Inherited Speech and Language Impairment

Timbre blending of wind instruments: acoustics and perception

Perception and Sound Design

Effect of task constraints on the perceptual. evaluation of violins

EMS : Electroacoustic Music Studies Network De Montfort/Leicester 2007

Cognitive modeling of musician s perception in concert halls

STUDY OF VIOLIN BOW QUALITY

Brain-Computer Interface (BCI)

Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

A study of the influence of room acoustics on piano performance

How do we perceive vocal pitch accuracy during singing? Pauline Larrouy-Maestri & Peter Q Pfordresher

A Computational Model for Discriminating Music Performers

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

TYING SEMANTIC LABELS TO COMPUTATIONAL DESCRIPTORS OF SIMILAR TIMBRES

Children s recognition of their musical performance

Musical Illusions Diana Deutsch Department of Psychology University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Music Complexity Descriptors. Matt Stabile June 6 th, 2008

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Instrument Recognition in Polyphonic Mixtures Using Spectral Envelopes

A FUNCTIONAL CLASSIFICATION OF ONE INSTRUMENT S TIMBRES

ONLINE ACTIVITIES FOR MUSIC INFORMATION AND ACOUSTICS EDUCATION AND PSYCHOACOUSTIC DATA COLLECTION

A QUERY BY EXAMPLE MUSIC RETRIEVAL ALGORITHM

Abnormal Electrical Brain Responses to Pitch in Congenital Amusia Isabelle Peretz, PhD, 1 Elvira Brattico, MA, 2 and Mari Tervaniemi, PhD 2

Population codes representing musical timbre for high-level fmri categorization of music genres

Music Segmentation Using Markov Chain Methods

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

Recognising Cello Performers Using Timbre Models

Perceptual differences between cellos PERCEPTUAL DIFFERENCES BETWEEN CELLOS: A SUBJECTIVE/OBJECTIVE STUDY

University of Huddersfield Repository

MUSI-6201 Computational Music Analysis

MEANINGS CONVEYED BY SIMPLE AUDITORY RHYTHMS. Henni Palomäki

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

Subjective evaluation of common singing skills using the rank ordering method

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

INTER GENRE SIMILARITY MODELLING FOR AUTOMATIC MUSIC GENRE CLASSIFICATION

Analysis of vibration signals using cyclostationary indicators

Speech and Speaker Recognition for the Command of an Industrial Robot

A Need for Universal Audio Terminologies and Improved Knowledge Transfer to the Consumer

Brain.fm Theory & Process

ROLE OF FAMILIARITY IN AUDITORY DISCRIMINATION OF MUSICAL INSTRUMENT: A LATERALITY STUDY

A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL STUDY OF LOW AMPLITUDE SUPERSONIC AIRCRAFT NOISE AND OTHER TRANSIENT SOUNDS

About Giovanni De Poli. What is Model. Introduction. di Poli: Methodologies for Expressive Modeling of/for Music Performance

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

CS229 Project Report Polyphonic Piano Transcription

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

What Can Experiments Reveal About the Origins of Music? Josh H. McDermott

UC San Diego UC San Diego Previously Published Works

Speech To Song Classification

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

CTP431- Music and Audio Computing Musical Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology KAIST Juhan Nam

Acoustic Scene Classification

The quality of potato chip sounds and crispness impression

Room acoustics computer modelling: Study of the effect of source directivity on auralizations

From quantitative empirï to musical performology: Experience in performance measurements and analyses

Features for Audio and Music Classification

Experimental Study of Attack Transients in Flute-like Instruments

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

The role of texture and musicians interpretation in understanding atonal music: Two behavioral studies

Spectrum analysis and tone quality evaluation of piano sounds with hard and soft touches

Stewart, Lauren and Walsh, Vincent (2001) Neuropsychology: music of the hemispheres Dispatch, Current Biology Vol.11 No.

Noise assessment in a high-speed train

Embodied music cognition and mediation technology

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Violin timbre and the picket fence

A User-Oriented Approach to Music Information Retrieval.

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

1. BACKGROUND AND AIMS

Automatic morphological description of sounds

Analysis, Synthesis, and Perception of Musical Sounds

Recognising Cello Performers using Timbre Models

Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

DERIVING A TIMBRE SPACE FOR THREE TYPES OF COMPLEX TONES VARYING IN SPECTRAL ROLL-OFF

Arts, Computers and Artificial Intelligence

Dial A440 for absolute pitch: Absolute pitch memory by non-absolute pitch possessors

EE391 Special Report (Spring 2005) Automatic Chord Recognition Using A Summary Autocorrelation Function

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

K-12 Performing Arts - Music Standards Lincoln Community School Sources: ArtsEdge - National Standards for Arts Education

Transcription:

Comparative Study made on Piano Timbre Perception Cosmin Gherghinoiu, Sciences cognitives, Institut de Psychologie - Université Paris, Paris, France, cosghe@yahoo.fr Charles Besnainou, Michèle Castellengo LAM, Laboratoire d AcoustiqueMusicale, rue de Lourmel, 0, Paris, France {chbesnai, castel}@lam.jussieu.fr 0 piano professionals and musical acoustics experts have participated to this listening experiment. We compared results obtained with two different perceptual tasks: similarity judgment on a same-different evaluative scale and free categorization. Chromatic scales and melodies played by a single pianist on different concert pianos and recorded under identical acoustic conditions constituted the two types of stimuli. Data extracted from subject s descriptions suggest a distinct vocabulary linked to piano timbre. Similar statistics obtained with two different listening tests illustrated that piano players are able to describe precisely the feature of timbre through listening as well as playing. The classes of similarities organized by listeners gathered instruments with close spectrums. Even if the subjects had used different listening strategies, the classifications were the same for the majority of them. Preliminary sound analysis supports the semantics used for describing features of timbre. Introduction The first level to recognize sound is the source identity, but timbre has no precise definition because it refers to a quality more than to a dimension (i.e. quantity). Then, sounds timbre can be compared if they are produced by similar sources. Problematic Our research is focused mainly on the categorization aspects of the piano timbre. The aim is to find relations between timbre perception and the physical/acoustical variations responsible in perceptive categorization and quality preference. We expect significant correlations between verbal descriptors and piano groups characters. Differences between pianos, revealed through auditory categorization tasks could help in defining acoustical descriptors of subjects gathering. The comparative analysis of these vectors must bring information on timbre categorization and perception. Test Setup. Experimental Methods The method of timbre (dis)similarity judgment demands the subjects to listen to pairs of stimuli and to quantify their degree of (dis)similitude on a graded different-same perceptive scale (fig.). This method develops a total listening and a spontaneous perceptive judgment, allowing subjects the possibility of answering what they really perceived. The interpretation of data is made through the tree analysis method. The same method of analysis is used in the model of free categorization, which is a technique of experimentation elaborated according to the principle of prototypical categorization. Subjects can freely setup classes of stimuli by following the (dis)similarity criteria. All items must be classified. This task has two parts: the first requires the organization of the stimuli in classes following timbre characteristics; the second demands semantic qualification as well as the establishment of a prototype for each one of these classes.. Procedure The instruments were chosen in order to create a homogeneous group in terms of manufacture and quality. Nine concerto pianos were used in this experiment; except a single / queue (piano ) all pianos were / queue. The pianos manufacture was hidden in order to avoid subjectivity in quality judgments. The sound was captured in the same position of microphones by rotating the pianos. A single pianist played the same two musical sequences on each instrument.. Listening Test Stimuli were presented stereo over professional speakers in a phono-isolated room at LAM by using a McIntosh computer interface. Generally, 0 min were enough for passing the entire test and all the subjects were able to finish it. More than 0% of participants had already participated to previous listening tests comcerning musical or noise stimuli.

Forum Acusticum 00 Budapest. Participants 0 professional musicians, women and men between and years old, were tested. Several pianists had participated in a previous study in which the same instruments were used. The participants were in majority pianists and acousticians but there were also several luthiers and tuner craftsmen. The subject s panel was split in two, in order to examine the influence of stimuli order.. Sound Stimuli Two types of stimuli were used. The first was a chromatic scale descending from La 0 Hz to La 0 Hz and the second was a seconds sequence from The Obermann s Valley by Frantz Liszt, treated with a short fade in and fade out. In task A, the chromatic scales couples were presented with a sec. inter-trial interval, the interval between items of a pair being, sec. Subjects were previously familiarized with the nature of stimuli.. Tasks Three auditory tasks (A, B, B) and one visual task (C) were presented to the participants. Task A ( min) required the subjects to listen to a long sequence of pairs of downward chromatic scales (LA 0Hz to LA 0Hz) played on various pianos. Musicians had to estimate the degree of similitude on a different-same evaluative scale as shown in the next representation: DIFFERENT undecided SAME 0 Figure : different-same evaluative scale There were scales of this type, each one of them corresponding to a piano pair. Regarding the traditional method, I introduced an additional element, the possibility of answering "undecided" ("do not know"). The role of the "undecided" box was to divide the scale in two distinct perceptive sections: different not different. However, if the subject chooses "undecided", that could also represent a behavioral "marker". Task B (0 min) required the subjects to freely set up groups of pianos by judging them trough timbre similarities. This categorization task had two sections: B and B, each one representing the application of the same method on different stimuli. Stimuli used in B were individual chromatic scales from task A. In the B task, the piano items corresponded to a sec. musical sequence from the "Obermann s Valley" by F Liszt. Subjects were asked to define categorization criteria only after the classes were already formed. Stimuli were displayed on the computer screen. Participants had the opportunity to listen freely the piano items by activating the corresponding shortcuts. It is necessary to specify that we changed the items depending on tasks feature in order to avoid subjectivity in categorization preference. In the visual task C the stimuli were the same chromatics scales as those used in tasks A and B, this time presented like images (sonogram representations, fig.,). Figure : piano & (0 khz, 00 bands, 00% time window), hamming Figure : piano & (0 khz, 00 bands, 00% time window, hamming) The instructions of the visual task remained identical to those of the listening task B, the subject having to gather the images through free criteria. The images were simplified sonograms of the chromatic scales as shown in the previous example.. Analysis Method Developed on contrast models, the tree analysis method transforms the average matrix of all individual answers (proximity matrix fig. ) in a tree representation, as showed in the figure below. In our case, proximity matrix has rows and columns corresponding to the pianos we used. The number situated at the intersection between rows and columns indicates how different the pianos were inside the

Forum Acusticum 00 Budapest couple (=same, 0=different). Any possible pair and order configuration are included. 0,00,00 GRAPHIQUE EVOLUTION REPONSES POUR LES MEMES PIANOS,00,00 Proches vers différents,00,00,00,00 Moyenne Sujet,00,00 0,00 Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Ordre réelle de paires GRAPHIQUE EVOLUTION REPONSES POUR LES MEMES PIANOS 0,00 Figure : Proximity matrix for subject in task A The tree structure (fig, etc.) is constructed through this proximity matrix and offers a bi-dimensionally image, the experimenter having no choice in choosing the representation. Each tree knot represents a category and each leaf represents an object included in this category. The distances between the group s elements are interpreted in terms of distinctive traits and the distances between the groups as commons traits for the group s elements. For example, by measuring distance through the shortest way between the end of two leafs we can determine how distant the corresponding stimuli were. In task A (fig.) the largest perceptive distance was found between Piano & while the shortest corresponds to the Piano &. The coefficients related to branches knots represent the taux d arboricite [arborescent rate (A.R.)] and the class s knots symbolize le taux de quadruplets bien representees (Q.B.R.) [Well Represented Quadruplets Rate]. Tree structure requires several statistic conditions. Previous research showed that tree structure becomes pertinent when the values of these coefficients are greater than 0.. We considered this method is well adapted to our study since the nature of physical parameters involved in the perceptive organization was not preset. Results and Discussions In order to verify if the musicians were able to judge timbre similarity in a coherent way, we constituted in task A several behavioral markers. More precisely, specific pairs formed by repeating the same stimulus (Piano Piano, Piano - Piano etc) were positioned at equal distance in the sequence. The next example illustrates the answers evolution for subjects and 0 (fig ) in case of the pairs formed by repetition of the same piano. The average answers are represented in blue and the individual answers in red. Proches vers différents,00,00,00,00,00,00,00,00,00 0,00 Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Ordre réelle de paires Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Piano - (/) Moyenne Sujet 0 Figure : Graphics on evolution of answers for pairs formed by the same piano. Subjects & 0. Observations made on markers reveal that the great majority of participants were highly precise in their evaluations. However, isolated subjects (like subject 0) had difficulties in finding correctly the highest () degree of similarity for the pairs formed by the repetition of the same piano. Despite this, nobody was eliminated from the statistics by considering that all answers are pertinent. By studying subject s trees, we found that interactions between subjects and tasks or between subjects and stimuli (fig ) were not significant, fact which supports the solid musical formation of the participants. This validates similar results obtained through the marker method. Sujet0 Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet0 Sujet Sujet0 Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet0 Sujet Sujet0 Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet0 Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Figure : Subject trees for B and B tasks Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet0 Sujet Sujet Sujet0 Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet Sujet An important global observation is that the perceptual distances between piano classes revealed the same configurations for both methods and stimuli conditions (fig., )

Forum Acusticum 00 Budapest 0.0 0. 0.0 0. 0.0 0.0 0. 0.0 0. 0. 0. 0.0 Figure : Task A, Piano tree. Task B, Piano tree Figure 0: Task A. Acousticians 0.0 0. 0.0 0. 0. 0. 0.0 0. 0. 0.0 0.0 0. 0. 0. 0.0 0.0 Figure : Task B, Piano tree. Task C, Piano tree Subjects didn t encountered major difficulties regarding task A and nobody stopped during the stimuli series. Concentration as well as frequent changes in the listening strategy due to the repetition of stimuli, determined the subjects to consider the task A the most interesting but also the most difficult one. The statistics of the tree configuration had the A.R. 0. and Q.B.R 0. (fig. ). This means the groups' structure did not have enough coherence, however the distances between elements in these classes are relevant. Attention was paid to stimuli order effects inside series but no significant influences were found among the orders of presentation (fig. ) 0.0 0. 0.0 0. 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0. 0.0 Figure : Task A. First order & second order of stimuli Musical formation of the participants was also taken into account and the results showed non-significant interactions between these categories. (Fig. 0&) 0.0 Figure : Task A. Pianists & Other musicians However, it appears that piano classes organized by acousticians (fig. 0) are the most coherent (coefficient ). Thus, they established in the task A a distinct group formed by pianos &, configuration that is absent in classes gathered by the others. Interestingly, this configuration is very well defined in the visual task C (fig. ). This could suggest that specific listening strategies as well as specific skills in analyzing visual representations of sounds, give acousticians more pertinence in their answers. The main classes constituted in all these tasks were piano & and piano &. The tree configuration related to B task (fig. ) had A.R. 0. and QBR 0.. For B (fig. ) the corresponding coefficients were 0. and 0.. Task B (music) was considered the most pleasant and took more time than B (chromatic scales). Listening to a musical phrase requires superior cognitive functions because of the complex structure of the stimulus. Second part of these two tasks required correlations between piano sound and semantic descriptors. The comparison between task correspondent trees reveals quite identical structures (fig., ). Same configuration was obtained in task C (fig ). More interesting is the fact that in this visual task the classes were highly homogeneous revealing very short perceptual distances between elements inside groups. That means classes of similarities were formed especially by inclusion rather by exclusion. This task was only to minutes long, constraining the subjects to apply a quick evaluation, through a holistic analysis

Forum Acusticum 00 Budapest of the images. We intended to establish if visual and auditory systems are managed in same manner through the categorization tasks. Results showed close tree configurations with those from the auditory tasks. In order to examine connections between verbalization corpora and the acoustical structure of stimuli we chose the best represented piano classes which correspond to the same two couples in all tasks: Piano & and Piano &. As previously mentioned, in the second part of tasks B and B subjects described the characteristics of the groups, which were already formed in the first part. For the first group we often met adjectives like: clair [clear], brillant, net [precise] (B) as well as brillant, metallique [metallic], clair, ouvert [open] (B). Considering the second group constituted by pianos &, the verbalizations were: sourd [dull], doux [soft], feutré [felty] (B) or feutré, doux, intérieur (B). Interestingly, despite the same tree organization, the adjective sourd was mostly used for defining the chromatic scales, feutré being used in case of musical sequence. These two words are synonyms in the piano language ; in spite of this, subjects had significant preferences in using these attributes depending on the type of stimuli. The subjects were also asked to establish the best representative (prototype) for each class of pianos but they encountered difficulties for this requirement. The detailed analysis of the acoustic descriptors will constitute the next aim of our study. The present research institutes only a global approach by observing in sound spectrum architecture possible connections with the piano classes and semantics. Thus, we found that the average spectrum concerning the same chromatic scale played on different pianos supports the verbal descriptors as well as group s configuration. Regarding the first piano couple, the spectrum structure shows a relatively equilibrate distribution of energy and reveals the presence of superior partials (fig., ) Figure : Piano chromatic scale average spectrum. FFT size: 0 bands, triangular function, 0 khz On the contrary, the second group formed by pianos & had an unequal distribution of energy, mostly concentrated in the inferior spectrum (fig, ). Figure : Piano chromatic scale average spectrum. FFT size: 0 bands, triangular function, 0 khz Figure : Piano chromatic scale average spectrum. FFT size: 0 bands, triangular function, 0 khz Figure : Piano chromatic scale average spectrum. FFT size: 0 bands, triangular function, 0 khz For the first group, spectral envelope follows a quite straight slope, better observed in case of piano (fig ). The other couple presents a clear trimming between inferior and superior spectrum manifested around, /, khz.

Forum Acusticum 00 Budapest Conclusions Preliminary observations in sound spectrums suggest that two main axes correspond to piano classes and their semantic descriptors. The most obvious was the Brilliant Opaque (or synonyms) dimension. Another direction appears to be équilibré (balanced) / non équilibré (unbalanced) (or synonyms), and should correspond to the repartition of energy in sound spectrum. Trees configuration revealed that coefficients of classes were quite powerful. Except several difficulties in establishing the prototypes all subjects responded to the tasks demands. The main gathering concerns two big classes, the most obvious being,,, and,,. The piano & were difficult to classify and appear in the same category, in this case formed by the exclusion of these two elements from the other categories. Inside these groups, the most significant classes were & (brilliant) and & (sourd) [dull]. From the perspective of quality and preference, they found piano as being the most pleasant piano. This result is not surprising since piano was the unique ¾ queue used in our experiment. We also noticed that, piano was qualified as very aggressive, (B test), the sonogram revealing a higher energy that was unequally distributed in sound spectrum. Piano was also difficult to classify which could come from the staccato manner of interpretation on this particular instrument. Several musicians included in the present investigation had also participated in previous research made on semantic attributes used in piano timbre descriptions. The study was made on the same instruments. The difference consisted in the fact that the subjects had the possibility of a free playing on the instruments while in the present work they were constrained to listen only. Comparisons between these two tests revealed similar attributes and common timbre features as well as close tree configurations. This statement suggests that pianists were able to describe and categorize timbre in the same manner through listening as well as playing. Order effects as well as interactions between stimuli, subjects and methods did not revealed significant influences in the final classification. Same results were obtained by studying musical formation or age and gender interactions. Data extracted from subject s descriptions suggests a distinct vocabulary linked to piano timbre. Classes of similarities organized by listeners gathered instruments with close spectrums. Even if the subjects had used different listening strategies, the classifications were the same for the majority of them. Also, preliminary sound analysis supports the semantics used by subjects for describing the features of piano timbre. Acknowledgements Firstly, I wish to thank all colleagues and students who had voluntarily participated in our experiment. I especially wish to thank the members of LAM for their informational support and interest for this study. We would also like to thank the National Conservatory of Music from Paris for providing us with the necessary instruments and space without which this study could not have been carried out in the required conditions. References A. Askenfelt, (0) Five lectures on the Acoustics of Piano, Stockholm, Royal Swedish Academy of Music, publication n J P.Barthélemy, Guénoche A, Les arbres et les représentations de proximité Ed. Masson., Paris () Barthélemy J P. (), Similitude, Arbres et Typicalité «Sémantique et cognition», D Dubois (CNRS, Paris), pp 0 Belin, P., M. Zilbovicius, S. Crozier, et al. Lateralization of speech and auditory temporal processing. J. Cognit. Neurosciences. 0, pp 0 Bregman A. S. (0), Auditory scene analysis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, p - Busson S. (00) «Etude des attributs perceptifs d un ensemble de pianos» DEA Acoustique Appliquée, Université du Maine Laboratoire d Acoustique Musicale Paris Castéllengo M. (), La perception des sons complexes, Cours d Acoustique Musicale, CNSMD Paris Castellengo M, Dubois D. ( 00) Timbre ou timbres? Propriété du signal, de l instrument, ou construction cognitive?, CNRS- Laboratoire d Acoustique, Musicale, Paris Gherghinoiu C. (00), Etude sur la perception du timbre de piano DFS Acoustique Musicale CNSMDP, Laboratoire d Acoustique Musicale, Paris Handel S. () Listening. An introduction to the perception of the auditory events, Cambridge, MA: MIT Presse Johnsrude I, Penhune V, Zatorre R. (000), Functional specificity in the right auditory cortex for perceiving pitch direction, Brain Köhler W. La Psychologie de la Forme, Gallimard, Paris () McAdams S., Faure A. () Comparaisons de profils sémantiques et de l espace perceptif de timbres musicaux, S.F.A, Teknea Peretz I, Hyde K. (00), What is specific to music processing? Insights from congenital amusia, Trends Cogn.Sci., pp Robin, D.A., D. Tranel & H. Damasio, (0), Auditory perception of temporal and spectral events in patients with focal left and right cerebral lesions, Brain Lang., pp Rosch E. () Principles of Categorization, Cognition and categorisation, Eds Eleanor Rosch and Barbara Lloyd. Laurence Erlaum Ass. Publishers, Hillsdale, NJ. Samson, S. & R.J. Zatorre, (), Contribution of the right temporal lobe to musical timbre discrimination Neuropsychologia n, pp 0. Tversky A., Gati I. () Studies of Similarity in Cognition and Categorisation, Eds Eleanor Rosch and Barbara Lloyd. Laurence Erlaum Ass. Publishers, Hillsdale, NJ. pp - Zatorre, R.J. & S. Samson. (), Role of the right temporal neocortex in retention of pitch in auditory short-term memory, Brain, pp 0. 0