The Power of Listening

Similar documents
Effects of Musical Training on Key and Harmony Perception

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

From "Hopeless" to "Healed"

What is music as a cognitive ability?

Effects of Asymmetric Cultural Experiences on the Auditory Pathway

Impaired learning of event frequencies in tone deafness

at least one work different to those offered in the preliminary audition. Scales, arpeggios and sight-reading may be tested.

Katie Rhodes, Ph.D., LCSW Learn to Feel Better

UNDERSTANDING TINNITUS AND TINNITUS TREATMENTS

SCHOOL OF MUSIC. Page 1 of 7

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

The Healing Power of Music. Scientific American Mind William Forde Thompson and Gottfried Schlaug

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

Music training and the brain

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

A sensitive period for musical training: contributions of age of onset and cognitive abilities

Cognitive modeling of musician s perception in concert halls

Trauma & Treatment: Neurologic Music Therapy and Functional Brain Changes. Suzanne Oliver, MT-BC, NMT Fellow Ezequiel Bautista, MT-BC, NMT

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

Affective Priming. Music 451A Final Project

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Music. Music Instrumental. Program Description. Fine & Applied Arts/Behavioral Sciences Division

Science in the News: Music and the Human Brain

Action Representation of Sound: Audiomotor Recognition Network While Listening to Newly Acquired Actions

Do musicians have different brains?

Musical talent: conceptualisation, identification and development

Resources. Composition as a Vehicle for Learning Music

The power of music in children s development

K-12 Performing Arts - Music Standards Lincoln Community School Sources: ArtsEdge - National Standards for Arts Education

INSTRUMENTAL MUSIC SKILLS

Abstract REVIEW PAPER DOI: / Peter Ahnblad. International Tinnitus Journal. 2018;22(1):72-76.

Lutz Jäncke. Minireview

Neural Signatures of the Aesthetic of Dance

MUSICAL EAR TRAINING THROUGH ACTIVE MUSIC MAKING IN ADOLESCENT Cl USERS. The background ~

A NIRS Study of Violinists and Pianists Employing Motor and Music Imageries to Assess Neural Differences in Music Perception

Embodied meaning in musical gesture Cross-disciplinary approaches

Tinnitus: How an Audiologist Can Help

Music Training and Neuroplasticity

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

Becoming musically enculturated: effects of music classes for infants on brain and behavior

Chapter Five: The Elements of Music

MUSIC EDUCATION AND MUSIC THERAPY (MED) & (MTY)

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

COWLEY COLLEGE & Area Vocational Technical School

Consciousness and Cognition

Advanced Placement Music Theory

Devices I have known and loved

MU 321 ELEMENTARY PIANO I

THE MOZART EFFECT: EVIDENCE FOR THE AROUSAL HYPOTHESIS '

The role of the Alexander technique in musical training and performing

EMPLOYMENT SERVICE. Professional Service Editorial Board Journal of Audiology & Otology. Journal of Music and Human Behavior

Course Overview. Assessments What are the essential elements and. aptitude and aural acuity? meaning and expression in music?

Connecticut State Department of Education Music Standards Middle School Grades 6-8

Activation of learned action sequences by auditory feedback

Nature of memory for music performance skills. Caroline Palmer. McGill University

MUSIC (MUS) Music (MUS) 1

FOR IMMEDIATE RELEASE. Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs) The following Q&A was prepared by Posit Science. 1. What is Tinnitus?

Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription

Music. Music-Instrumental

SMARTING SMART, RELIABLE, SIMPLE

EL DORADO UNION HIGH SCHOOL DISTRICT Educational Services. Course of Study Information Page

The e ect of musicianship on pitch memory in performance matched groups

Music Education (MUED)

Can parents influence children s music preferences and positively shape their development? Dr Hauke Egermann

Music BCI ( )

Music, Brain Development, Sleep, and Your Baby

Statistical learning and probabilistic prediction in music cognition: mechanisms of stylistic enculturation

Analysis on the Value of Inner Music Hearing for Cultivation of Piano Learning

Music (MUS) Courses. Music (MUS) 1

The Beat Alignment Test (BAT): Surveying beat processing abilities in the general population

Preparatory Orchestra Performance Groups INSTRUMENTAL MUSIC SKILLS

Topics in Computer Music Instrument Identification. Ioanna Karydi

Brain.fm Theory & Process

Learning and Liking of Melody and Harmony: Further Studies in Artificial Grammar Learning

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY

I. INTRODUCTION. Electronic mail:

Inhibition of Oscillation in a Plastic Neural Network Model of Tinnitus Therapy Using Noise Stimulus

Stewart, Lauren and Walsh, Vincent (2001) Neuropsychology: music of the hemispheres Dispatch, Current Biology Vol.11 No.

PELLISSIPPI STATE COMMUNITY COLLEGE MASTER SYLLABUS EAR TRAINING III MUS Class Hours: 1.0 Credit Hours: 1.0

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Using machine learning to support pedagogy in the arts

This Is Your Brain On Music. BIA-MA Brain Injury Conference March 30, 2017 Eve D. Montague, MSM, MT-BC

1/18/17. MUSIC 232: Fundamentals of Music Teaching GB: Loops Only Project

Evaluating trained singers tone quality and the effect of changing focus of attention on performance

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

MUSIC (MUS) Music (MUS) 1

Music. Associate in Arts in Music for Transfer (ADT: A.A.-T)

Evolutionary Computation Applied to Melody Generation

BAND Grade 7. NOTE: Throughout this document, learning target types are identified as knowledge ( K ), reasoning ( R ), skill ( S ), or product ( P ).

INSTRUCTIONS FOR AUTHORS

Overlap of Musical and Linguistic Syntax Processing: Intracranial ERP Evidence

Modeling Melodic Perception as Relational Learning Using a Symbolic- Connectionist Architecture (DORA)

Effects of Musical Tempo on Heart Rate, Brain Activity, and Short-term Memory Abstract


Making Connections Through Music

Comparing methods of musical pitch processing: How perfect is Perfect Pitch?

Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch

Volume 22, Number 4, December 2016 Copyright 2016 Society for Music Theory

Advanced Orchestra Performance Groups

Chords not required: Incorporating horizontal and vertical aspects independently in a computer improvisation algorithm

Transcription:

The Power of Listening Auditory-Motor Interactions in Musical Training AMIR LAHAV, a,b ADAM BOULANGER, c GOTTFRIED SCHLAUG, b AND ELLIOT SALTZMAN a,d a The Music, Mind and Motion Lab, Sargent College of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 02215, USA b Department of Neurology, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts 02115, USA c The Media Lab, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Boston, Massachusetts 02139, USA d Haskins Laboratories, New Haven, Connecticut 06511, USA ABSTRACT: We trained musically naive subjects to play a short piano melody by ear in a fully monitored computerized environment and tested their potential to acquire a functional linkage between actions and sounds. Individual notes that were simply acoustic pretraining signals became physically meaningful posttraining. In addition, we found preliminary evidence that passive listening to a newly learned musical piece can enhance motor performance in the absence of physical practice. KEYWORDS: musical training; performance; auditory-motor; implicit learning INTRODUCTION When playing a musical instrument, movements are naturally paired with direct auditory feedback. Musical training, therefore, can establish a multimodal functional mapping that links what we hear and what we play. Professional musicians often report feeling a physical sensation when listening to music they know how to play. Similarly, recent studies in the visual-motor domain have shown that when dancers watched familiar and performable dancing movements, their brains automatically simulated those movements. 1 While one may speculate on the underlying neural perception-action mechanisms in the musician s brain, 2 it is unclear whether such an audiomotor linking capability is an innate predisposition and even a prerequisite for becoming a musician, or is simply experience dependent. 3 To address this, we trained nonmusicians to play a novel piece of music and closely monitored their learning process. We expected that subsequent listening to the acquired musical Address for correspondence: Amir Lahav, The Music, Mind and Motion Lab, Boston University, 635 Commonwealth Ave., Boston, MA 02215. Voice: 617-353-7497. lahav@bu.edu Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1060: 189 194 (2005). 2005 New York Academy of Sciences. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.042 189

190 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES piece might unconsciously prime the listener s corresponding motor repertoire, which in turn might lead to motor improvement. METHODS Experiment 1 Fifty-eight right-handed nonmusician college-level subjects (32 women, 26 men; mean age = 22.3 ± 2.8) attended a single piano session, in which they learned to play a novel musical piece (FIG. 1) on a piano keyboard, using their right hands and a set of five adjacent keys (F-G-A-B -C), in a fixed fingering position (i.e., the same finger always hit the same key). Motivated by previous training tools,4 we designed innovative MIDI-based software for learning by ear with no sight-reading required. Subjects learned to play the piano role (solo) along with a prerecorded accompaniment (guitar, bass, and drums), while a computer didactically notified them when note (wrong key press) or timing (± >1/16th) errors occurred. To complete a piano FIGURE 1. The musical piece: Subjects learned the piano part gradually, section by section, in the following measure order: 1 2; 3 4; 1 4; 5 6; 1 6; 7 8; 1 8.

LAHAV et al.: THE POWER OF LISTENING 191 FIGURE 2. Learning times of the musical piece are shown for group distribution and for individual subjects (inset). session, subjects went through a series of trials until reaching error-free performance. The minimum possible learning time was 12 minutes. Pitch-Recognition-Production Test Before and after their piano-training session subjects were presented with single notes (taken from the piano piece) in random order, and were asked after hearing each note, to press the corresponding piano key. Percent correct responses was measured. Experiment 2 Following the initial piano session (Exp. 1), subjects (n = 36) were randomly assigned into three groups and underwent three additional 20-minute listening/practicing sessions over a period of one week. The piano-listening group passively listened to the same musical piece they had previously learned. During listening sessions, subjects lay supine with their palms facing up. Finger positions were monitored digitally using a motion-tracking system and a passive-marker glove to ensure that subjects followed instructions to stay as still as possible. The nature-listening group

192 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES FIGURE 3. Mean PRP test score before and after piano training session. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. listened to nature sounds, to control for treatment effects due merely to auditory exposure. The practicing group received additional practice sessions with the initial musical piece. Performance Test Subjects were asked to play the whole musical piece as accurately as possible, while their keyboarding was not accompanied by the original rhythmic-harmonic background instrumentation. Subjects note/timing errors were measured. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Nonmusicians Learn to Play by Ear We first examined how nonmusicians learn to play a musical piece for the first time (Exp. 1). The average learning time to error-free performance was 28.5 minutes (STD = 12.9). Interestingly, learning times followed a bimodal distribution (FIG. 2), with most subjects learning relatively quickly and a small group taking considerably longer. A similar distribution pattern was found also for learning a different musical piece (data not shown). Classifying subjects by their learning times may open an experimental window for future brain imaging studies comparing neural activity of super learners versus poor learners.

LAHAV et al.: THE POWER OF LISTENING 193 FIGURE 4. Mean correct notes for the musical piece played at the right time during performance test. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. A Single Piano Session Facilitates Pitch Recognition Here we ask whether learning to play a musical piece by ear could act to induce a pitch-to-key mapping, in which perceived notes can be identified independently of their original musical context. To this end, a pitch-recognition-production (PRP) test was performed before and after the initial piano session. To rule out learning effects during the PRP test, subjects did not receive knowledge of results (auditory feedback) when pressing the piano keys. Results showed that subjects improved their scores from approximately 30% pretraining to about 60% posttraining (FIG. 3, P = 1 10 15 ). These findings hint at a newly learned pitch recognition skill, consistent with the view that the ability to encode pitch develops with experience. 5 Passive Listening to Music Improves Motor Performance We tested the three listening/practicing groups (see METHODS, Exp. 2) on subjects ability to play the previously learned musical piece. On average, the piano-listening group performed significantly better in the performance test than the naturelistening group (P =.01), but not as well as (though not far behind) the practicing group (FIG. 4, P = 1 10 7 ). Analysis of our digital motion-tracking system verified that subjects fingers indeed did not move during listening sessions. These findings suggest that during passive listening, neural mechanisms linking sounds and actions

194 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES may implicitly facilitate musical motor performance. We are now in the process of investigating further behavioral and functional imaging measures to determine in greater detail the processes underlying this motor improvement (manuscript in preparation). [Competing interests: The authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.] REFERENCES 1. CALVO-MERINO, B., D.E. GLASER., J. GREZES, et al. 2005. Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex 15: 1243 1249. 2. HAUEISEN, J. & T.R. KNOSCHE. 2001. Involuntary motor activity in pianist evoked by music perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13: 786 792. 3. SCHLAUG, G. & C. GASER. 2003. Brain structures differ between musicians and nonmusicians. J. Neurosci. 23: 9240 9245. 4. BANGERT, M. & E. ALTENMÜLLER. 2003. Mapping perception to action in piano practice: a longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neurosci. 4: 26 40. 5. TRAINOR, L.J. 2005. Are there critical periods for musical development? Dev. Psychobiol. 46: 262 278.