Sonority restricts laryngealized plosives in Southern Aymara

Similar documents
Sonority as a Primitive: Evidence from Phonological Inventories

CHAPTER 1 CLUSTER PHONOTACTICS AND THE SONORITY SEQUENCING PRINCIPLE. organized into well-formed sequences according to universal principles of

Semester A, LT4223 Experimental Phonetics Written Report. An acoustic analysis of the Korean plosives produced by native speakers

Myanmar (Burmese) Plosives

Sonority as a Primitive: Evidence from Phonological Inventories Ivy Hauser University of North Carolina

LINGUISTICS 321 Lecture #8. BETWEEN THE SEGMENT AND THE SYLLABLE (Part 2) 4. SYLLABLE-TEMPLATES AND THE SONORITY HIERARCHY

The sonority hierarchy and NO LONG VOWEL: Theoretical implications

The odds of eternal optimization in OT

1.0 Reconstruction or the Proto-Germanic Obstruent Inventory 1.1 Vennemann's Approach to Internal Reconstruction or Proto-Germanic

Lingua Inglese 2A. Sounds, modals, and Variation across gender and age

PEOPLE S KNOWLEDGE OF PHONOLOGICAL UNIVERSALS: EVIDENCE FROM FRICATIVES AND STOPS. A dissertation presented. Tracy Jordan Lennertz

LING 202 Lecture outline W Sept 5. Today s topics: Types of sound change Expressing sound changes Change as misperception

Vowel sets: a reply to Kaye 1

Week 6 - Consonants Mark Huckvale

PHONETIC-INSTRUMENTATION OF BANGLA ASPIRATION: A SPECTROGRAPHIC ANALYSIS.

Washo Possession: A Phonology/Morphology Problem

DU MPhil PhD in Linguistics. Topic:- DU_J18_MPHIL_LING_Topic01. 1) Clicks are common in languages of. [Question ID = 5506]

Phonology. Submission of papers

English Consonants - how can we classify them? Phonetics and Phonology. English Consonants - how can we classify them?

Vowel Sound ɨ close mid unrounded. Vowel Sound ɔ open-mid back rounded. Consonant Sound p. voiceless bilabial plosive

English Phonetics and Phonology. 1. Voiced and voiceless plosives. Voiced and voiceless plosives: Word-initial position

Does Gujarati Stress Avoid the Lowest Sonority Vowel [ə]? Shu-hao Shih Rutgers University

MUSIC PERCEPTION INFLUENCES PLOSIVE PERCEPTION IN WU DIALECTS

Katalin Tamási & Iris Berent

It is all downhill from here: a typological study of the role of Syllable Contact in Romance languages *

Note : Answer all questions.

J ournal. Mishearings of Content Words by ESL Learners 1

Jokes and the Linguistic Mind. Debra Aarons. New York, New York: Routledge Pp. xi +272.

The interaction of tone, sonority, and prosodic structure Paul de Lacy

The Role of Sonority in Blackfoot Phonotactics *

Para-Linguistic Mechanisms of Production in Human Beatboxing : a Real-time Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study

Swedish Adjective Patterns

The Musical Aspects of the Ancient Egyptian Vocalic Language

Barwick, Linda, Birch, Bruce, & Evans, Nicholas. (2007). Iwaidja Jurtbirrk songs: bringing language and music together. Australian Aboriginal

Neural evidence for a single lexicogrammatical processing system. Jennifer Hughes

Berkeley. today, with its emphasis on humanistic research, on breadth, and on expertise

INFORMATION FOR AUTHORS OF GRADUATE THESES (IN ENGLISH) IN THE FIELDS OF ENGLISH LANGUAGE TEACHING, LINGUISTICS, AND APPLIED LINGUISTICS

Organised Phonology Data

[1]. S" = main stress, S = secondary stress, s = unstressed. Proto-Germanic: S s s s s s S s s s s s s S s s. Pintupi: S s S s S s S s S s S s s S s s

PONTIFICIA UNIVERSIDAD CATÓLICA DEL PERÚ - PUCP FIELD SCHOOL PROGRAM IN PERU ETHNOMUSICOLOGY IN CUZCO 2015 SEASON

Organised Phonology Data

Phonetic Aspects of "Speech-Laughs"

GLASOVNI SISTEM ANGLEŠKEGA JEZIKA

Measuring oral and nasal airflow in production of Chinese plosive

Conventions for segmentation

It is all downhill from here: A typological study of the role of Syllable Contact in Romance languages 1

Australian singer, American features: Performing authenticity in country music 1

Paralinguistic mechanisms of production in human beatboxing : A real-time magnetic resonance imaging study

BACHELOR'S DEGREE PROGRAMME Term-End Examination December, 2014

DEGREE IN ENGLISH STUDIES. SUBJECT CONTENTS.

Sonority Contour Preferences in Philippine Languages *

(Received 6 March 2012; revised 30 October 2012; accepted 17 December 2012)

Music Performance Panel: NICI / MMM Position Statement

Advanced Phonetics and Phonology

PSYCHOLOGICAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL EFFECTS ON LAUGHTER SOUND PRODUCTION Marianna De Benedictis Università di Bari

A real time study of plosives in Glaswegian using an automatic measurement algorithm

/s/-stop Blends: Phonetically Consistent Minimal Pairs for Easier Elicitation

Adisa Imamović University of Tuzla

Khoisan. African languages: classification, history, typology Esko Niiranen

BACHELOR'S DEGREE PROGRAMME Term-End Examination CirD-7E3 June, 2018 ELECTIVE COURSE : ENGLISH BEGE-102 : THE STRUCTURE OF MODERN ENGLISH

South American Indians and the Conceptualization of Music

The Old Khotanese Metanalysis

Good Guys Good Names Bad Guys Bad Names?

ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE AND AESTHETICS

WordFinder. Verginica Barbu Mititelu RACAI / 13 Calea 13 Septembrie, Bucharest, Romania

With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long!

Puns Lost in Translation. Contrasting English Puns and Their German Translations in the Television Show "How I Met Your Mother"

Texas Music Education Research

ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL INSIGHTS INTO LANGUAGE AND SPEECH PROCESSING

Controlling Peak Power During Scan Testing

First Step Towards Enhancing Word Embeddings with Pitch Accents for DNN-based Slot Filling on Recognized Text

Acoustic Correlates of Lexical Stress in Central Minnesota English

STYLISTIC ANALYSIS OF MAYA ANGELOU S EQUALITY

Syllabling on instrument imitation: case study and computational segmentation method

IPA NEWS EDITORIAL REPORT: JIPA

Rhetorical Questions and Scales

ENG2163 1st Assignment March 2015 Tarjei Straume ENG2156 HISTORY OF THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE SPRING st Assignment

Organised Phonology Data

Automatic Laughter Detection

Speech and Speaker Recognition for the Command of an Industrial Robot

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

Plosive voicing acoustics and voice quality in Yerevan Armenian

Article-level metrics: a comparison between publishers. Colin Batchelor Data Science Bologna, September 2018

SEGMENTS WITH INHERENTLY FALLING SONORITY*

Organised Phonology Data

288 ~lu~l~c 1,API, to set forth such questions of theoretical or practical character and the answers given to them.

Guide to Preparation of a Manuscript for SinSpeC * Some might need a Second Line. Florian Schäfer, Someone Else and Someone Else

Underlying Word-final Plosives in Caning, a Nilo-Saharan Language of Sudan Timothy M. Stirtz, SIL South Sudan,

Thank you for choosing to publish with Mako: The NSU undergraduate student journal

EPISODE 26: GIVING ADVICE. Giving Advice Here are several language choices for the language function giving advice.

Irony and the Standard Pragmatic Model

GLOSSARY OF TERMS. It may be mostly objective or show some bias. Key details help the reader decide an author s point of view.

Automatic Laughter Detection

MALTESE DIPHONE STATISTICAL ANALYSIS. The Text Corpora

Organised Phonology Data

Estudios de lingüística inglesa aplicada. ELIA Journal GUIDELINES FOR PUBLICATION

Methods of analysis for tonal text-setting. The case study of Fe Fe Bamileke

In the sentence above we find the article "a". It shows us that the speaker does not need a specific chair. He can have any chair.

Language Paper 1 Knowledge Organiser

Similarity matrix for musical themes identification considering sound s pitch and duration

Transcription:

Sonority restricts laryngealized plosives in Southern Aymara CUNY Phonology Forum Conference on Sonority 2016 January 14, 2016 Paola Cépeda & Michael Becker Department of Linguistics, Stony Brook University paola.cepeda@stonybrook.edu

Southern Aymara Aymara (or Jaqi) is an Andean family of languages which includes Southern Aymara, Jaqaru, and Kawki (this one is almost extinct). Currently, Southern Aymara is spoken in Southern Peru, Northern Chile and Western Bolivia. Pacific Ocean FIGURE 1. Southern Aymara current extension (Cépeda 2011) 2

Inventories The vowel and consonant inventory of Southern Aymara appear in the following two tables (Briggs 1993; Cerrón- Palomino 2000; Cépeda 2011). Laryngealized plosives (1) VOWEL INVENTORY OF SOUTHERN AYMARA Front Back High i u Low a (2) CONSONANT INVENTORY OF SOUTHERN AYMARA Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Uvular Plain plosives p t ʧ k q Aspirated plosives pʰ tʰ ʧʰ kʰ qʰ Ejective plosives p' t' ʧ' k' q' Fricatives s x χ Nasals m n ɲ Laterals l ʎ ɾ Glides w j w 3

Previous work: Rule-based approaches Laryngealized plosives are always the first plosive in a root (Landerman 1994). (3) LEFTWARD ORIENTATION a. [ʧʰaqa] to get lost (but *[ʧaqʰa]) c. [lap'a] lice b. [k'uti] flea (but *[kut'i]) d. [nakʰa] to burn Due to a diachronic simplification process (Cerrón-Palomino 2000), only homorganic plosives assimilate the laryngeal feature (Landerman 1994). (4) LARYNGEAL ASSIMILATION IN HOMORGANIC PLOSIVES a. [ʧaʧa] husband b. [kʰakʰa] mute c. [t'ant'a] bread However, peripheral varieties such as Moquegua Aymara show laryngeal assimilation (aspiration only) with heterorganic plosives (Cépeda 2011). (5) ASPIRATION ASSIMILATION IN HETERORGANIC PLOSIVES a. [kʰuʧi]~[kʰuʧʰi] pig b. [tʰaki]~[tʰakʰi] road 4

Previous work: Constraint-based approaches Aspirated and ejective plosives form a class with respect to long VOT only in Quechua and Aymara (Gallagher 2012). Homorganic plosives are allowed to co-occur in a root. There are co-occurrence restrictions for heterorganic segments specified with different laryngeal features (MacEachern 1999). (6) CO-OCCURRENCE RESTRICTIONS a. [t'alpʰa] wide (C Cʰ with initial alveolar, palatal or velar) b. [q'apʰi] fragrance (Uvulars precede labials) When the same laryngeal feature is involved, laryngeal co-occurrence restrictions show a preference for maximizing the perceptual differences contrasting roots with other roots (Gallagher 2010, working on Cochabamba Quechua). Dialects that the ban multiple laryngealized plosives have different constraint rankings from those that do not (Mackenzie 2013). 5

The goal of this talk We propose a theoretical analysis of the distribution of non-initial laryngealized plosives in Southern Aymara that predicts their acceptability in novel roots. The main observation is that laryngealized plosives have a restricted distribution in non-initial position due to a phonotactic constraint involving the sonority of the root-initial segment: (7) GENERALIZATION FOR THE DISTRIBUTION OF NON-INITIAL LARYNGEALIZED PLOSIVES Non-initial laryngealized plosives in Southern Aymara are less common in roots that begin with a plosive or a vowel, and are most frequent in roots that begin with a fricative, a nasal, a liquid or a glide. 6

Roadmap We offer a descriptive account of the distribution of non-initial laryngealized plosives in Büttner & Condori s (1984) dictionary. We propose a stringent constraint family for the sonority hierarchy in root-initial segments, and generate a MaxEnt Grammar model (Wilson 2006). We show that these constraints are not enough to replicate the lexical statistics. We postulate a positional constraint related to the leftward orientation for laryngeal features in Southern Aymara. With this constraint, the model improves its predictions. We compare this model with one generated by the UCLAPL (Hayes & Wilson 2008), whose constraints do not necessarily target the sonority hierarchy. We finally offer the conclusions of this study. 7

The lexicon study FIGURE 2 shows a sonority curve for the distribution of laryngealized plosives in 1,968 disyllabic roots were extracted from Büttner & Condori s (1984) dictionary of Puno Southern Aymara (Peru): Plosive-initial roots (8%) Fricative-initial roots (37%) Nasal-initial roots (34%) Glide-initial roots (24%) Vowel-initial roots (9%) FIGURE 2. Observed percentage of non-initial laryngealized plosives for each root-initial segment 8

Root-initial sonority hierarchy In Southern Aymara, onsetless syllables are largely dispreferred and only appear in root-initial position (Cerrón-Palomino 2000; Cépeda 2011). (8) ROOT-INITIAL SONORITY HIERARCHY Plo > Fri > Nas > Liq > Gli > Vow Categories in a scale such as the sonority one in (8) can be conflated in constraints that stand in a subset relation with respect to their violation marks. That is, they can be expressed in a stringency hierarchy (De Lacy 2003). Accordingly, the corresponding stringent constraint family can be postulated as in (9): (9) STRINGENT CONSTRAINT FAMILY FOR ROOT-INITIAL SONORITY *{VOW}, *{VOW, GLI}, *{VOW, GLI, LIQ}, *{VOW, GLI, LIQ, NAS}, *{VOW, GLI, LIQ, NAS, FRI}, *{VOW, GLI, LIQ, NAS, FRI, PLO} 9

Sonority and non-initial laryngealized plosive The stringent constraint family for root-initial sonority in (9) can combine with the occurrence of a non-initial laryngealized plosive in Southern Aymara, as proposed in (10): (10) STRINGENT CONSTRAINT FAMILY FOR NON-INITIAL LARYNGEALIZED PLOSIVE *{VOW}LAR *{VOW, GLI}LAR *{VOW, GLI, LIQ}LAR *{VOW, GLI, LIQ, NAS}LAR *{VOW, GLI, LIQ, NAS, FRI}LAR *{VOW, GLI, LIQ, NAS, FRI, PLO}LAR 10

Stringent constraints in MaxEnt Grammar Using the MaxEnt Grammar Tool (Wilson 2006), we trained a MaxEnt weighted constraint model on the Southern Aymara lexicon using the stringent constraint family in (10). The model was unable to replicate the curve in FIGURE 2, as shown in FIGURE 3. The acceptability of internal laryngealized plosives did not drop from medium- to lowsonority initial segments. FIGURE 3. Prediction for non-initial laryngealized plosives for the whole lexicon using stringent constraints only 11

Adding a positional constraint The stringent constraint family for the distribution of non-initial laryngealized plosives in (10) is not enough. The low probability for root-initial plosives to co-occur with non-initial laryngealized plosives can be captured by and a restriction on the position of the laryngeal features in a root (the leftward orientation). We adapt a constraint from McCarthy (2003) and postulate the following: (11) COINCIDE Assign one violation mark to every laryngeal features not occurring on the leftmost plosive. 12

Stringent constraints + COINCIDE We trained a new MaxEnt weighted constraint model on the Southern Aymara lexicon using the stringent constraint family in (10) and the positional constraint in (11). The predictions of this analysis are shown in FIGURE 4. The model matches the lexical statistics very closely and generates the expected sonority curve. FIGURE 4. Prediction for non-initial laryngealized plosives for the whole lexicon using stringent constraints + COINCIDE 13

A UCLAPL model We also used the UCLA Phonotactic Learner (Hayes & Wilson 2008) and trained a phonotactic model on the Southern Aymara lexicon and a testing list of 16,866 real and nonce disyllabic roots. Reporting on the difference in probability between roots with laryngealized and non-laryngealized consonants, non-initial laryngealized plosives are predicted to be less probable in roots that begin with a plosive (-9%), a fricative (-7%) or a vowel (-7%). The UCLAPL predictions can generate the sonority curve but not as precisely as the model we proposed in FIGURE 4. 14

Conclusions We have identified a previously unknown generalization on the distribution of non-initial laryngealized plosives in Southern Aymara roots: (12) GENERALIZATION Laryngealized plosives are restricted with both low- and high-sonority initial segments, and are more freely attested with medium sonority initial segments. By using stringent constraints for the sonority hierarchy and a restriction on the position of the laryngeal features in a root, we have offered an analysis that predicts the acceptability of non-initial laryngealized plosives in novel roots. We are currently preparing to test these predictions with a nonce word rating task with native Southern Aymara speakers in Puno (Peru). 15

References Büttner, T. & D. Condori (1984) Diccionario aymara-castellano. Arunakan liwru: aymara-kastillanu. Puno: Proyecto Experimental de Educación Bilingüe. Cépeda, P. (2011) Elementos para la descripción gramatical del aimara de Moquegua. Doctoral Dissertation, Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú. Cerrón-Palomino, R. 2000. Lingüística aimara. Cuzco: Centro Bartolomé de las Casas. De Lacy, P. (2003) Conflation and Hierarchies. In Astruc, L. & M. Richards (Eds.) Cambridge Occasional Papers in Linguistics 1. Cambridge: CUP, 83-100. Gallagher, G. (2012) Perceptual similarity in non-local laryngeal restrictions. Lingua 122, 112 124. Gallagher, G. (2010) Perceptual distinctness and long distance laryngeal restrictions. Phonology 27, 435-480. Hayes, B. & C. Wilson (2008) A Maximum Entropy Model of Phonotactics and Phonotactic Learning. Linguistic Inquiry 39(3), 379-440. Landerman, P. (1994) Glottalization and Aspiration in Quechua and Aymara Reconsidered. In Cole, P., G. Hermon & M.D. Martín (Eds.) Language in the Andes. Newark: University of Delaware, 332-378. MacEachern, M.R. (1999) Laryngeal Cooccurrence Restrictions. New York: Garland. Mackenzie, S. (2013) Laryngeal co-occurrence restrictions in Aymara. Phonology 30(2), 297-345. McCarthy, J. (2003) OT constraints are categorical. Phonology 20(1), 75-138. Wilson, C. (2006) Learning phonology with substantive bias: an experimental and computational study of velar palatalization. Cognitive Science 30(5), 945-982. 16

Thank you! Yusulupay! paola.cepeda@stonybrook.edu 17