On the strike note of bells

Similar documents
DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Musical Acoustics Lecture 16 Interval, Scales, Tuning and Temperament - I

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

Simple Harmonic Motion: What is a Sound Spectrum?

POST-PROCESSING FIDDLE : A REAL-TIME MULTI-PITCH TRACKING TECHNIQUE USING HARMONIC PARTIAL SUBTRACTION FOR USE WITHIN LIVE PERFORMANCE SYSTEMS

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Lecture 1: What we hear when we hear music

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Author Index. Absolu, Brandt 165. Montecchio, Nicola 187 Mukherjee, Bhaswati 285 Müllensiefen, Daniel 365. Bay, Mert 93

Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords

Beethoven s Fifth Sine -phony: the science of harmony and discord

The Pythagorean Scale and Just Intonation

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Note on Posted Slides. Noise and Music. Noise and Music. Pitch. PHY205H1S Physics of Everyday Life Class 15: Musical Sounds

Javanese Gong Wave Signals

AN INTRODUCTION TO MUSIC THEORY Revision A. By Tom Irvine July 4, 2002

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

NOVEL DESIGNER PLASTIC TRUMPET BELLS FOR BRASS INSTRUMENTS: EXPERIMENTAL COMPARISONS

NON-LINEAR EFFECTS MODELING FOR POLYPHONIC PIANO TRANSCRIPTION

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Pitch correction on the human voice

The Composer s Materials

Lecture 7: Music

Augmentation Matrix: A Music System Derived from the Proportions of the Harmonic Series

EE391 Special Report (Spring 2005) Automatic Chord Recognition Using A Summary Autocorrelation Function

Physics and Neurophysiology of Hearing

An Effective Filtering Algorithm to Mitigate Transient Decaying DC Offset

OBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF A MELODY EXTRACTOR FOR NORTH INDIAN CLASSICAL VOCAL PERFORMANCES

Vocal-tract Influence in Trombone Performance

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

CTP 431 Music and Audio Computing. Basic Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology (GSCT) Juhan Nam

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

Determination of Sound Quality of Refrigerant Compressors

Appendix A Types of Recorded Chords

3b- Practical acoustics for woodwinds: sound research and pitch measurements

Available online at International Journal of Current Research Vol. 9, Issue, 08, pp , August, 2017

Welcome to Vibrationdata

2018 Fall CTP431: Music and Audio Computing Fundamentals of Musical Acoustics

PHY 103: Scales and Musical Temperament. Segev BenZvi Department of Physics and Astronomy University of Rochester

1 Ver.mob Brief guide

6.5 Percussion scalograms and musical rhythm

The characterisation of Musical Instruments by means of Intensity of Acoustic Radiation (IAR)

COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF BELL SOUNDS FROM SEVERAL ROMANIAN ORTHODOX MONASTERIES AND CHURCHES

Is Your Piano Out of Tune?

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Interplay between musical practices and tuning in the marimba de chonta music Jorge E. Useche 1, Rafael G. Hurtado 1,* and Federico Demmer 2.

ADVANCED PROCEDURES FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC NOISE EVALUATION

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

LESSON 1 PITCH NOTATION AND INTERVALS

Study Guide. Solutions to Selected Exercises. Foundations of Music and Musicianship with CD-ROM. 2nd Edition. David Damschroder

arxiv: v1 [physics.class-ph] 22 Mar 2012

Music Source Separation

Musical Sound: A Mathematical Approach to Timbre

The perception of concurrent sound objects through the use of harmonic enhancement: a study of auditory attention

Melodic Minor Scale Jazz Studies: Introduction

Topics in Computer Music Instrument Identification. Ioanna Karydi

CONSONANCE AND DISSONANCE 4.2. Simple integer ratios Why is it that two notes an octave apart sound consonant, while two notes a little more or

Well temperament revisited: two tunings for two keyboards a quartertone apart in extended JI

Music Information Retrieval with Temporal Features and Timbre

Organ Tuner - ver 2.1

WIND INSTRUMENTS. Math Concepts. Key Terms. Objectives. Math in the Middle... of Music. Video Fieldtrips

The Composer s Materials

The Effect of Time-Domain Interpolation on Response Spectral Calculations. David M. Boore

The Scale of Musical Instruments

Automatic Rhythmic Notation from Single Voice Audio Sources

Music Representations

Harmonic Series II: Harmonics, Intervals, and Instruments *

Music Theory. Fine Arts Curriculum Framework. Revised 2008

Semi-automated extraction of expressive performance information from acoustic recordings of piano music. Andrew Earis

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

A Psychoacoustically Motivated Technique for the Automatic Transcription of Chords from Musical Audio

Music Radar: A Web-based Query by Humming System

Instrument Recognition in Polyphonic Mixtures Using Spectral Envelopes

Physics and Music PHY103

Progress in calculating tonality of technical sounds

Different aspects of MAthematics

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

Musical Signal Processing with LabVIEW Introduction to Audio and Musical Signals. By: Ed Doering

Federation Bells Composer s Guide - Monday, July 01, Federation Bells Composers Manual

EFFECTS OF REVERBERATION TIME AND SOUND SOURCE CHARACTERISTIC TO AUDITORY LOCALIZATION IN AN INDOOR SOUND FIELD. Chiung Yao Chen

Music Genre Classification and Variance Comparison on Number of Genres

Preferred acoustical conditions for musicians on stage with orchestra shell in multi-purpose halls

Visual and Aural: Visualization of Harmony in Music with Colour. Bojan Klemenc, Peter Ciuha, Lovro Šubelj and Marko Bajec

Robert Alexandru Dobre, Cristian Negrescu

Noise. CHEM 411L Instrumental Analysis Laboratory Revision 2.0

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

Exploring the Rules in Species Counterpoint

Harmonic Generation based on Harmonicity Weightings

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Intonation preferences for major thirds with non-beating ensemble sounds

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

Does Saxophone Mouthpiece Material Matter? Introduction

Automatic music transcription

Syllabus: PHYS 1300 Introduction to Musical Acoustics Fall 20XX

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Registration Reference Book

Transcription:

Loughborough University Institutional Repository On the strike note of bells This item was submitted to Loughborough University's Institutional Repository by the/an author. Citation: SWALLOWE and PERRIN, 2001. On the strike note of bells. IN: Proceedings on the International Symposium on Musical Acoustics, Davide Bonsi, Diego Gonzalez, Domenico Stanzial, Perugia, Italy pp 367-370 Additional Information: This is a refereed conference paper. Metadata Record: https://dspace.lboro.ac.uk/2134/2450 Please cite the published version.

ON THE STRIKE NOTE OF BELLS G. M. Swallowe*, R. Perrin +, Department of Physics, Loughborough University, Leicestershire, LE11 3TU, UK + Also at the Institute of Fundamental Sciences, Massey University, Palmerston North, NZ *G.M.Swallowe@lboro.ac.uk Abstract A strike note, characteristic of the particular bell, is heard when a bell is struck. It is observed that the pitch of this note sometimes does not correspond to the frequency of any one of the bell s normal modes. The origin of this strike note has been a subject of controversy for over 100 years. Previous empirical investigations have mainly made use of musically trained listeners and real or recorded bell sounds. The short duration of the actual strike note and possible influence of musical training on observations may invalidate the conclusions of previous investigators. In this work use has been made of computer generated simulated bell sounds and untrained listeners. It is demonstrated that a strike note may be isolated by beating with a pure test tone and this technique is used to investigate 28 bell-like sounds using a total of 60 listeners. It is concluded that virtual pitch theory provides the best method of predicting the presence or absence of a strike note and its frequency, but that it does not work in every case. INTRODUCTION When a bell is struck the sound emitted consists of a number of simultaneously produced single frequency components or partial tones generated by the excitation of normal modes of the particular bell. The normal modes of a bell do not in general form a harmonic series although the five lowest, and most prominent, partials are tuned by bellfounders for the production of large church and carillon bells. Higher partials are not usually tuned. After a bell is struck the first sound heard is an inharmonic one of metal striking on metal. This is due to high frequency partials and rapidly dies away leaving an audible strike note of definite musical pitch. This in turn decays, giving way to the more slowly decaying Hum tone. A strike note is heard whether the bell is tuned or not. Its musical pitch sometimes does not correspond to the frequency of any one of the bell s normal modes, especially in untuned cases, and its origin has been a subject of controversy for many years. Lord Rayleigh [1] concluded that the strike note was at the frequency of the octave below the fifth partial tone (Nominal) in both tuned and untuned bells. In a tuned bell this corresponds to the Fundamental but an untuned one often has no mode at this frequency. Work by Jones [2] on a number of American carillons generally supported Rayleigh s conclusion but also concluded that the difference tone between the fifth and seventh partials may be of importance. Meyer and Klaes [3] showed that the strike note was not physically present in the sound produced by the bell but corresponded to a frequency close to the difference between the fifth and seventh partials and attributed this to a physiological effect in the ear. Arts [4], who carried out measurements on dozens of bells, concluded that an octave below the fifth partial corresponded much more closely to the strike note than did the difference between fifth and seventh partials. Later work by Schouten and t Hart [5] and Pfundner [6] attributes the strike note to the residue ; a tone generated by the perception of a number of frequencies in harmonic or close to harmonic relationship whose combination produces the perception of a fundamental frequency which is not physically present. Rossing [7] applied this theory to chimes, in which the strike note phenomenon is also observed, and concluded that the residue theory provided a satisfactory explanation for the strike notes. Schad and Frik [8] attribute the strike note to the combination of missing fundamentals of several harmonic series in the bell s physical frequency spectrum. However, this work is essentially theoretical and does not report on actual listener observations of strike note frequencies. Terhardt [9] has developed a version of the theory of virtual pitch and used it to attempt to explain the strike note (Terhardt and Seewann [10]). It leads to the prediction of one or more virtual pitches from a complex tonal sound which may or may not correspond to

physically present frequencies. The experiments described below seek to make definite measurements of strike note frequencies and hence differentiate between the competing theories of the strike note. EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES In previous work most of the observers were musically trained so that their perception may well have been influenced by the tradition of musical harmonic scales and they may therefore not be considered as objective observers. This work revisits the strike note phenomenon using computer and oscillator generated tones, which could essentially generate a strike note of indefinite duration, and uses non musically trained listeners to determine the existence and frequency of the strike note. Two sets of experiments carried out using different sets of listeners are described. These are classified as Series 1 and Series 2. The volunteer listeners were mainly students of ages 18-22 but did also include approximately 20% of older volunteers spanning the age range 28-65. Table 1: Frequencies (Hz) and frequency ratios relative to the strike note of bell-like sounds derived from the modified Chladni Law. Conventional partial numbers are listed in the first row. The observed strike notes listed in the final column have an accuracy of ±3 Hz 1 st 3 rd 4 th 5 th 7 th 8 th Sound Frequency (Hz) and frequency ratios (in brackets) relative to strike note Strike note 1 103 (0.52) 249 (1.25) 297 (1.49) 399 (2.00) 597 (2.99) 816 (4.08) 200 2 87 (0.44) 235 (1.19) 282 (1.43) 384 (1.95) 626 (3.18) 765 (3.88) 197 3 90 202 275 455 562 809 4 141 (0.47) 373 (1.24) 453 (1.51) 597 (1.99) 915 (3.05) 1197 (3.99) 300 5 154 (0.51) 384 (1.27) 440 (1.46) 626 (2.07) 915 (3.03) 1161 (3.84) 302 6 141 360 455 562 950 1265 7 204 (0.51) 481 (1.19) 597 (1.48) 816 (2.02) 1197 (2.97) 1614 (4.00) 403 8 193 (0.49) 499 (1.26) 626 (1.58) 765 (1.93) 1161 (2.93) 1621 (4.09) 396 9 202 (0.50) 455 (1.13) 562 (1.39) 809 (2.00) 1265 (3.13) 1624 (4.02) 404 10 249 (0.50) 597 (1.20) 751 (1.51) 1018 (2.04) 1497 (3.00) 1982 (3.97) 499 11 235 (0.46) 626 (1.23) 765 (1.50) 994 (1.95) 1524 (2.99) 2034 (3.99) 510 12 275 562 809 950 1439 2029 13 297 (0.49) 719 (1.19) 915 (1.52) 1197 (1.99) 1816 (3.01) 2415 (4.00) 603 14 282 (0.48) 694 (1.18) 915 (1.55) 1161 (1.97) 1823 (3.09) 2369 (4.02) 590 15 275 680 950 1265 1821 2478 16 347 (0.50) 848 (1.22) 1052 (1.51) 1381 (1.99) 2109 (3.03) 2778 (4.00) 695 17 331 (0.46) 839 (1.18) 1076 (1.51) 1430 (2.00) 2144 (3.00) 2848 (3.99) 714 18 360 (0.52) 809 (1.17) 1102 (1.60) 1439 (2.09) 2029 (2.94) 2720 (3.94) 690 Series 1 Previous work (Swallowe et al [11], Perrin et al [12]) has shown that the frequency spectra of bell-like sounds can be approximately described by a modified Chladni law of the form f bn) m. n = c( m + p where m and n are non negative integers while b, c and p are constants such that 1 b 2 and 1.4 p 2.4. Eighteen groups of six partials which approximated to frequency ratios in a bell were produced using this formula. The frequencies and frequency ratios of these tones are set out in Table 1. The partials were of equal intensity. A combination of an AMIGA 500 computer (which can generate 4 simultaneous tones) and two Bruel and Kjaer type 1022 signal generators were used to produce the tones. They were mixed in an audio mixer and fed to speakers via an audio amplifier. Since the strike note is observed in the region of the second partial this frequency was not produced and the 6 frequencies in the sounds can be approximated to 1 st, 3 rd, 4 th, 5 th, 7 th, and 8 th partials of a conventional bell enabling both the octave below the fifth and the difference tone between 5 th and 7 th to be investigated. The conventional 6 th partial was not used because it is relatively unimportant in the sound

of a conventional bell (Schad and Warlimont [13]). All of the eighteen groups investigated produced pleasant bell-like sounds. An additional test frequency produced by a further Bruel and Kjaer type 1022 oscillator was fed into the sound mixer together with the tones listed in Table 1 and an observer in a sound proof booth scanned the test frequency between 100 Hz and the upper frequency of the sound under investigation, and listened for beats. When beats were found the frequency was noted. All of the tones present in the sounds were readily identified by prominent beats with the test tone. Weak but audible beats due to difference tones between most of the pairs on tones in each sound were also detected. In addition to these, other beats of a different character, could be prominently heard in 14 of the 18 sounds and they are indicated as strike notes in Table 1. A total of 12 volunteer listeners took part in these tests and all but 2 could identify the strike note. The data showed that the strike note is different in character and much more easily observed than difference tones. Difference tones were weak, occured in all the sounds and required only the presence of the two relevant tones to be detected. The strike note was not consistently found at either the Rayleigh criterion frequency of an octave below the fifth or close to the difference tone between the fifth and seventh of a conventional bell. Removal of any of the other frequencies either diminished or removed the strike note. The strike note appears therefore to be produced by the totality of the physically present tones. Series 2 The Series 1 experiment used theoretically predicted bell-like sounds generated from 6 physical frequencies. It could be argued that the strike note phenomenon produced was artificial in that it was not generated from a set of partial frequencies and amplitudes present in a real bell. In this set of experiments two AMIGA computers were used to generate a total of 8 tones (4 from each computer) which were mixed and fed to a speaker via an audio amplifier. The output amplitude at each frequency was adjusted so that it contributed a sound intensity approximately equal to the measured relative intensities of the bells in Schad and Warlimont [13] whose published values of the frequencies of ideal and experimentally determined bell partials were used to simulate 10 bells as listed in Table 2. The simulated bell sounds were played to ten volunteer listeners. A Bruel and Kjaer type 1022 oscillator connected to another speaker was provided to the volunteers and they were asked to identify all the tones that they could find in the sound by slowly scanning in the frequency range ±100 Hz around the octave below the fifth partial and listening for beats. The listeners found all the frequencies in the range tested that were physically present in the sound. In addition some observers found an additional frequency in the expected region of the strike note and this is recorded in Table 2 as the strike note. Where no additional frequency was observed the 2 nd partial is recorded, in brackets in Table 2, as the strike note. All of the simulated bell sounds were fed to a PC running a Fourier analysis programme. This, as expected, identified all the partials in the sounds and showed that none of the additional frequencies recorded as a strike note were physically present in the sounds. Table 2: Partial frequencies and strike note of simulated bells. Partial Frequencies (Hz) Strike Bell First Second Third Fourth Fifth Sixth Seventh Eighth 1 293 586 693 883 1172 1200 1394 1470 (585) 2 145 290 360 432 580 690 766 854 (290) 3 102 202 240 323 403 479 538 603 (201) 4 208 392 494 623 832 989 1111 1245 416 5 220 365 462 660 779 1093 1173 1296 390 6 135 262 343 392 535 548 637 671 268 7 127 254 302 381 508 640 678 761 (253) 8 142 284 338 425 568 716 758 851 (284) 9 266 543 643 856 1142 1322 1418 1500 571 10 276 531 647 856 1134 1314 1410 1471 568 DISCUSSION Sounds 1,4,7,9,10,11,13,16,17 of Series 1 have two or more harmonics of the reported strike note in their spectrum so the strike note can be explained as a missing fundamental. Additionally sounds 5,8,14,and 18 have two or more frequency components whose ratios to the recorded strike are close to integer ratios and could

therefore be explained as beats of mistuned consonances. However, sounds 6,12 and 15 also have such ratios but no strike note was observed which suggests that beats of mistuned consonances cannot offer an explanation for the strike note. Bells 1,2,3,7 and 8 of Series 2 have a second partial which coincides with an octave below the 5 th and therefore the strike note is expected to be at the same frequency as the second partial and thus not be independently detectable. In bells 4 and 5 the octave below the 5 th differs significantly from the second partial but second and third harmonics of this frequency are present in the physical spectrum so strike notes at this frequency are expected as a missing fundamental. In bells 9 and 10 the octave below the 5 th again differs greatly from the second partial but only the second harmonic of this frequency is physically present so it cannot correspond to a missing fundamental. The concept of the strike note being due to the residue of higher frequencies is not supported by these results. Only the lower partials of simulated bells were used in this study but the strike note was clearly perceived in the expected region. Pfundner s [6] conclusion of the strike note being due to a missing fundamental of frequencies starting with the fifth or higher partials therefore cannot be correct. Schlad and Frik s [8] concept of a strike note being due to a combination of several missing fundamentals also does not explain the results of these experiments. An application of a simplified form of Terhardt s virtual pitch theory [9] based on matching subharmonics to the sounds and taking into account the most significant virtual pitches leads to the correct prediction of the strike note, or lack of one, in 15 of the 18 sounds of Series 1 and 7 of the 10 sounds of Series 2. In addition it predicts strike notes in bells 3 and 6 of Series 2 where none was found. However, weak beating was heard at these predicted frequencies so the theory could be said to have worked. In bells 9 and 10 of Series 2 this approach failed to predict the strong strike notes heard. It also predicted a strike note for sound 12 of Series 2 but none was heard. The virtual pitch theory could be said to have worked in 86% of the tests. The possibility that the unpredicted strike notes were due to a superposition of several difference tones at almost the same frequency was considered. A calculation of the possible difference tones note of Series 2 bells 9 and 10 shows that only one difference tone has a frequency close to the observed strike of bell 9 and none of bell 10. Superposition of several difference tones does not therefore offer an explanation for these strike notes. CONCLUSIONS The results of these investigations can be summarised as a) Strike notes can be detected by beating with a test frequency. b) A strike note can be produced even in the absence of overtones to generate a missing fundamental. c) The Rayleigh rule provides a good estimate of the frequency of a strike note, when one exists, but cannot predict the absence of one. d) Virtual pitch theory is the best method of predicting the presence or absence of a strike note as well as its frequency. However, it does not work in every case. REFERENCES [1] Rayleigh, Lord On bells, Phil. Mag., 29, pp. 1-17, 1890 [2] Jones, A. T., The Strike Note of Bells, J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 1, pp. 373-381, 1930 [3] Meyer, E. and J. Klaes, J. On the Strike note of Bells, Naturwissenschaften, 39, pp. 697-701, 1933 [4] Arts, J. The Sound of Bells. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 9, pp. 344-347, 1938 [5] Schouten, J. F. and t Hart, J. The Strike Note of Bells, Neth. Acoust Soc. Pub. No. 7, pp. 8-19, 1965 [6] Pfundner, J. On the Strike Note of Bells, Acustica, 12, pp. 153-157, 1962 [7] Rossing, T. D. Acoustics of Percussion Instruments, Phys. Teach., 14, pp. 546-556, 1976 [8] Schad, C.R. and Frik, G. On the Strike Note of Bells, Acustica, 80, pp. 232-237, 1994 [9] Terhardt, E. Calculating Virtual Pitch, Hearing Research, 1, pp. 155-182, 1979 [10] Terhardt, E. and Seewann, M. Aural and Objective determination of the Strike Note of historical Church Bells, Acustica, 54, pp. 129, 1984 [11] Swallowe, G.M., Charnley T.,and Perrin, R. New Musical Scales, J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 94, pp. 1166-1167, 1993

[12] Perrin, R., Charnley, T., Banu H., and Rossing T.D., Chladni s law and the Modern English Church Bell, J. Sound Vib., 102, pp. 11-19, 1985. [13] Schad, C.R. and Warlimont, H. Acoustical Investigations of the influence of the Material on the Sound of Bells, Acustica, 29, pp. 1-14, 1973.