Musical learning and cognitive performance

Similar documents
INFORMATION AFTERNOON. TUESDAY 16 OCTOBER 4pm to 6pm JAC Lecture Theatre

Composing with Hyperscore in general music classes: An exploratory study

Practicing a Musical Instrument in Childhood is Associated with Enhanced Verbal Ability and Nonverbal Reasoning

Graduate Bulletin PSYCHOLOGY

18 Benefits of Playing a Musical Instrument

EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY (ED PSY)

Becoming an expert in the musical domain: It takes more than just practice

6 th Grade Instrumental Music Curriculum Essentials Document

Is composition a mode of performing? Questioning musical meaning

DOWNLOAD OR READ : STUDIES IN MUSICAL EDUCATION HISTORY AND AESTHETICS PAPERS AND PROCEEDINGS SCHOLARS CHOICE EDITION PDF EBOOK EPUB MOBI

Most students have their first year of band in the 6 th grade, and move on to a performing band for 7 th and 8 th grade.

Chords not required: Incorporating horizontal and vertical aspects independently in a computer improvisation algorithm

GROWING UP IN A PHILHARMONIC BAND a cultural perspective 1

The power of music: its impact on the intellectual, social and personal development of children and young people

VALLEY CENTRAL HIGH SCHOOL

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY

The effects of music listening and learning: cognitive transfer, well being and education

PSYCHOLOGY (PSY) - COURSES Fall 2018 Spring 2019

Bachelor of Arts in Psychology

Chapter Five: The Elements of Music

The influence of performers stage entrance behavior on the audience s performance elaboration

EFFECTS OF ORFF-SCHULWERK PROCESS OF IMITATION ON ELEMENTARY STUDENTS READING FLUENCY

Psychology. Department Location Giles Hall Room 320

Validity. What Is It? Types We Will Discuss. The degree to which an inference from a test score is appropriate or meaningful.

The Mathematics of Music and the Statistical Implications of Exposure to Music on High. Achieving Teens. Kelsey Mongeau

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

REQUIREMENTS FOR MASTER OF SCIENCE DEGREE IN APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY CLINICAL/COUNSELING PSYCHOLOGY

The role of the Alexander technique in musical training and performing

Psychology PSY 312 BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR. (3)

Music Education (MUED)

College of MUSIC. James Forger, DEAN UNDERGRADUATE PROGRAMS. Admission as a Junior to the College of Music

A sensitive period for musical training: contributions of age of onset and cognitive abilities

Primary Music Objectives (Prepared by Sheila Linville and Julie Troum)

Annotation and the coordination of cognitive processes in Western Art Music performance

Evaluation of Music Education: Musical Competences and Selfconfidence

PSYCHOLOGY (PSY) Psychology (PSY) 1

MLA Header with Page Number Bond 1. This article states that learning to play a musical instrument increases neuroplasticity and

Evaluation of Children's Responses to Literature

Weekly Assignment 1 Creativity Esperanza Muino Florida International University Spring, 2016

The role of texture and musicians interpretation in understanding atonal music: Two behavioral studies

Hearing Loss and Sarcasm: The Problem is Conceptual NOT Perceptual

MENC: The National Association for Music Education

PSYCHOLOGY APPLICATION DEADLINES

River Dell Regional School District. Visual and Performing Arts Curriculum Music

Copyright 2006 The Authors. Deposited on: 09 October 2013

Vuzik: Music Visualization and Creation on an Interactive Surface

The Nature of Children's Singing Voices: Characteristics and Assessment

West Windsor-Plainsboro Regional School District String Orchestra Grade 9

Effects of Musical Tempo on Heart Rate, Brain Activity, and Short-term Memory Abstract

How to explain the process of creating a musical interpretation: The development of a methodology

CAMELSDALE PRIMARY SCHOOL MUSIC POLICY

Instrumental Music Curriculum

PSYCHOLOGY (PSY) - COURSES Fall 2017

The Power of Listening

Memory and learning: experiment on Sonata KV 331, in A Major by W. A. Mozart

Metaphorical cognitive feedback: Rethinking musical emotion communication enhancement

Metaphors: Concept-Family in Context

Learning to Teach the New National Curriculum for Music

Master of Arts in Psychology Program The Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences offers the Master of Arts degree in Psychology.

Music Therapy at l école secondaire Joseph-Charbonneau

The effect of focused instruction on young children s singing accuracy

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

MUSIC EDUCATION AND MUSIC THERAPY (MED) & (MTY)

Music Education & Music Therapy

Jenks. Course Planning Information and Enrollment Sheet. West Intermediate

Indiana Music Standards

You can log in and test with password: teacher

Walworth Primary School

Finger motion in piano performance: Touch and tempo

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RHYTHM APTITUDE AND RHYTHM ACHIEVEMENT OF FIRST, SECOND, AND THIRD GRADE STUDENTS. Kelly Kristine Harding

College of MUSIC. James Forger, DEAN UNDERGRADUATE PROGRAMS. Admission as a Junior to the College of Music

Central Valley School District Music 1 st Grade August September Standards August September Standards

Zooming into saxophone performance: Tongue and finger coordination

Music Education Choral C50206BM Voice, Keyboard or Guitar

SYMPOSIA: MUSICAL TRAINING FOR CHILDREN

An exploration of the pianist s multiple roles within the duo chamber ensemble

Psychology. 526 Psychology. Faculty and Offices. Degree Awarded. A.A. Degree: Psychology. Program Student Learning Outcomes

THE CATHOLIC UNIVERSITY OF AMERICA

Arts Education Essential Standards Crosswalk: MUSIC A Document to Assist With the Transition From the 2005 Standard Course of Study

ARTICULATION AGREEMENT FORM

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

B.M. IN MUSIC EDUCATION WITH MINOR IN EDUCATION

Music. Colorado Academic

Multidimensional analysis of interdependence in a string quartet

13. ENHANCING MUSIC LISTENING IN EDUCATIONAL CONTEXT

Maintaining skill across the life span: Magaloff s entire Chopin at age 77

UCSB LIBRARY COLLECTION SPACE PLANNING INITIATIVE: REPORT ON THE UCSB LIBRARY COLLECTIONS SURVEY OUTCOMES AND PLANNING STRATEGIES

Visual Arts Curriculum Framework

EASTERN ARIZONA COLLEGE Elementary Theory

CORRELATIONS BETWEEN THE ACQUIRED KNOWLEDGE AND PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS IN CLASS TEACHING ON THE SUBJECT OF MUSIC EDUCATION

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A.

1. The Basic Elements of Music. 2. Ragtime. 3. Jazz. 4. Musical Theater. 5. Rock. 6. Folk Music. II. Course Learning Outcomes Course Learning Outcome

A STUDY OF AMERICAN NEWSPAPER READABILITY

Praxis Music: Content Knowledge (5113) Study Plan Description of content

Making Connections Through Music

GRIFFITHSTOWN PRIMARY SCHOOL

Can parents influence children s music preferences and positively shape their development? Dr Hauke Egermann

The roles of expertise and partnership in collaborative rehearsal

Logic and Philosophy of Science (LPS)

Transcription:

International Symposium on Performance Science ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 The Author 2009, Published by the AEC All rights reserved Musical learning and cognitive performance Carlos Santos-Luiz 1, Daniela Coimbra 2,3, and Carlos Fernandes da Silva 4 1 College of Education, Polytechnic Institute of Coimbra, Portugal 2 Superior School of Music, Porto Polytechnic Institute, Portugal 3 School of the Arts, Portuguese Catholic University, Portugal 4 Department of Educational Sciences, University of Aveiro, Portugal According to literature, there are broad associations between music and cognitive abilities, which apparently result from the frequency of music lessons. The aim of the present work was to test these associations between a group of music students and a control group of students with no music education, thereby contributing toward understanding the link between music learning, intelligence, and academic achievement. The Battery of Reasoning Tests (BPR/7-9), collated for the Portuguese population, as well as the academic classifications of two groups of students, provided the basis for this study s quantitative analysis. The sample population comprised students who frequented Year 7 of the basic music course in specialist music education and of the basic course in mainstream education (no music lessons). Compared with the students in the control group, those in the groups which included formal music education showed increases in their general capacity of reasoning (g factor) and in tasks of spatial and numerical reasoning. Equally, there were increases in academic achievement. The results suggest that the duration of exposure to music lessons is associated with an increased intelligence and increased academic achievement. Keywords: specialist music education; mainstream education; general capacity of reasoning; academic achievement; cognitive skills It has been suggested that music may be more than just an art form, and that it may indeed be able to produce benefits in other domains, such as cognitive abilities or general intelligence. According to Schellenberg (2005, 2006a), different lines of research have shown that both passive listening to music and formal musical training reveal such a connection. However, the differ-

350 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG ences between these two activities make it unlikely that both could have similar effects upon the non-musical aspects of human behavior (Schellenberg 2006a). This study focuses upon the hypothesis that music lessons could provide benefits in non-musical areas of cognition. Of the musical activities that appear to improve cognitive performance, the most significant appear to be the playing of a musical instrument (particularly keyboard instruments), vocal training, musical learning in general, and rhythmic training. The underlying transfer effect (Barnett and Ceci 2002) may prove to be unique in children that have received music lessons for prolonged periods; as such, lessons involve activities such as: Focused attention, regular practice, reading music, memorizing extended passages and entire pieces, learning about rules of pattern formation that define western musical structures, incremental improvement of fine motor skills, and learning to express emotions through music (Schellenberg 2006b, p. 466). The combination of these activities with genetic predisposition and environmental factors may play an important role, not only in music but also in other areas of cognition (Barbro 2006), particularly during childhood when cerebral development is still very flexible (Huttenlocher 2002). Some researchers believe that music lessons are associated to specific sub-areas of intellectual skills. These include spatial skills (Bilhartz et al. 2000, Rauscher et al. 2006), reading (Lamb and Gregory 1993, Butzlaff 2000), subcomponents of literacy such as vocabulary (Orsmond and Miller 1999) and verbal memory (Ho et al. 2003), mathematics (Vaughn 2000, Maureen 2008), and mathematical subcomponents such as arithmetic (Rauscher et al. 2006) and geometry (La Mont 2008). According to Schellenberg (2004) the most parsimonious explanation of these diffuse associations is that they stem from a common component, such as general intelligence (Schellenberg 2004, p. 511). Schellenberg (2004, 2006a) also claims that music improves general intelligence. Increased intelligence and enhanced performance in subtests for academic competence were found in music students. There is also evidence that the duration of musical training is positively associated to intelligence, and that this association is general (i.e. not limited to a specific subgroup of intellectual skills) (Schellenberg 2006b). The research described in this paper examines associations between music lessons and general capacity of reasoning (the so-called g factor ), and between the duration of the musical training and the increase in the general capacity of reasoning. The performance of music students in each reasoning

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 351 task (abstract, numerical, verbal, mechanical, and spatial) was compared with that of students who had not had music lessons. The general academic performance of the students was also taken into account. Participants METHOD The sample population (n=134) consisted of students from the seventh grade of the basic music course (specialized musical education program), and others undergoing conventional education. The average age was 13 years. The students were divided into three groups, two experimental and one control: (1) students receiving specialized musical education, who had had between 5-7 years of musical training (n=63, SME 5-7 ), (2) students from specialized musical education with 3-4 years of musical training (n=21, SME 3-4 ). The control group (n=50) consisted of students undergoing conventional schooling ( CS ), which did not include music lessons. All students were from a similar socioeconomic and cultural background. Materials The data used in this study was obtained using a Battery of Reasoning Tests, BPR/7-9 (Almeida and Lemos 2006), designed to assess general capacity of reasoning (g factor). These tests, which had been specially adapted to the Portuguese population, included five instruments to evaluate performance in different tasks of reasoning: abstract, numerical, verbal, mechanical, spatial. Procedure The participants were recruited from ten schools around Portugal by means of an invitation addressed to their parents or guardians. The pupils were tested individually using the BPR/7-9 at the beginning of the school year. Statistical tests were then performed to gauge differences between groups. RESULTS Comparing the average g factor scores, it was found that the SME group with 5-7 years of musical education had the highest average, followed by the SME group with 3-4 years of musical education (see Figure 1). Sheffé s post hoc tests showed that only the differences between the groups CS and SME 5-7 were significant (mean difference=-2.12, SE=0.493, p=0.000). In short, the students that had musical education displayed a higher general capacity

352 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG 16 12 Average 8 4 0 CS without SME 3-4 yrs SME 5-7 yrs musical training musical training musical training Figure 1. Mean general capacity of reasoning for the CS, SME 3-4, and SME 5-7 groups. of reasoning than the students from the control group, a difference which became more pronounced with the duration of musical training. Using MANOVA, the performances of the three groups was compared with relation to each task. Scheffé tests were used to assess the differences between the various tests in the three groups. Although the students from the SME 5-7 and SME 3-4 groups scored better than those from the CS group in abstract and verbal reasoning and those from the SME 5-7 group scored better than the CS group for mechanical reasoning, the differences were not significant. However, in the spatial reasoning tasks, the difference between the averages obtained by the CS group and the SME 5-7 and SME 3-4 groups was significant (respectively, mean difference=-2.97, SE=0.710, p=0.000, and mean difference=-2.44, SE=0.974, p=0.046). In the numerical reasoning task, there was a significant difference between the CS group and the SME 5-7 group (mean difference=-3.15, SE=0.666, p=0.000). DISCUSSION The results of this study corroborate the hypothesis that there is a significant relationship between musical education and cognitive growth. This association between the general capacity of reasoning and the number of years involvement with musical education is confirmed by literature dealing with the relationship between musical education and cognitive ability (Schellenberg 2005). As in some of the studies cited above, this research showed that associations were limited to specific tasks. However, when we considered educational performance as a whole, it was found that music lessons affected general intellectual abilities. Students that had a musical education generally obtained higher grades in academic achievement than their counterparts who had not, and this difference seemed to increase with the number of years of

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 353 music lessons. We might infer that music lessons play an important role in the results achieved, rather than extracurricular activities as this was tested elsewhere (Santos-Luiz et al. 2009). The music students in our sample had been tested for musical aptitude, among other things, before embarking on their musical studies. Researchers examining whether musical aptitude is associated to other cognitive capacities have noted that performance in this type of task tends to be positively correlated with general intelligence (Lynn et al. 1989). Therefore, it is possible that increased musical aptitude through musical training is accompanied by improved general cognition (Schellenberg 2006a). The association of music with intelligence and academic performance may result from a simple characteristic or from a cluster of abilities that are fostered by musical education or musical stimulation. Musical structures are abstractions that involve a range of transformations, and mental representations of music must be sufficiently abstract to allow recognition of similarities between varying patterns. Formal musical training may promote the ability to recognize regular patterns and think in a flexible way (Schellenberg 2006b). These abilities are central to the g factor. This study therefore appears to corroborate the assumption that there are associations between music lessons and intellectual ability, although further research is required before this can be asserted with confidence. Acknowledgments This work has been supported by the Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia (FCT) and Programa Operacional Ciência e Inovação 2010 (POCI 2010), co-funded by the Portuguese Government and European Union by the FEDER Program. Address for correspondence Carlos Santos-Luiz, Department of Music, College of Education, Polytechnic Institute of Coimbra, Praça Heróis do Ultramar, Coimbra 3030-329, Portugal; Email: cluis@esec.pt References Barbro B. J. (2006). Music and brain plasticity. European Review, 14. pp. 49-64. Barnett S. M. and Ceci S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128, pp. 612-637. Bilhartz T. D., Bruhn R. A., and Olson J. E. (2000). The effect of early music training on child cognitive development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 20, pp. 615-636.

354 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG Butzlaff R. (2000). Can music be used to teach reading? Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, pp. 167-178. Ho Y.-C., Cheung M.-C., and Chan A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: Cross sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology, 17, pp. 439-450. Huttenlocher P. R. (2002). Neural Plasticity. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Harvard University Press. Lamb S. J. and Gregory A. H. (1993). The relationship between music and reading in beginning readers. Educational Psychology, 13, pp. 19-27. La Mont K. A. (2008). Exploring the Foundations of the Relationship between Music Training and Mathematical Cognition. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Harvard University. Lynn R., Wilson R. G., and Gault A. (1989). Simple musical tests as measures of Spearman s g. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, pp. 25-28. Maureen H. (2008). The effects of music instruction on learning in the Montessori classroom. Montessori Life, 20, pp. 24-31. Orsmond G. I. and Miller L. K. (1999). Cognitive, musical and environmental correlates of early music instruction. Psychology of Music, 27, pp. 18-37. Rauscher F. H., LeMieux M., and Hinton S. C. (2006). Quality piano instruction affects at-risk elementary school children s cognitive abilities and self-esteem. Paper presented at the Ninth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Bologna, Italy. Santos-Luiz C., Coimbra D., and Silva C. F. (2009). Impacto do ensino especializado da música no desempenho académico. Paper presented at the IV Encuentro de Primavera, University of Granada, Spain. Schellenberg E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, pp. 511-514. Schellenberg E. G. (2005). Music and cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, pp. 322-325. Schellenberg E. G. (2006a). Exposure to music: The truth about the consequences. In G. E. McPherson (ed.), The Child as Musician (pp. 111-134). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Schellenberg E. G. (2006b). Long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, pp. 457-468. Vaughn K. (2000). Music and mathematics: Modest support for the oft-claimed relationship. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, pp. 149-166.