Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas: Implications for intelligibility and ease of singing

Similar documents
The role of vocal tract resonances in singing and in playing wind instruments

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Formant frequency tuning in singing

The Perception of Formant Tuning in Soprano Voices

Real-time magnetic resonance imaging investigation of resonance tuning in soprano singing

Physiological and Acoustic Characteristics of the Female Music Theatre Voice in belt and legit qualities

Welcome to Vibrationdata

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. X-ray study of articulation and formant frequencies in two female singers

Making music with voice. Distinguished lecture, CIRMMT Jan 2009, Copyright Johan Sundberg

A comparison of the acoustic vowel spaces of speech and song*20

Vocal-tract Influence in Trombone Performance

increase by 6 db each if the distance between them is halved. Likewise, vowels with a high first formant, such as /a/, or a high second formant, such

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

the mathematics of the voice. As musicians, we d both been frustrated with groups inability to

Analysis of the effects of signal distance on spectrograms

Kent Academic Repository

How We Sing: The Science Behind Our Musical Voice. Music has been an important part of culture throughout our history, and vocal

(Adapted from Chicago NATS Chapter PVA Book Discussion by Chadley Ballantyne. Answers by Ken Bozeman)

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

Saxophonists tune vocal tract resonances in advanced performance techniques

THE KARLSON REPRODUCER

Assessment may include recording to be evaluated by students, teachers, and/or administrators in addition to live performance evaluation.

Vocal tract resonances in singing: Variation with laryngeal mechanism for male operatic singers in chest and falsetto registers

DEVELOPING THE MALE HEAD VOICE. A Paper by. Shawn T. Eaton, D.M.A.

CHAPTER 20.2 SPEECH AND MUSICAL SOUNDS

The Interplay between Glottis and Vocal Tract during the Male Passaggio

How players use their vocal tracts in advanced clarinet and saxophone performance

EVTA SESSION HELSINKI JUNE 06 10, 2012

White Paper JBL s LSR Principle, RMC (Room Mode Correction) and the Monitoring Environment by John Eargle. Introduction and Background:

ANALYSING DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE INPUT IMPEDANCES OF FIVE CLARINETS OF DIFFERENT MAKES

Pitch. There is perhaps no aspect of music more important than pitch. It is notoriously

In 2015 Ian Howell of the New England Conservatory introduced

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

Chamber Music. Guitar X NADAL: American Folk Songs for Guitar. 96pp. 9 x 12. $10.95

Received 27 July ; Perturbations of Synthetic Orchestral Wind-Instrument

AUD 6306 Speech Science

Unit Outcome Assessment Standards 1.1 & 1.3

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Voice source characteristics in different registers in classically trained female musical theatre singers

Available online at International Journal of Current Research Vol. 9, Issue, 08, pp , August, 2017

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

Loudness and Pitch of Kunqu Opera 1 Li Dong, Johan Sundberg and Jiangping Kong Abstract Equivalent sound level (Leq), sound pressure level (SPL) and f

International Journal of Computer Architecture and Mobility (ISSN ) Volume 1-Issue 7, May 2013

EE513 Audio Signals and Systems. Introduction Kevin D. Donohue Electrical and Computer Engineering University of Kentucky

2. AN INTROSPECTION OF THE MORPHING PROCESS

Instrument Recognition in Polyphonic Mixtures Using Spectral Envelopes

Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

Vocal tract adjustments in the high soprano range

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Spectral correlates of carrying power in speech and western lyrical singing according to acoustic and phonetic factors

Pitch-Synchronous Spectrogram: Principles and Applications

How do clarinet players adjust the resonances of their vocal tracts for different playing effects?

Interactions between the player's windway and the air column of a musical instrument 1

Augmentation Matrix: A Music System Derived from the Proportions of the Harmonic Series

Vocal tract resonances in speech, singing, and playing musical instruments

Timbre blending of wind instruments: acoustics and perception

LESSON 1 PITCH NOTATION AND INTERVALS

Glottal behavior in the high soprano range and the transition to the whistle register

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Acoustic analysis of three male voices of different quality

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

3 Voiced sounds production by the phonatory system

Vibration Measurement and Analysis

Some Phonatory and Resonatory Characteristics of the Rock, Pop, Soul, and Swedish Dance Band Styles of Singing

1. Introduction NCMMSC2009

OPERA SAN JOSE Study Guide: Introduction to Opera

Jaw Harp: An Acoustic Study. Acoustical Physics of Music Spring 2015 Simon Li

Efficient Computer-Aided Pitch Track and Note Estimation for Scientific Applications. Matthias Mauch Chris Cannam György Fazekas

Lab P-6: Synthesis of Sinusoidal Signals A Music Illusion. A k cos.! k t C k / (1)

Tristan Und Isolde In Full Score (Dover Music Scores) By Richard Wagner;Opera and Choral Scores

POST-PROCESSING FIDDLE : A REAL-TIME MULTI-PITCH TRACKING TECHNIQUE USING HARMONIC PARTIAL SUBTRACTION FOR USE WITHIN LIVE PERFORMANCE SYSTEMS

Registration Reference Book

Tristan Und Isolde In Full Score (Dover Music Scores) By Richard Wagner;Opera and Choral Scores READ ONLINE

Correlating differences in the playing properties of five student model clarinets with physical differences between them

ACTIVE SOUND DESIGN: VACUUM CLEANER

WHAT INTERVALS DO INDIANS SING?

Siegfried WWV 86 C: Vocal Score READ ONLINE

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

Week 6 - Consonants Mark Huckvale

The characterisation of Musical Instruments by means of Intensity of Acoustic Radiation (IAR)

A PSYCHOACOUSTICAL INVESTIGATION INTO THE EFFECT OF WALL MATERIAL ON THE SOUND PRODUCED BY LIP-REED INSTRUMENTS

THE ACOUSTICS OF THE MUNICIPAL THEATRE IN MODENA

CTP 431 Music and Audio Computing. Basic Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology (GSCT) Juhan Nam

2014 Music Style and Composition GA 3: Aural and written examination

Norman Public Schools MUSIC ASSESSMENT GUIDE FOR GRADE 8

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

MELODY EXTRACTION FROM POLYPHONIC AUDIO OF WESTERN OPERA: A METHOD BASED ON DETECTION OF THE SINGER S FORMANT

RECOMMENDATION ITU-R BT (Questions ITU-R 25/11, ITU-R 60/11 and ITU-R 61/11)

DETECTING ENVIRONMENTAL NOISE WITH BASIC TOOLS

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

WHAT IS BARBERSHOP. Life Changing Music By Denise Fly and Jane Schlinke

On time: the influence of tempo, structure and style on the timing of grace notes in skilled musical performance

IBEGIN MY FIRST ARTICLE AS Associate Editor of Journal of Singing for

Simple Harmonic Motion: What is a Sound Spectrum?

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

THE LIBRETTI OF MOZART'S COMPLETED OPERAS: VOL. I BY NICO CASTEL DOWNLOAD EBOOK : THE LIBRETTI OF MOZART'S COMPLETED OPERAS: VOL. I BY NICO CASTEL PDF

Investigation of Digital Signal Processing of High-speed DACs Signals for Settling Time Testing

Voice source and acoustic measures of girls singing classical and contemporary commercial styles

Topic 10. Multi-pitch Analysis

Choral Sight-Singing Practices: Revisiting a Web-Based Survey

Transcription:

Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas: Implications for intelligibility and ease of singing John Smith and Joe Wolfe School of Physics, University of New South Wales, Sydney, New South Wales 252, Australia john.smith@unsw.edu.au Abstract: European vowels are mainly distinguished by the two lowest resonance frequencies (R1 and R2) of the vocal tract. Once the pitch frequency f exceeds the value of R1 in normal speech, sopranos can deliberately tune R1 to match f. This increases loudness, uniformity of tone, and ease of singing, at some cost to intelligibility. Resonance tuning would be assisted if the pitch of the note written for a vowel corresponded with its usual range of R1. Analysis of several soprano roles indicates that Wagner aided the acoustics of the soprano voice at high pitch when setting text to music. 29 Acoustical Society of America PACS numbers: 43.75.Rs [DD] Date Received: December 24, 28 Date Accepted: February 26, 29 1. Introduction In normal speech, the vibrating vocal folds generate a harmonically rich signal with pitch frequency f, which interacts with resonances of frequency Ri in the vocal tract to produce sound with a spectral envelope that exhibits broad peaks, called formants, 1 with frequency Fi (Fant, 197). These resonances have bandwidths of 1 Hz or so, and can be controlled independently of f by varying the position and shape of the tongue, jaw, lips, and larynx (Lindblom and Sundberg, 1971). Vowels in European languages are largely identified by the frequencies of the first two formants F1,F2 and thus of the resonances R1,R2 that produce them. In adult conversational speech, f is typically in the range 1 3 Hz, whereas the resonances R1 and R2 lie in the approximate ranges 3 8 and 8 2 Hz, respectively. Consequently, f and/or some of its harmonics (i.e., 2f,3f, etc.) usually fall close enough to each resonance to receive a useful power boost and to produce identifiable formants. The situation is similar for most singing ranges when f is less than about 5 Hz (near the high C of tenors). However, f for sopranos can range from 25 to 1 Hz or even higher. Four problems can arise when f exceeds significantly the normal range of R1 for a vowel. First, the acoustic load of the tract on the vocal folds and glottis changes from inertive ( mass-like ) when f R1 to compliant ( spring-like ) when f R1. Vocal fold vibration can then become less efficient and less stable (Titze 1988, 28; Titze et al., 28). Second, the sound level is usually reduced, as little acoustic energy will be radiated at frequency f. Third, a strong variation in the amplitudes of the fundamental and/or second harmonic may occur as the pitch changes, producing possibly undesirable discontinuities in timbre. Finally, the effective absence of F1 means that vowels with a similar value of F2 become indistinguishable. Indeed, as f increases, the spacing of harmonics can become so large that even F2 may also effectively disappear. For a soprano, a solution to all but the last of these problems is to tune R1 close to, but slightly above, f (Sundberg, 1975; Joliveau et al., 24a, 24b; Titze et al., 28). This should increase power, increase the ease of singing, and help maintain timbral homogeneity. Accordingly, sopranos often modify their vowels when singing at high pitch (e.g., Coffin, 1974, 1976). This solution, called resonance tuning, has no disadvantages when singing vocalise because there is no textual information. However, when singing text, F1 will consequently be similar to R1 and no longer have an appropriate value for many phoneme-pitch combinations. This increases the probability that vowels are confused as the difference between f and the EL196 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125 5, May 29 29 Acoustical Society of America

value of R1 in normal speech increases (Morozov, 1965; Scotto di Carlo and Germain, 1985; Hollien et al., 2). In an earlier study (Dowd et al., 1997), we found that correct recognition in speech decreases exponentially with displacement on the R1,R2 plane. (For French, the characteristic length is.36 times the average distance between vowels.) Thus, with resonance tuning, a vowel would be likely to be confused with another of higher R1 once f exceeded its normal R1 by about 1 2 Hz. This confusion could be minimized if a composer took advantage of vowel-pitch matching, i.e., if each vowel were sung with a fundamental frequency f that was consistent with its usual range of R1. Further, the singer would no longer need to tune the resonance significantly (Carlsson-Berndtsson and Sundberg, 1991): the libretto would partially do it for her, making it easier to sing with a high ratio of output power to input effort. Composers have long been aware of the problem of reduced intelligibility (Berlioz, 1844). But have they used vowel-pitch matching (whether consciously or unconsciously) to reduce it and/or to make the music easier to sing? If so, the vowel distribution would vary systematically with pitch: vowels associated with a low R1 would be sung less often at high pitch and vice versa. If not, we should expect the distribution of vowels to be independent of pitch, simply reflecting that of the libretto. A test of this hypothesis would require libretti with a large number of vowels in different words to ensure that any observed distribution was not a statistical accident. Compositions wherein the intelligibility of the text is important should also be more likely to show a non-uniform distribution of vowels with pitch. Not all operas are thus suitable: in some, the high pitch writing for soprano is aimed at technical display, rather than conveying text. In others, a relatively simple plot is adequately conveyed by repetition or by actions on stage. A composer who was also the librettist might be most likely to match vowels with pitch. For these reasons, this study focused initially on the two great Wagnerian soprano roles; Brünnhilde and Isolde. Each singer has to communicate lengthy, subtle aspects of plot via text alone. Further, the libretti were written by Wagner, published prior to composing the scores, and considered by him and others to be significant literary works in their own right. Four operas by other composers were then studied for comparison. 2. Methods Using published scores, the phoneme and fundamental (pitch) frequency f were recorded for each note sung by the chosen soprano(s) in each operas studied see Table 1. Any obvious ornamentation, grace notes, trills, and mordants were not included as they carry no textual information. (Because resonances and formants are broad, high precision in fundamental frequency is not required, so A4=44 Hz was assumed throughout). Wagner provided little ornamentation and generally writes only one note per syllable, (sometimes two for Isolde). To simplify presentation, the 12 vowels of German were grouped into the four standard categories according to their jaw height in the vocoid or Cardinal Vowel space. The range of R1 we associated with each category were as follows: closed 25 4 Hz, close-mid (or half-close) 4 55 Hz, open-mid (or half-open) 55 75 Hz, and open 75 1 Hz. Of course the values of R1 will vary with language, dialect, accent, etc. It is also possible that listeners may learn to use a different formant map for sopranos (i.e., a different categorization of the vowel plane), in much the same way that we use different maps for men, women, and children. Ultimately, we can only make an informed guess at the vowel sound imagined by the composer-librettist. Although there might be uncertainty about the range of resonance frequencies in each category, the important feature for this study is that their order with increasing frequency is known. The soprano roles of Fiordiligi in Così fan tutte and Sophie in Der Rosenkavalier were analyzed in a similar fashion. A slightly abbreviated analysis was made of the soprano roles in Don Giovanni by Mozart and The Barber of Seville by Rossini, which included only notes longer than a crochet (quarter note) or a quaver (eighth note) in slow sections, in the range from G4 to C6, a range below which resonance tuning by sopranos is not observed (Joliveau et al. 24a, 24b). There are thus no data on vowel-pitch matching for the closed vowels in these two operas. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125 5, May 29 J. Smith and J. Wolfe: Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas EL197

Table 1. Details of the operas. Opera Composer Librettist Year Language Soprano role Der Rosenkavalier a Strauss von Hofmannsthal 199 191 German Sophie Der Ring des Nibelungen b,c Wagner Wagner 1854 1874 German Brünnhilde Götterdämmerung b Wagner Wagner 1869 1874 German Brünnhilde Siegfried c Wagner Wagner 1856 1869 German Brünnhilde Tristan und Isolde d Wagner Wagner 1857 1859 German Isolde Die Walküre c Wagner Wagner 1854 1856 German Brünnhilde The Barber of Seville e Rossini Sterbini 1816 Italian All Così fan tutte f Mozart da Ponte 179 Italian Fiordiligi Don Giovanni g Mozart da Ponte 1787 Italian All a The edition of the score used was from Boosey & Hawkes, London, 1943. b The edition of the score used was from Klavierauszug-VEB Breitkopf und Härtel Musikverlag, Leipzig, 198. c The edition of the score used was from Klavierauszug-VEB Breitkopf und Härtel Musikverlag, Leipzig, 1984. d The edition of the score used was from Ernst Eulenberg, London, 19. e The edition of the score used was from Kalmus 368, New York. f The edition of the score used was from Neue Ausgabe sämtlicher Werke, Bärenreiter Kassel, Basel, 1991. g The edition of the score used was from Kassel Bärenreiter, 1975. 3. Results and discussion The upper part of Fig. 1(A) shows, for each pitch, the total number of notes sung for the combined soprano roles of Brünnhilde and Isolde. Each note has a particular pitch and an associated vowel. There are sufficient of these vowel-pitch combinations (more than 1 ) to provide useful statistics and a reasonably smooth distribution with pitch, although some preference for keys harmonically close to C major is apparent. Fewer notes are written at the extremes of the soprano range, presumably because very high notes are physically demanding and dramatically effective if used sparingly, and because sopranos low notes often have reduced dynamic range. The lower part of Fig. 1(A) shows the distribution of vowels grouped into the four categories for jaw height and thus R1. The vowel distribution does vary with frequency, and in a fashion that helps match R1 tof. Thus the closed and close-mid vowels with low R1 become less common as f rises above 5 6 Hz. Conversely, the fraction of open-mid and open vowels increases significantly above 6 Hz. The open vowels are, of course, preferred at high pitch, whereas other vowels would be seriously distorted by resonance tuning. However, open vowels are also used across the whole pitch range. The unmodified R1 for these vowels is sufficiently high that, for almost the whole of the soprano range, the vocal tract load at the glottis is inertive. Further, for notes in the low soprano range, R1 may be excited by a second or even third harmonic. So the open vowels are least likely to be distorted by resonance tuning. Figure 1(B) shows the data for the vowel-pitch combinations for the role of Fiordilgi in Così fan tutte, an opera buffa by Mozart. There is no significant variation of vowels with pitch, except surprisingly at low notes where the closed vowels (u and i) are less likely than at the highest notes; this could reduce intelligibility. Figure 2 shows the extent of pitch-resonance matching for the eight operas studied in terms of, a parameter we define as follows. For all notes lying in a frequency band corresponding to a particular jaw height, let g be the average fraction of notes whose vowel corresponds to that jaw height. Let h be the average fraction of notes at all other frequencies having those vowels. The parameter =g/h 1 then indicates the preference for the appropriate vowel-pitch combinations. Positive and negative values indicate favorable and unfavorable pitch-resonance matching, respectively. The vowel distribution is seen to change systematically with frequency EL198 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125 5, May 29 J. Smith and J. Wolfe: Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas

Vowel pitch pairs 1 8 6 4 2 A Brünnhilde & Isolde 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 8 6 4 2 B Fiordiligi in Così fan tutte 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 Cumulative vowel fraction 1..8.6.4.2 jaw height = closed close-mid open-mid open 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 Pitch frequency f o (Hz) 1..8.6.4.2 u vowel = o,ɔ i e,ε a 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Pitch frequency f o (Hz) 1 Fig. 1. Semi-logarithmic plots of the number of vowel-pitch combinations upper and the cumulative vowel fraction lower as function of written pitch frequency f for soprano roles in operas by Wagner and Mozart. On the lower figures, the open vowels lie between the axis and the lowest continuous line, open-mid between the two lowest continuous lines, etc. The dashed lines show the null hypothesis: equal distribution of vowels across pitch. Shaded areas indicate regions where R1 and pitch might be favorably matched. Heavily shaded areas indicate approximate regions where resonance tuning would be particularly favorable. in all four Wagner operas studied. The direction of the changes strongly supports the hypothesis of non-accidental matching of vowels with pitch, but it is worth considering non-acoustic causes. For example, high notes are often used for dramatic emphasis, so important declarative text might alter the distribution via preferential repetition at high pitch. Isolde often sings Tristan and Liebe, usually with descending pitch. Brünnhilde is also heavily involved with Sieglinde, Siegmund, Siegfried, and Liebe. However, removal of these words from the analysis did not significantly alter the overall distribution of vowels with frequency. The data presented in the figures do not include the war-whoops of Brünnhilde in Die Walküre because, although her repeated cries of Hojotoho Heiaha occur at high pitch, they convey negligible textual information. Their inclusion does not significantly alter the vowel distribution. γ 1.5 1..5.25 favourable Don Giovanni Cosi fan tutte The Barber of Seville Die Walküre Tristan und Isolde Siegfried Götterdämmerung Ring des Nibelungen Der Rosenkavalier Key to jaw height closed close-mid open-mid open Fig. 2. The extent of pitch-resonance matching for soprano roles in the eight operas studied. The degree of matching is indicated by, a parameter that indicates the preference for the appropriate vowel-pitch combinations see text. Positive and negative values of indicate favorable and unfavorable pitch-resonance matching, respectively. Values associated with open vowels high R1 are indicated by shading. The operas are shown in historical order from left to right. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125 5, May 29 J. Smith and J. Wolfe: Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas EL199

Do other composers or librettists match vowels with pitch? Figure 2 indicates that, for the limited sample of works studied, a significant increase in for open vowels occurs only in the operas by Wagner. Interestingly, for open vowels increased systematically as Wagner s experience as a composer increased. For comparison, the role of Sophie in the later opera Der Rosenkavalier by Richard Strauss shows no significant effect. Wagner s idea of opera was a continuous music drama. Earlier operas often linked separate arias and choruses with explanatory recitative and thus had less need for intelligibility at high pitch. Furthermore Wagner wrote for much larger orchestras than those available to Mozart or Rossini, and wrote vocal parts that severely test the stamina and capabilities of singers. Thus the employment of vowel-pitch matching could have helped satisfy the concomitant requirements of intelligibility, vocal power, and easier singing of difficult parts. A number of complications and differences are also relevant: in some operas, important phrases can be repeated several times at different pitches rarely in Wagner and Strauss, but often in Mozart. One might also consider the time available to composers to polish the operas; Rossini wrote some 26 operas in 7 years whereas Wagner wrote 14 in over 5 years. It is also possible that a composer-librettist might alter the overall vowel distribution from that of normal speech to favor more open vowels, particularly when writing text for orchestral accompaniment. First, singers can produce higher sound pressure levels using open vowels at any pitch (Gramming and Sundberg, 1988). Furthermore, the time-averaged power spectrum produced by an orchestra peaks around 5 6 Hz and then decreases with increasing frequency (Sundberg, 1977). Consequently, the voice of a singer using more open vowels would be expected to have more power above 5 Hz through reinforced harmonics, even when singing at lower pitch. Thus a libretto to be sung with orchestra might advantageously include a higher proportion of open vowels than written text. This was not investigated here, in part because of the difficulty in choosing appropriate texts to use as the non-operatic controls. Many factors, apart from vowels, are involved in intelligibility. For instance, if only one vowel in a string of phonemes produces a real word, then the vowel need not be recognized. Similarly, context in a sentence can mean that a weird can be understood, independently of the vowel. The results of this study also suggest that those translating a libretto for performance in another language might occasionally consider vowel-pitch matching among all of the other constraints. 4. Conclusions The authors are unaware of any written evidence about the composers intentions nor of whether they were advised on this issue by sopranos, with whom they sometimes had quite close relations. However, it appears that Wagner, either consciously or unconsciously, did take the acoustics of the soprano voice at high pitch into account when setting text to music. This is consistent with the increased importance of textual information in his operas, the increasing size of his orchestras, and the more complex vocal parts. Acknowledgment The support of the Australian Research Council is gratefully acknowledged. References and links 1 We follow the original definitions of Fant (197) and reserve the term formant for broad peaks in the spectral envelope and resonance for the acoustic resonances of the vocal tract that produce them see Wolfe et al. (29) for further discussion. Berlioz, H. (1844). Grand traité d instrumentation et d orchestration modernes (A treatise on modern instrumentation and orchestration), translated by M. C. Clarke (Novello, London). Carlsson-Berndtsson, G., and Sundberg, J. (1991). Formant frequency tuning in singing, Speech Transm. Lab. Q. Prog. Status Rep. 32, 29 35. Coffin, B. (1974). On hearing, feeling and using the instrumental resonance of the singing voice, NATS Bulletin 31, 26 3. EL2 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125 5, May 29 J. Smith and J. Wolfe: Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas

Coffin, B. (1976). Coffin s Sounds of Singing (The Scarecrow, Lanham). Dowd, A., Smith, J., and Wolfe, J. (1997). Learning to pronounce vowel sounds in a foreign language using acoustic measurements of the vocal tract as feedback in real time, Lang Speech 41, 1 2. Fant, G. (197). Acoustic Theory of Speech Production (Mouton, The Hague). Gramming, P., and Sundberg, J. (1988). Spectrum factors relevant to phonetogram measurement, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 2352 236. Hollien, H., Mendes-Schwartz, A. P., and Nielsen, K. (2). Perceptual confusions of high-pitched sung vowels, J. Voice 14, 287 298. Joliveau, E., Smith, J., and Wolfe, J. (24a). Tuning of vocal tract resonances by sopranos, Nature (London) 427, 116. Joliveau, E., Smith, J., and Wolfe, J. (24b). Vocal tract resonances in singing: The soprano voice, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 2434 2439. Lindblom, B. E. F., and Sundberg, J. E. F. (1971). Acoustical consequences of lip, tongue, jaw, and larynx movement, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 5, 1166 1179. Morozov,V.P.(1965). Intelligibility in singing as a function of fundamental voice pitch, Sov. Phys. Acoust. 1, 279 283. Scotto di Carlo, N., and Germain, A. (1985). A perceptual study of the influence of pitch on the intelligibility of sung vowels, Phonetica 42, 188 197. Sundberg, J. (1975). Formant technique in a professional female singer, Acustica 32, 89 96. Sundberg, J. (1977). The acoustics of the singing voice, Sci. Am., 82 91. Titze, I. R. (1988). The physics of small-amplitude oscillations of the vocal folds, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 1536 1552. Titze, I. R. (28). Nonlinear source-filter coupling in phonation: Theory, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123, 2733 2749. Titze, I. R., Tobias, R., and Popolo, P. (28). Nonlinear source-filter coupling in phonation: Vocal exercises, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123, 192 1915. Wolfe, J., Garnier, M., and Smith, J. (29). Vocal tract resonances in speech, singing and playing musical instruments, HFSP J., 3(1), 6 23. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125 5, May 29 J. Smith and J. Wolfe: Vowel-pitch matching in Wagner s operas EL21