The Nature of Children's Singing Voices: Characteristics and Assessment

Similar documents
The effect of focused instruction on young children s singing accuracy

The effect of male timbre vocal modeling in falsetto and non-falsetto on the singing and pitch accuracy of second grade students

The effects of range of notes, gender, and songs with text or without text on the vocal accuracy of first, second and third grade students

THE EFFECT OF REPERTOIRE ON THE SINGING ACHIEVEMENT OF SECOND GRADE STUDENTS. Christine Elyse Hadfield

STRATEGIES FOR WORKING WITH INACCURATE SINGERS

Choral Sight-Singing Practices: Revisiting a Web-Based Survey

The effects of harmonic accompaniment on the music achievement, aptitude, and musical effectiveness of first grade children

Effects of a remedial singing method on the vocal pitch accuracy of inaccurate elementary singers

STUDENT LEARNING OBJECTIVE (SLO) PROCESS TEMPLATE

Anthony Leach Professor of Music Thesis Reader

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RHYTHM APTITUDE AND RHYTHM ACHIEVEMENT OF FIRST, SECOND, AND THIRD GRADE STUDENTS. Kelly Kristine Harding

Title: The Application of Gordon s Empirical Model of Learning Sequence to Teaching the Recorder. Author(s): June C. McDonald

THE EFFECTS OF COMPUTER MUSIC LEARNING ACTIVITIES ON THE TONAL APTITUDES OF CANADIAN STUDENTS ALLAN F. ANDERSON

Music. on Scale and. Specificc Talent Aptitude: Visual Arts, Music, Dance, Psychomotor, Creativity, Leadership. Performing Arts,

Introduction to Performance Fundamentals

MENC: The National Association for Music Education

Differentiated Approaches to Aural Acuity Development: A Case of a Secondary School in Kiambu County, Kenya

AUDITION PROCEDURES:

Developing Musicianship through Improvisation: An Eclectic Teaching Module for Pre-school Children Preeyanun Promsukkul

Assessment may include recording to be evaluated by students, teachers, and/or administrators in addition to live performance evaluation.

National Coalition for Core Arts Standards. Music Model Cornerstone Assessment: General Music Grade 2

A COMPARISON OF COMPOSITIONAL TEACHING METHODS: PAPER AND PENCIL VERSUS COMPUTER HARDWARE AND SOFTWARE

in the Howard County Public School System and Rocketship Education

National Coalition for Core Arts Standards. Music Model Cornerstone Assessment: General Music Grades 3-5

Indiana Music Standards

CAB CALLOWAY HIGH SCHOOL VOCAL ASSESSMENTS

Grade-Level Academic Standards for General Music

Grade One General Music

VOCAL MUSIC CURRICULUM STANDARDS Grades Students will sing, alone and with others, a varied repertoire of music.

KVMEA Judges Handbook First Edition 2012

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

PERFORMING ARTS Curriculum Framework K - 12

Texas Music Education Research

College of MUSIC. James Forger, DEAN UNDERGRADUATE PROGRAMS. Admission as a Junior to the College of Music

Validity. What Is It? Types We Will Discuss. The degree to which an inference from a test score is appropriate or meaningful.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Effects of Using Graphic Notations. on Creativity in Composing Music. by Australian Secondary School Students. Myung-sook Auh

California Subject Examinations for Teachers

HEARTBEAT STEADY BEAT DEBORAH PRATT, M.A. RESEARCH FINDINGS ON THE MUSICAL DEVELOPMENT OF CHILDREN FROM BEFORE BIRTH THROUGH AGE EIGHT

PASADENA INDEPENDENT SCHOOL DISTRICT Fine Arts Teaching Strategies

Diocese of Richmond Consensus Curriculum for Music

Visual Arts, Music, Dance, and Theater Personal Curriculum

Oskaloosa Community School District. Music. Grade Level Benchmarks

Week. self, peer, or other performances 4 Manipulate their bodies into the correct

University Microfilms International tann Arbor, Michigan 48106

Standard 1 PERFORMING MUSIC: Singing alone and with others

Becoming an expert in the musical domain: It takes more than just practice

Musical learning and cognitive performance

Newport Public Schools Curriculum Framework. Subject: Classroom Music Standard #1 Grade Level Grade 1 Standard Benchmarks Suggested Resources/

EFFECTS OF ORFF-SCHULWERK PROCESS OF IMITATION ON ELEMENTARY STUDENTS READING FLUENCY

Primary Music Objectives (Prepared by Sheila Linville and Julie Troum)

Musical Developmental Levels Self Study Guide

OVER THE YEARS, PARTICULARLY IN THE PAST

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

Connecticut Common Arts Assessment Initiative

Music Standard 1. Standard 2. Standard 3. Standard 4.

EASTERN ARIZONA COLLEGE Elementary Theory

GRADE FOUR GENERAL MUSIC

Eighth Grade Music Curriculum Guide Iredell-Statesville Schools

SOA PIANO ENTRANCE AUDITIONS FOR 6 TH - 12 TH GRADE

MMS 8th Grade General Music Curriculum

Perceiving Differences and Similarities in Music: Melodic Categorization During the First Years of Life

6 th Grade Instrumental Music Curriculum Essentials Document

NON-NEGOTIBLE EVALUATION CRITERIA

Florida Performing Fine Arts Assessment Item Specifications for Benchmarks in Course: M/J Chorus 3

Praxis Music: Content Knowledge (5113) Study Plan Description of content

NON-NEGOTIBLE EVALUATION CRITERIA

Music. Last Updated: May 28, 2015, 11:49 am NORTH CAROLINA ESSENTIAL STANDARDS

MENC: The National Association for Music Education

HSC Music 2 Marking Guidelines Practical tasks and submitted works

First Year Evaluation Report for PDAE Grant Accentuating Music, Language and Cultural Literacy through Kodály Inspired Instruction

PASADENA INDEPENDENT SCHOOL DISTRICT Fine Arts Teaching Strategies

CHD100- Music in Early Childhood - Handout

Artistic Process: Performing Accomplished / Advanced Ensembles

TExES Music EC 12 (177) Test at a Glance

Florida Performing Fine Arts Assessment Item Specifications for Benchmarks in Course: Music Grade Two

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. School of Music THE EFFECT OF IMPROVISATION ON THE TRANSFER OF TECHNIQUE.

MUSIC100 Rudiments of Music

Curriculum Development In the Fairfield Public Schools FAIRFIELD PUBLIC SCHOOLS FAIRFIELD, CONNECTICUT MUSIC THEORY I

SAMPLE ASSESSMENT TASKS MUSIC GENERAL YEAR 12

Students will know Vocabulary: steady beat melody rhythm patterns loud/soft fast/slow improvise

Chapter 117. Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills for Fine Arts Subchapter A. Elementary

MUSIC DEPARTMENT MUSIC COURSES CAN BE USED AS ELECTIVE CREDITS

Westbrook Public Schools Westbrook Middle School Chorus Curriculum Grades 5-8

Brief Report. Development of a Measure of Humour Appreciation. Maria P. Y. Chik 1 Department of Education Studies Hong Kong Baptist University

MUSIC DEPARTMENT MUSIC PERSPECTIVES: HISTORY OF POPULAR MUSIC A/B /656600

Chapter 117. Texas Essential Knowledge and Skills for Fine Arts. Subchapter A. Elementary

Absolute Memory of Learned Melodies

A comparison of the Kodaly method and the traditional method to determine pitch accuracy in grade 6 choral sight-singing

Rhythmic Characteristics of Improvisational Drumming Among Preschool Children

University of Western Ontario Don Wright Faculty of Music Kodaly Summer Music Course KODÁLY Musicianship Level I SYLLABUS

Central DeWitt Community School District. K--12 Music Standards

Course Title: Chorale, Concert Choir, Master s Chorus Grade Level: 9-12

Serban Nichifor Compositeur, Professeur

The Public Schools of Brookline Performing Arts Learning Expectations Grades K-4

How do scoops influence the perception of singing accuracy?

Application and Audition Guidelines Incoming 6 th Grade Students

Honors Music Theory South Carroll High School : Fall Semester

Grade 4 General Music

2001 HSC Music Extension Marking Guidelines

Transcription:

The Nature of Children's Singing Voices: Characteristics and Assessment Joanne Rutkowski Pennsylvania State University Singing is often the window to the young child's musical life. Children sing during play, alone and with others, and group singing activities are encouraged in day care centers and preschool settings. However, some children seem to have difficulty using their singing voices and that musical window, unfortunately, becomes closed for them. Over the past 50 years, numerous studies have investigated topics related to the child singing voice and its development. Terminology used to describe the various stages of development of the child voice and/or the types of problem singers, however, have been inconsistent. Terms such as "inaccurate singers" (Anderson in Welch, 1979; Reuter; 1956a, 1956b), "backward singers" (Fieldhouse in Welch, 1979), and "non-singer" (McKenzie, 1948) have been utilized. Bentley (1968) disliked the term "monotone" since most of these children did produce more than one tone, but used the term stating that it was less derogatory than other labels presently in use. Several persons have discussed different types of singers. Nye and Nye (1970) indicated two: "Non-singers", those individuals who do not have use of the singing voice, and "problem singers", individuals who have a very limited range, usually not higher than E3 or F3. Hartzell (1949) established three classifications: Children who can establish and maintain tonality, children who can establish tonality but do not maintain it, and children who can do neither. Kirkpatrick (1962) used Hartzell's classifications but relabeled each category using the terms, "singers", "partial singers", and "non-singers". Gaiser (1961) used the term "nonsinger" to refer to "children whose singing performance varies from the norm in that they habitually sing several tones away from the group, usually below, or vary uncertainly from tone to tone" (p. 4). She also employed three classifications of "non-singers": "monotones" who only sing one tone; "near-singers" who sing multiple tones but lack control of tones; and "followers" who imitate a group but cannot sing alone. Joyner (1969), while working with boys age II, employed four categories to describe their vocal achievement: "Normal singers" were those who could sing in a low and high key; "Grade A monotones" were tuneful in the low key but not in the higher key; "Grade B monotones" were those who were erratic at both pitch levels; "Grade C monotones" are always untunefui. Forcucci (1975) also utilized four categories to describe different types of singers: "independent singers" sing in-tune without assistance; "dependent or lazy singers" sing in-tune within a group; "uncertain singers" sing out-of-tune with or without a group; "restricted range singers" are those usually thought to be "monotones" even though they can actually produce more than one pitch. Gordon (1971, 1979) also used four categories as he described problem singers: "Non-singers" attempt to 201

sing either in the speaking voice range or above the singing range. "Out-of-tune singers" either have a sense of pitch or lack both aspects. Young (1971) observed several stages of voice range development in kindergarten and first grade children: D3 to F3-sharp or A2 to E3-flat; A2 to F3-sharp; A2 to C4; A2 to D4 and above. These four voice stages were not labeled. Young (1971) and Gordon (1971, 1979) both indicated the existence of a voice break or register lift in the child voice from approximately 83.flat to D4-flat. Children seem to have the most difficulty producing tones in this range. As can be seen, terminology used to describe children's singing has been extremely inconsistent. Background and Purpose of This Study Since standard terminology has not been in evidence for labeling the various stages through which a child's voice progresses, Rutkowski (1990a) developed the Singing Voice Development Measure (SVDM) to establish a more consistent means of describing the various stages of child singing voice development and to provide a consistent means for teachers and researchers to more accurately measure and describe the use a child has of his!her singing voice. Precise assessment is of concern when conducting research in which the singing voice is a factor and when designing music instruction for children. It seems logical that a child must gain use of the singing voice before intonation problems can be researched and evaluated. Surely an instrumentalist must know the fingering for a particular note and be able to produce a sound on the instrument before intonation problems become a concern. (A trumpeter will not play an "F" in-tune until she!he can play an "F".) Further, many children do not demonstrate accurate intonation within a phrase until approximately age 5.5 or 6 years of age (Davidson et ai., 1981). Consequently, it seems that the use a child has of his!her singing voice may be a construct separate from and requisite to the ability to sing in-tune. Rutkowski (1990a) investigated this hypothesis and found it valid. The specific focus of this paper is to provide a review of the revisions made to SVDMsince its development was first reported (Rutkowski, 1990a). Singing Voice Development Initial Version Measure The initial version of SVDMas reported in 1990 is presented in Figure 1. The initial singing voice behaviors were identified upon "consultation with several elementary vocal music specialists as well as a compilation of results from previous studies" (Rutkowski, 1990a, p. 85). A thorough discussion of the selection of song and pattern material as well as testing and rating procedures can be found in the initial report and are not enumerated here. However, the conclusions and recommendations from that study were as follows. Several rating scales have been used to assess children's singing voice achievement. However, none exclusively measured use of singing voice: Melodic contour and intonation were of primary concern. Since these existing scales were not appropriate for measuring use of singing voice, a rating scale to measure this domain needed to be designed, piloted, and implemented. SVDMwas designed, piloted, and implemented. It was shown to be a valid instrument to measure 202

children's use of singing voice (Rutkowski, 1984, 1986).."" Even though SVDMwas shown to be a valid mea~tite for children's use of singing voice, several further revisions were recommended. The testing procedure, while successful, was very time consuming. Since the Patterns and Song subtests were highly correlated on both the pretest and posttest (Rutkowski, 1986), it seemed that one subtest would be sufficient for measuring children's use of singing voice. The children's mean scores on both subtests were very similar. However, their gain scores were slightly better for the patterns. Although reliability coefficients were also similar for both subtests, the raters indicated that once they SVDM: Initial Version Figure 1 RATING SCALE 1 "Pre-singer" does not sing but chants the song text 2 "Speaking Range Singer" sustains tones and exhibits some sensitivity to pitch but remains in the speaking voice range (usually A2 to C3) 3 "Uncertain Singer" waivers between speaking and singing voices, uses a limited range when in singing voice (usually up to F3) 4 "Initial Range Singer" exhibits use of initial singing range (usually D3 to A3) 5 "Singer" exhibits use of extended range (sings beyond the register lift: B3-flat and above) SONG: "Bakerman" Come a. long. Come a. long. What shall we play? I'll be ba ker. man. just for to. day. PATIERNS: ~i_h B"ld'h" di See the birds In 1 e lrcc, 01 a nest wit some IWlgs an some caves. 203

had become familiar with rating the pattern performances they found these performances easier to rate than the song performances. Furthermore, when utilizing a song for evaluation, a lengthy orientation period is required in order to familiarize the children with the criterion song. While an orientation period is also necessary when utilizing patterns for evaluation, this period need not be as long since the pattern performances are an echo activity. In addition, although singing a song, rather than patterns, is generally considered singing, it seems that performing a song involved aspects other than use of singing voice. These include memorization of text, rhythm patterns, and tonal patterns. A child may not be employing singing voice simply because slhe cannot remember some of these other components. Therefore, measuring use of singing through performance of a song may not yield a valid score. Since the singing of patterns involves echoing, rather than memorization, the role of these other components would be diminished. Upon consultation with the raters, it was felt that the first and second patterns should be eliminated. Since the first pattern was chanted, rather than sung, it may have encouraged children to use their speaking voice rather than singing voice on the subsequent patterns. Similarly, the second pattern was sung in a speaking voice range and may have encouraged children to use their speaking voice range on the subsequent patterns. Furthermore, while the text used for the pattern performances seemed interesting and easily remembered by the children, the vowel and consonant combinations were not easily sung. The use of a neutral syllable, rather than words, would alleviate this problem. However, this investigator has observed that children often experience difficulty singing on a neutral syllable. Research studies investigating the use of a neutral syllable for singing with children have yielded contradictory results (Goetze, 1985; Levinowitz, 1989; Smale, 1987; White, Sergeant, & Welch, 1997). Until more information is available regarding this matter, it was recommended that new, more singable, text be identified and used for the pattern performances. Finally, it was recommended that the SVDMbe used to evaluate use of singing voice for research purposes as well as in a classroom setting to assist the music teacher in providing more appropriate instruction. Since content validity was established for SVDMfor measuring children's use of singing voice (Rutkowski, 1984, 1986), it was inferred that the categories established for use of singing voice do exist. It should be re-emphasized that these categories are not concerned with accuracy of intonation or melodic contour. Some children in stages 2 to 5 will sing in-tune within the limits of their voice range. However, many children in these stages will be out-of-tune singers. Version 2 Based on previous recommendations, SVDMwas revised to include only pattern performances within a singing voice range and more singable text (See Figure 2). This version of the instrument was used in a study conducted during the 1989-1990 school year (Rutkowski, 1996). In addition, since some evidence existed to indicate that those children with above average music aptitude may in 204

Figure 2 SVOM: Version 2 RATINGSCALE 1 "Pre-singer" does not sing but chants the song text 2 "Speaking Range Singer" sustains tones and exhibits some sensitivity to pitch but remains in the speaking voice range (usually A2 to C3) 3 "Uncertain Singer" waivers between speaking and singing voices, uses a limited range when in singing voice (usually up to F3) 4 "Initial Range Singer" exhibits use of initial singing range (usually 03 to A3) 5 "Singer" exhibits use of extended range (sings beyond the register lift: 83-flat and above) PATTERNS: See the bird, In the tree. See it ny, o ver me. fact be "non-singers" (Rutkowski, 1986), the relationship between use of singing voice and developmental music aptitude, as measured by the Primary Measures of Music Audiation (PMMA) (Gordon, 1979) was also investigated. PMMAwas administered following the guidelines in the test manual. Prior to administration of SVDM,the teacher practiced the patterns with the children (6- year-olds) following the exact procedure that was used for individual testing. For individual testing, each child reported to a familiar, private room where hislher voice was tape recorded as slhe echoed the teacher singing the SVDMpatterns. Two raters, who had used SVDMin previous research, were employed to score SVDMfor this study. These raters have been shown to have high inter- and intrarater reliability (Rutkowski, I990b). The raters completed their evaluations privately; they were not together while rating the performances. Inter-class correlation coefficients were computed to determine the agreement between raters when scoring for SVDM.High coefficients of.90 and.99 indicated that the raters used the SVDMsimilarly when rating the children's singing voice performance. Pearson product moment correlations were computed to determine the relationship between SVDMand PMMAscores. All correlation coefficients were low (.126 to.182) and suggest a very small relationship between use of singing voice and developmental tonal aptitude. Based on these results, it was recommended that research investigating children's music abilities be conducted with children who have use of singing voice and that teachers caution against using a child's singing ability to determine hislher music potential. 205

The raters commented that many children seemed to be inconsistent in their use of singing voice: They fluctuated between two stages. The SVDMscale was expanded to allow for these behaviors and is presented below: 1 "Pre-singer" does not sing but chants the song text. 1.5 "Inconsistent Speaking Range Singer" sometimes chants, sometimes sustains tones and exhibits some sensitivity to pitch but remains in the speaking voice range (usually A2to C3). 2 "Speaking Range Singer" sustains tones and exhibits some sensitivity to pitch but remains in the speaking voice range (usually A2 to C3). 2.5 "Inconsistent Limited Range Singer" wavers between speaking and singing voice and uses a limited range when in singing voice (usually up to F3). 3 "Limited Range Singer" exhibits use of limited singing range (usually 03 to F3). 3.5 "Inconsistent Initial Range Singer" sometimes only exhibits use of limited singing range, but other times exhibits use of initial singing range (usually 03 to A3). 4 "Initial Range Singer" exhibits use of initial singing range (usually 03 to A3). 4.5 "Inconsistent Singer" sometimes only exhibits use of initial singing range, but other times exhibits use of extended singing range (sings beyond the register lift: B3-flat and above). 5 "Singer" exhibits use of extended singing range (sings beyond the register lift: B3- flat and above). The raters also noted that it may be more appropriate to have children sing 8 patterns rather than 4 in order to provide a more valid assessment of their singing voices. Version 3 The revised version of SVDM(Version 3), developed and used in a study conducted during the 1991-1992 school year (Rutkowski, 1993a, 1993b), is presented in Figure 3. As can be seen, the additional rating levels allowed for more accurate evaluation of the children's use of singing voice and 8 patterns were included rather than 4. The question of whether to evaluate children's use of singing voice with text or neutral syllable performances, raised in previous studies (Goetze, 1985; Levinowitz, 1989; Rutkowski, 1990b; Smale, 1987, White, Sergeant, & Welch, 1997), was investigated also. First grade children's singing performances were tape-recorded as in previous studies. Each child sang the 8 patterns on both the neutral syllable "bum" and text. Half of the children sang the patterns first with words and then with the neutral syllable; the other half of the children sang the neutral syllable first. The same two raters who evaluated children's singing performances in the previous Rutkowski studies were employed for this investigation as well. Two tape recordings were prepared for each rater: One with the word performances and the other with the neutral syllable performances. Although these raters had already been shown to have high intra-rater reliability, it was felt that the refining of the singing stages on SVDMwarranted a re-examination of each rater's consistency with the scale. Therefore, 16 performances were repeated on each tape to allow for investigation of intra-rater reliability. The raters completed their evaluations privately; they were not together while rating the performances. Pearson product moment correlation coefficients were computed to determine inter- and intra-rater reliability for SVDM.Inter-rater reliabilities ranged from.80 to.85 while intra-rater reliabilities ranged from.82 to.93. These coeffi- 206

Figure 3 SVOM: Version 3 RATING SCALE 1 "Pre-singer" does not sing but chants the song text 1.5 "Inconsistent Speaking Range Singer" sometimes chants, sometimes sustains tones and exhibits some sensitivity to pitch but remains in the speaking voice range (usually A2 to C3) 2 "Speaking Range Singer" sustains tones and exhibits some sensitivity to pitch but remains in the speaking voice range (usually A2 to C3) 2.5 "Inconsistent Limited Range Singer" waivers between speaking and singing voices and uses a limited range when in singing voice (usually up to F3) 3 "Limited Range Singer" exhibits consistent use of limited singing range (usually D3to F3) 3.5 "Inconsistent Initial Range Singer" sometimes only exhibits use of limited singing range, but other times exhibits use of initial singing range (usually D3 to A3) 4 "Initial Range Singer" exhibits consistent use of initial singing range (usually D3 to A3) 4.5 "Inconsistent Singer" sometimes only exhibits use of initial singing range, but other times exhibits use of extended singing range (sings beyond the register lift: B3-f1at and above) 5 "Singer" exhibits use of extended singing range (sings beyond the register lift: B3- flat and above) PATTERNS: ~& i See the bird; in the uee; see it Oy; 0 "er me. ~.. Look up now; id the sly, there it goes; Oy idg by. cients indicate that the raters used the SVDMsimilarly when rating the children's singing voice performances. While the raters were not as consistent with each other as they have been in previous studies, coefficients from.80 to.85 still indicate a reliable measure. An analysis of variance was performed to determine if a significant difference existed between the neutral syllable and text performances. Though not statistically significant (p ~.07), the mean for neutral syllable performances (7.032) was higher than the mean for word performances (6.436). It was concluded that first graders did not perform significantly better on SVDMwhen using a neutral syllable than when using words. However, in many 207

instances, individual children scored 2 to 6 points higher when singing on a neutral syllable, a noticeable difference on a 9-point scale. It seems that some children sing better when they are not asked to sing words, perhaps since singing words is too closely related to their speech patterns, while other children do not find singing words a problem. Therefore, it is strongly recommended that both types of performances be utilized for evaluating children's use of singing voice. Conclusions and Recommendations In conclusion, it appears that the hypothesis regarding use of singing voice as a separate but requisite behavior to the ability to sing with accurate intonation is well-founded. Consequently, it seems appropriate for this domain to be considered when singing voice development or achievement is being evaluated. However, since use of singing voice, as measured by SVDM,has been shown to have a weak relationship with tonal aptitude, as measured by PMMA,teachers and researchers need to be aware that singing ability does not necessarily reflect a child's music ability. The revised SVDMappears to be a valid measurement instrument to assess use of singing voice. It seems that the 9-point scale is functional and easily used by raters and that the task of echoing 8 short tonal patterns on text and a neutral syllable, that exhibit various levels of singing voice development, is appropriate. Reference List Bentley, A. (1968). Monotones: a comparison with normal singers in terms if incidence and musical abilities. Music Education Research papers NO.1. London: Novello & Co., Ltd. Davidson, L., McKernon, P., & Gamer, H. (1981). The acquisition of song: A developmental approach. In Documentary Report of the Ann Arbor Symposium (pp. 301-315). Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference. Forcucci, S. L. (1975, October). Help for inaccurate singers. Music Educators Journal, 57-61. Gaiser, P. E. (1961). A study of tone-matching techniques as remedial instruction for nonsingers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR Goetze, M. (1985). Factors affecting accuracy in children's singing. Dissertation Abstracts International, 46, 2955A. (University Microfilms No. DA8528488) Gordon, E. E. (1971). The psychology of music teaching. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Gordon, E. E. (1979). Primary measures of music audiation. Chicago: G.I.A. Publications. Hartzell, RE. (1949). An exploratory study of tonality apprehension and tonal memory in young children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH. Joyner, D. R (1969). The monotone problem. Journal of Research in Music Education, 17(1), 115-124. Kirkpatrick, W C. (1962). Relationships between the singing ability of pre-kindergarten children and their home musical environment. Dissertation Abstracts, 23, 886. (University Microfilms No. 62-3736) Levinowitz, L. M. An experimental study of the comparative effects of singing songs with words and without words on children in kindergarten and first grade. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 863A. (University Microfilms No. DA8716497) McKenzie, J. S. (1948, March-April). New methods for nonsingers. Educational Music Magazine, 20-21, 52-54. Nye, R E., & Nye, V.T. (1970). Music in the elementary school (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 208

Reuter, G. S. (I 956a, January-February). Remedial treatment of inaccurate singers. Educational Music Magazine, 19-20,56-61. Reuter, G. S. (l956b, March-April). Remedial treatment of inaccurate singers: Part II. Educational Music Magazine, 41-45. Rutkowski, J. (1984). Development of a rating scale to assess individual children's use of the vocal instrument. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY. Rutkowski, J. (1986). The effect of restricted song range on kindergarten children's use of singing voice and developmental music aptitude. Dissertation Abstracts International, 47, 4706A. (University Microfilms No. DA8619357) Rutkowski, J. (l990a). The measurement and evaluation of children's singing voice development. The Quarterly: Center for Research in Music Learning and Teaching, I (1-2),81-95. Rutkowski, J. (l990b). The comparative effective of individual and group singing activities on kindergarten children's use of singing voice and developmental music aptitude. Paper presented at the national meeting of the Music Educators National Conference, Washington DC. Rutkowski, J. (l993a). The effectiveness of frequency of instruction and individuavsmallgroup singing activities within the traditional large group setting on first graders' use of singing voice and developmental music aptitude. Unpublished paper presented at the Symposium on Research in General Music, Tucson AZ. Rutkowski, J. (l993b). The use of words versus a neutral syllable for evaluating children's use of singing voice. Unpublished paper presented at the Eastern divisional meeting of the Music Educators National Conference, Springfield MA. Rutkowski, J. (1996). The effectiveness of individuavsmall group singing activities on kindergartners' use of singing voice and developmental music aptitude. Journal of Research in Music Education, 44(4), 353-368. Smale, M. J. (1987). An investigation of pitch accuracy of four-and five-year-old singers. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 2013A. (University Microfilms No. DA872385I) WeIch, G. F. (1979). Poor pitch singing: A review of the literature. Psychology of Music, 7(1), 50-58. White, P.J., Sergeant, D.C., and WeIch, G.F. Some observations on the singing development of five-year-olds. In Singing Development: Childhood and Change An Overview (pp. 15-18). London: Roehampton Institute. Young, W T. (1971). An investigation of the singing abilities of kindergarten and first-grade children in East Texas. (Report No. PS-006-178). Nagodoches, TX: Stephen F. Austin State University. 209