Times to Fight and Times to Relax: Singing and Humming at the Beginnings of Human Evolutionary History

Similar documents
Aposematic Model vs. Sexual Selection Model of Human Evolution

Sexual Selection Or Natural Selection?

Times to Fight: Music and War

Screech, Hoot, and Chirp: Natural Soundscapes and Human Musicality

Anthro 1401, University of Utah Evolution of Human Nature Study Guide. Alan Rogers

Music, Culture and the Evolution of the Human Mind: Looking Beyond Dichotomies

Music s Place in Evolutionary Psychology

Programme PROGRAMME. IAST-Fondation Royaumont Conference: The Origins of Music in Human Society. Abbaye de Royaumont, December 14-16, 2017

What Can Experiments Reveal About the Origins of Music? Josh H. McDermott

A Musical Species. By Caroline Atkinson

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A.

Supplemento n. 6 a «Illuminazioni» n. 18 (ottobre-dicembre 2011) Alessandra Anastasi THE SINGING OF PRIMATES

PUBLIC SCHOOLS OF EDISON TOWNSHIP DIVISION OF CURRICULUM AND INSTRUCTION. Chamber Choir/A Cappella Choir/Concert Choir

Voices From the Deep. Description. Objectives. Essential Questions. Background Information

Toward a New Comparative Musicology. Steven Brown, McMaster University

What is music as a cognitive ability?

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

Cohesion-based theories of the evolution of human musical. behaviour

Review of Bug Music: How Insects Gave Us Rhythm and Noise. David Rothenberg Picador pp., Paperback

The Ur Song and its Impact on Music Therapy Russtanna Faimon Mentor: Dr. Ronald Crocker University of Nebraska at Kearney College of Fine and

Music s Physical and Mental Influence on Humans

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

Course Description: Required Texts:

Can parents influence children s music preferences and positively shape their development? Dr Hauke Egermann

The Origins of Future Consciousness

BIBB 060: Music and the Brain Tuesday, 1:30-4:30 Room 117 Lynch Lead vocals: Mike Kaplan

THESE SEVEN COMMENTARIES, KINDLY SOLICITED THOUGHTS ON AN EMPIRICAL APPROACH TO THE EVOLUTIONARY ORIGINS OF MUSIC

Primates have been laughing for 10m years

What are the true functions of creation stories (myths)? How should they be viewed today?

Age differences in women s tendency to gossip are mediated by their mate value

Sound UNIT 9. Discussion point

Part 1: A Summary of the Land Ethic

Literature: Words across the Universe

2008 ENG Edited by

SOCI 421: Social Anthropology

Table of Contents UNIT 1: THE BIG PICTURE OF MUSIC Music for Everyone What Is Music? Writing & Reading Music Lessons...

12/7/2018 E-1 1

Chapter 12: Introduction to Module 2 Evolution

Objectives: Performance Objective: By the end of this session, the participants will be able to discuss the weaknesses of various theories that suppor

Keynote speech evolutionary biology Example of an existing collaboration and highlight of recent research results A Keynote

This excerpt from. The Origins of Music. Nils L. Wallin, Björn Merker and Steven Brown, editors The MIT Press.

9.20 M.I.T Lecture #26 Critique of Cultural determinism

Affective response to a set of new musical stimuli W. Trey Hill & Jack A. Palmer Psychological Reports, 106,

THE NEUROSCIENCES AND MUSIC III DISORDERS AND PLASTICITY. The Music Instinct. The Evolutionary Basis of Musicality. Steven Mithen

Why Creativity is Sexy: A Review of the Evidence of Sexual Selection for Creative Abilities in Humans

THE IRON MAN VISUAL STORY

Region 11 Math & Science Teacher Academy Partnership Life Science 7-12 August 11, 2011

Tinnitus: How an Audiologist Can Help

Existing Music Science

Research Paper Instructions Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Spring 2010

To what extent can we apply the principles of evolutionary theory to storytelling?

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

December 16, 1997 Outline

Patel, Iversen, Bregman & Schulz 1. Studying synchronization to a musical beat in nonhuman animals

The laughing brain - Do only humans laugh?

Björn Merker: neuroscientist/zoömusicologist

Thoughts and Emotions

Living With Each Energy Type

Music in Therapy for the Mentally Retarded

EVOLUTION OF A DHH DRUM CLUB DORAN BLAKESLEE, LCSW SWCCCASE


JUSTUS AND HUTSLER (2005) and McDermott and MULTIMODAL AFFECTIVE INTERACTION: A COMMENT ON MUSICAL ORIGINS

1 Introduction. Peter Carruthers and Andrew Chamberlain

DEMOGRAPHIC DIFFERENCES IN WORKPLACE GOSSIPING BEHAVIOUR IN ORGANIZATIONS - AN EMPIRICAL STUDY ON EMPLOYEES IN SMES

The Theory of Mind Test (TOM Test)

Music, Art, and Biology: Critical Analysis and Creative Response Courtney Brown. Class Description

WHY DO PEOPLE CARE ABOUT REPUTATION?

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

Mating Intelligence, Moral Virtues, and Methodological Vices

What counts as a convincing scientific argument? Are the standards for such evaluation

Running head: FACIAL SYMMETRY AND PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS 1

Choral Sight-Singing Practices: Revisiting a Web-Based Survey

Honors Thesis Proposal. Beethoven s Violin Sonata Op. 23: Freedom of Interpretation in Passages of Formal Anomaly

Alex. 1 Early life 2 Training 3 Accomplishments 4 Death 5 Criticisms 6 See also 7 References 8 External links

Seed Grant In Performance: Johann Nauwach s Teütscher Villanellen

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

Free Ebooks How The Mind Works

The Moral Animal. By Robert Wright. Vintage Books, Reviewed by Geoff Gilpin

Short Bounce Rolls doubles, triples, fours

Week. self, peer, or other performances 4 Manipulate their bodies into the correct

PHILOSOPHICAL APPLICATIONS OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE COURSE STRUCTURE

BIOS 3010: Ecology, Dr Stephen Malcolm

English Speaking Training - e-booklet

Elizabeth K. Schwartz, MA, LCAT, MT-BC

2010 CCAFL Croatian Continuers Sample Answers

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Is Hegel s Logic Logical?

RE: ELECTIVE REQUIREMENT FOR THE BA IN MUSIC (MUSICOLOGY/HTCC)

KINDERGARTEN-CURRICULUM MAP

Tonality: The Shape of Affect

Hip Hop, Yoga, & Letter Writing: Engaging the Brain and the Body to Support Trauma Work

KEY STAGE 3 MUSIC PROJECTS

The Philosophy of Human Evolution

MUSIC COURSE OF STUDY GRADES K-5 GRADE

Stephen Davies, The Artful Species: Aesthetics, Art, and Evolution (2013)

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

The Evolution of Music in Comparative Perspective

Tinnitus can be helped. Let us help you.

Bas C. van Fraassen, Scientific Representation: Paradoxes of Perspective, Oxford University Press, 2008.

The Adaptive Function of the Arts

Transcription:

Times to Fight and Times to Relax: Singing and Humming at the Beginnings of Human Evolutionary History ABSTRACT The article discusses the function of music in human evolution and suggests that music initially had a binary form of expression, serving several important adaptive functions of physical survival. These two main forms of expressions were: (1) Loud and rhythmically-precise organized singing, coupled with threatening body movements, loud drumming and stone throwing. This form was a key factor for (a) defending hominids from predators, (b) providing them with protein-rich food via aggressive scavenging, and (c) strengthening bonds between the group members; (2) Soft and gentle humming, which was a key factor to allow hominids (1) to maintain contact within group, (2) to watch out for predators, and (3) to relax. JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 1

After the long period of neglect, the scholarly body of works on the evolutionary origins of music is experiencing a rapid and fruitful growth. This process started with the appearance of the collection of articles The Origins of Music (Wallin, Merker, Brown 2000) and many publications followed. This article reviews the main existing hypotheses on the evolutionary origins of music, put forward by scholars of various generations: Spencer, 1857, Charles Darwin, 1871, Richard Wallaschek, 1891, Otto Jespersen, 1895, Ernst Newman, 1905, Carl Stumpf, 1911, 1943, Carl Bucher, 1919, Siegfried Nadel, 1930, Curt Sachs, 1943, Miron Kharlap, 1972, John Blacking, 1973, Roger Wescott, 1973, Ivan Fonagy, 1981, Juan Roederer, 1984, Bruce Richmann, 1993, John Barrow, 1996, Dan Sperber, 1996, Steven Pinker, 1997, Nathan Kogan, 1996, Geoffrey Miller, 2000, Steven Brown, 2000, 2003, Bjorn Merker, 2000, Robin Dunbar, 1996, 2004, Ellen Dysannayake, 2000, Francua- Bernard Masche, 2000, William Benzon, 2001, Edward Hagen and Gregory Bryant, 2003, Steven Mithen, 2005, Timothy Justus and Geffrey Hatsler, 2005, Josh McDermott and Marc Hauser, 2003, 2005, Bruno Nettl, 2005, Victor Grauer, 2006, 2007, and Tecumseh Fitch, 2006. The author propose firstly comparison of the musical behavior of humans and singing animal species to determined whether there are any features that apply specifically to human musical behavior alone. A few of such unique features are discussed in the article (some of them for the first time). Humans are the only singing species on our planet that live on the ground. All the singing species known today (avian and humming birds, parrots, whales and dolphins, bats, sea lions and seals, to name some) live in the trees, or in the water. This unique feature of human singing behavior has never been discussed before, and the author suggests that this could be the crucial factor in determining the origins of human musicality. In order to explain this fact, the author looks at the differences in living conditions on the trees and on the ground. The biggest difference is the predator threat in the trees and on the ground. Trees allow different animal species with different body sizes to live at different heights from the ground, according to their weight. Lighter animals can live higher, on thinner branches of the trees, avoiding contact with bigger (and heavier) predators. Unlike trees, living on the ground does not allow such differentiation of the living space for the animals according to their body weight, so all the ground animals, from small rabbits to much larger leopards, lions and huge elephants spend all their lives on the same ground level. This puts enormous survival pressure on singing behavior of the species living on the ground. It is widely known that singing (and generally loud vocalizations) is very dangerous for animals, as singers are revealing their whereabouts to all potential predators. Unlike ground living species, tree living species do not have to worry about being heard, as most of the predators cannot reach them anyway. Only when the tree-living singing species go down to the ground do they become vulnerable to the attacks of the ground predators. An important fact that confirms this idea is that virtually all birds that spend the day singing and freely JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 2

communicating with each other in the trees will stop making sounds when they come to the ground. The author suggests that this difference between the living conditions in the trees and on the ground must be the reason why, of the more 4500 singing species that populate our planet, most live in trees, a few live in water, and only one species (Homo sapiens) lives on the ground. In a recent publication (Jordania, 2006, 2007) the author suggested that early hominids started using loud rhythmic singing/shouting, accompanied by vigorous threatening body movements and object throwing to defend themselves from predators. Loud screaming, threatening movements, drumming and object throwing are well-known among African apes when they are confronted with deadly predators or competitors. The power of loud group vocalization is widely known from cases where a large group of unarmed shouting humans can scare away a hungry man-eating tiger from its prey, or when a shouting human group can drive large and dangerous animals towards the intended place. Tomas Geissmann, following the model of behavior of apes in critical situations, wrote about the possibility of the origins of human singing as a means to scare away aggressors and competitors (Geissmann, 2000), and Bruno Nettl also mentioned the same possibility (Nettl, 2005:265). Human musical behavior includes another well-known element, unknown among other singing species the presence of the precise rhythm. Rhythmic unity brought a few new important features into human defensive singing and made it much more efficient: (1) loud singing/shouting is physically louder if it is precisely organized rhythmically; (2) rhythmically well-organized group vocalizations send a strong message to the predator (aggressor, competitor) about the unity and determination of the group to fight; and (3) doing repetitive physical actions in a big group in precise rhythmic unity (working, marching) is an extremely effective way to create a strong bond between the members of a human group. For example, long hours of military drill have proven to be the best way to transform a group of new recruits into a well-bonded and determined group of soldiers (McNeill, 1995). Contemporary study of the use of music by the USA military personnel in Iraq also revealed the tremendous power of loud rhythmic music in preparing soldiers for the combat operation (Pieslak, 2009). An important addition to the audio-visual defense must have been the throwing of various objects at the predator (if the rhythmic shouting/singing could not stop their aggression). Works of William Calvin (1982, 1993) are of special importance in this regard, although Calvin mostly studied object throwing as a means of hunting, mostly neglecting the defensive potential of throwing in hominid prehistory. With the use of rhythmically-unified loud singing, threatening body movements and object throwing, our hominid ancestors could obtain protein-rich food as well. The idea of early hominids being mostly scavengers initially came from Louis Binford (1986) and is accepted by many today. The notion of confrontational scavenging (Blumenschine, 1986) fits very well with the reconstructed behavior of early hominids. JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 3

This method of audio and visual intimidation (including object throwing) brings another important element to an understanding of early hominid behavior and even morphology. This was the non-contact method of defense and attack. This new revolutionary method of confrontation can explain some of the well-known morphological changes during the millions of the years of human evolution (such as the decrease of the size and number of hominid teeth, or obtaining a gentle, sensitive and hairless skin). * * * * * The second part of the article Times to relax looks at the evolutionary role of universal and neglected human vocal behavior humming. The author suggest that human humming was the equivalent of the so called contact calls of social animals (Macedonia, 1986, Oda, 1996). Although contact calls might seem to be a haphazard audio result of the big social group being together, they fulfill few important functions: (1) when members of the group hear these contact calls, they are assured that they are among kin and that there is no predator threat at the moment; (2) if any of the members of the animal social group notice anything dangerous in the environment, they stop making the contact calls, stay motionless and start scanning the surroundings. Other members of the group quickly follow suit and soon the whole group is silent, scanning the surroundings in suspicion. After some time, if the animals decide the alarm was false, they resume their activity (for example, feeding) and the low relaxing humming sound comes back. Therefore, it is silence that is perceived as a danger signal by social animals. In the same way, the presence of watchmen s songs among some bird species also demonstrates that it is silence that signals danger for social species. Therefore, the ostensibly insignificant and haphazard sounds of contact calls (or humming among humans) play an important role for the group, carrying the relaxing message everything is all right. There is overwhelming evidence that humming among contemporary humans is the expression of well-being, comfort, enjoyment, agreement. There are people who hum during most of their activities, some hum only sometimes, and others only sing in their heads because of social etiquette. The author suggests that humming is not a recent or haphazard vocal behavior among humans. It has rather been accompanying the social life of our distant ancestors for many millions of the years, allowing them to maintain contact, to relax and to watch out for predators. The vocal grooming hypotheses suggested by Leslie Aiello and Robin Dunbar (1993) fits extremely well with this suggestion of the role of gentle group humming for early hominid groups. JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 4

Lullabies, one of the truly universal musical genres, could be the remnant of the ancient hummed expression of safety and relaxation. The genetic character of lullabies has been suggested by Josh McDermott and Mark Hauser (2003, 2005). Besides expressing positive feelings and relaxation, humming and singing can help a person in physical or emotional stress to alleviate negative feelings and to recover. A few of such cases are discussed in the article. Healing songs are another universal (or near-universal) human musical genre. Musical therapy has a direct link to the comforting and re-assuring power of music coming from our prehistory. An interesting case of a person who can stop her own panic attacks with the help of singing is discussed. The well-known English saying whistling in the dark is also discussed, as it is based on the widely held belief that whistling or singing in a scary situation (in darkness, in the forest, etc) could help a lone traveler. As time passes, there is an increasing amount of so-called background music around us. Today we hear music not only at the concerts, major social events or celebrations, but also at plenty of everyday situations: while shopping, in cars, train, buses and airplanes, at political rallies, sporting events, in elevators, etc. Some complain that hearing music in such places, where people do not really listen to the music, is a sign of decline in musical taste. The author suggests that this kind of highbrow attitude towards music is not historically justified, as music might have started to help our ancestors in very practical ways: to defend themselves from predators, to get protein-rich food, to watch out for predators or to relax. Listening to music for the pleasure (particularly at concerts) is a much later phenomenon. In this context the infamous background music is the evolutionary continuation of the ancient human habit of humming, and it has the worthwhile purpose of helping humans to feel better, to have a more positive attitude, or to fight the fright of small spaces in the elevators. At the end of the article author discusses one of the best known adaptive functions of music sexual attraction of the opposite sex. He suggests that this function seems to be secondary to the other functions of music (defense, obtaining food, relaxing) discussed in this article. Two important features of human music point to this: (1) among most of the singing species that use music to attract the opposite sex, only one sex (usually males) sings, and (2) singing among competing males is understandably solo. In human music, both men and women sing, and singing in groups with precise rhythmic coordination is common. These facts point to the importance of the role of cooperation (not competition) in the origins of human musical behavior. JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 5

REFERENCES Aiello, Leslie, and Robin L. Dunbar. (1993). Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. Current Anthropology 34:184-93. Barrow, John D. (1995). The Artful Universe. The Clarendon Press, Oxford. (second edition: Oxford University Press, 2005) Benzon, William. (2001). Beethoven s Anvil: Music in mind and culture. Oxford, Oxford University Press. Binford, Louis. R. (1986). Human ancestors: Changing views of their behavior. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 3:235-257. Blacking, John. (1973). How musical is Man? Seattle: University of Washington Press. Blumenschine, Robert J. (1986). Early hominid scavenging opportunities: Implications of carcass availability in the Serengeti and Ngorongoro ecosystems. Oxford, England: B.A.R. Brown, Steven. (2000). The Musilanguage model of human evolution. In The origins of music. Edited by Nils Wallin, Bjorn Merker and Steven Brown, pp. 271-300. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Brown, Steven. (2003). Contagious heterophony: A new theory about the origins of music. In The First International Symposium on Traditional Polyphony, 2002. Edited by Rusudan Tsurtsumia and Joseph Jordania, pp. 54-78. Tbilisi State Conservatory. Bücher, Karl. (1919). Arbeit und Rhythmus, Leipzig: Reinicke. Calvin, William H. (1982). Did throwing stones shape hominid brain evolution? Ethology and Sociobiology 3:115-124. Calvin, William H. (1993). The unitary hypotheses: A common neural circuitry for novel manipulations, language, plan-ahead, and throwing? In Cognition in Human Evolution. Edited by Kathleen R. Gibson and Tim Ingold, pp. 230-250. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Darwin, Charles. (1981). The descent of Man. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1981 [1871]. Dissanayake, Ellen. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts in early motherinfant interaction. In The origins of music. Edited by Nils Wallin, Bjorn Merker and Steven Brown, pp. 389-410. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Dunbar, Robin I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. London: Faber and Faber. Dunbar, Robin. (2004). The human story. London: Faber and Faber. Fitch, William Tecumseh. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition 100: 173-215. Fonagy, Ivan. (1981). Emotions, Voice and Music, in Research Aspects on Singing. Publications issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of Music No. 33:51-79. JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 6

Geissman, Thomas. (2000). Gibbon songs and human music from an evolutionary perspective, in The origins of Music. Edited by N. Wallin, B. Merker and S. Brown, pp. 103-124. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Grauer, Victor A. (2006). Echoes of Our Forgotten Ancestors. The World of Music 48(2):5-59. Grauer, Victor A. (2007). New perspectives on the Kalahari debate: A tale of two genomes. Before Farming 2:1-14. Hagen, Edward. H. and Gregory A. Bryant. (2003). Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nature, 14:21-51. Hauser, Marc, and Josh MCdermott (2003). The evolution of the music faculty: A comparative perspective. Nature Neuroscience 6: 663-668. Humming makes you happy. London Zoo electronic Newsletter from 14 th March 2008. The address: <ttp://www.zsl.org/zsl-london-zoo/news/hummingmakes-you-happy, 438, NS.html> Jespersen, Otto. (1983). Progress in language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Co, 1983 [1895]. Jonathan Pieslak. (2009). Sound Targets: American Soldiers and Music in the Iraq War. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. Jordania, Joseph. (2006). Who Asked the First Question? The Origins of Human Choral Singing, Intelligence, Language and Speech. Tbilisi State University, Logos. Jordania, Joseph. (2007). Origins of rhythm and the defence strategy of human ancestors. In The Third International Symposium on Traditional Polyphony, 2006. Edited by Rusudan Tsurtsumia and Joseph Jordania, pp. 55-65. Tbilisi State Conservatory. Justus, Timothy, and Jeffrey J. Hustler. (2005). Fundamental issues in the evolutionary psychology of music: Assessing innateness and domain specificity. Music perception 23:1-27. Kharlap, Miron G. (1972). Russian folk musical system and the problem of origins of music, in Early forms of the art. Convener Sergey IU. Nekliudov. Edited by Eleazar M. Meletinski, pp. 221-274. Moscow: Iskusstvo. (In Russian). Kogan, Nathan. (1997). Reflections on Aesthetics and Evolution Critical Review vol. 11, 2, spring issue. Kortlandt, Adriaan. (1973). Commentary on the article of Gordon Hewes Primate Communication and the Gestural Origin of Language. Current anthropology 14:13-14. MCdermott, Josh and Marc Hauser. (2005). The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and Evolution. Music Perception 23 (1):29-59. Macedonia, Joseph M. (1986). "Individuality in the contact call of the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). American Journal of Primatology," 11 163-179. MCneill, William H. (1995). Keeping together in time: Dance and drill in Human History. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Mache, Fransois-Bernard. (2000). The necessity of and problems with a universal musicology. In The origins of music. Edited by Nils Wallin, Bjorn Merker and Steven Brown, pp. 473-479. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 7

Merker, Bjorn. (2000). Synchronous chorusing and human origins. In The origins of music. Edited by Nils Wallin, Bjorn Merker and Steven Brown, pp. 315-328. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Mithen, Seven. (2005). The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson. Miller, Geoffrey. (2000). Evolution of human music through sexual selection. In The origins of music. Edited by Nils Wallin, Bjorn Merker and Steven Brown, pp. 329-360. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Nadel, Siegfried F. (1930). The origins of music. Musical Quarterly 16:531-546. Nettl, Bruno. (2005). The Study of Ethnomusicology: Thirty-one Issues and Concepts. Second extended edition (First edition 1983). University of Illinois Press. Newman, Ernst. (1969). Spencer, Herbert, on the origin of music. In Newman, musical Studies. New York: Haskell House Publishers, 1969 [1905]. Oda, Ryo. (1996). Effects of Contextual and Social Variables on Contact Call Production in Free-Ranging Ringtailed Lemurs (Lemur Catta), International Journal of Primatology 17, 191-205. Pinker, Steven. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. Richman, Bruce. (1993). On the evolution of speech: singing in the middle term. Current Anthropology 34:721-22. Roederer, Juan. (1984). The search for a survival value of music. Music Perception 1(3):350-56. Sachs, Curt. (1943). The rise of music in the ancient world. New York: Norton. Shoemark, Helen. (2006). Infant-directed singing as a vehicle for regulation rehearsal in the medically fragile full-term infant. Australian Journal of Music Therapy, 17, 54-63. Spenser, Herbert. (1857). The origin and function of music. Frazer Magazine 56:396-408. Sperber, Dan. (1996). Expanding Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Blackwell, Oxford. Stumpf, Carl. (1911). Die Anfange der Music. Leipzig. [Russian edition, Leningrad, 1926]. Wallaschek, Richard. (1891). On the origin of music. Mind 16:375-386. Wallin, Nils, Bjorn Merker, and Steven Brown (Ed.). (2000). The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Wescott, Roger. (1973). Comments to the article of F. Livingstone Did the australopithecines sing? Current Anthropology 14, No 1-2, February-April 1973: 27-28. Wickler, Wolfgang. (1985). Coordination of vigilance in bird groups. The watchman s song hypothesis. Z. Tierpsychol. 69:250-253. JORDANIA, Joseph (2009) 8