We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Similar documents
Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

9.35 Sensation And Perception Spring 2009

Music Representations

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

CTP431- Music and Audio Computing Musical Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology KAIST Juhan Nam

The Physics Of Sound. Why do we hear what we hear? (Turn on your speakers)

CTP 431 Music and Audio Computing. Basic Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology (GSCT) Juhan Nam

I. LISTENING. For most people, sound is background only. To the sound designer/producer, sound is everything.!tc 243 2

Creative Computing II

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

2018 Fall CTP431: Music and Audio Computing Fundamentals of Musical Acoustics

Psychoacoustics. lecturer:

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Music Theory: A Very Brief Introduction

Lecture 1: What we hear when we hear music

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

Note on Posted Slides. Noise and Music. Noise and Music. Pitch. PHY205H1S Physics of Everyday Life Class 15: Musical Sounds

Reference Manual. Using this Reference Manual...2. Edit Mode...2. Changing detailed operator settings...3

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

CSC475 Music Information Retrieval

Musical Signal Processing with LabVIEW Introduction to Audio and Musical Signals. By: Ed Doering

PSYCHOACOUSTICS & THE GRAMMAR OF AUDIO (By Steve Donofrio NATF)

Music Representations

Math and Music: The Science of Sound

Simple Harmonic Motion: What is a Sound Spectrum?

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Fan Noise

Semi-automated extraction of expressive performance information from acoustic recordings of piano music. Andrew Earis

UNIT 1: QUALITIES OF SOUND. DURATION (RHYTHM)

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Affective Sound Synthesis: Considerations in Designing Emotionally Engaging Timbres for Computer Music

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

Musical Sound: A Mathematical Approach to Timbre

BBN ANG 141 Foundations of phonology Phonetics 3: Acoustic phonetics 1

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

Sound Magic Piano Thor NEO Hybrid Modeling Horowitz Steinway. Piano Thor. NEO Hybrid Modeling Horowitz Steinway. Developed by

Music 175: Pitch II. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015

Lecture 2 What we hear: Basic dimensions of auditory experience

Study on the Sound Quality Objective Evaluation of High Speed Train's. Door Closing Sound

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

ADSR AMP. ENVELOPE. Moog Music s Guide To Analog Synthesized Percussion. The First Step COMMON VOLUME ENVELOPES

Loudness and Sharpness Calculation

Foundations and Theory

3b- Practical acoustics for woodwinds: sound research and pitch measurements

Tempo and Beat Analysis

Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant

Harmonic Analysis of the Soprano Clarinet

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Topics in Computer Music Instrument Identification. Ioanna Karydi

Physics and Neurophysiology of Hearing

Digital audio and computer music. COS 116, Spring 2012 Guest lecture: Rebecca Fiebrink

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

On the strike note of bells

Cathedral user guide & reference manual


The Cocktail Party Effect. Binaural Masking. The Precedence Effect. Music 175: Time and Space

Augmentation Matrix: A Music System Derived from the Proportions of the Harmonic Series

EE513 Audio Signals and Systems. Introduction Kevin D. Donohue Electrical and Computer Engineering University of Kentucky

MUSI-6201 Computational Music Analysis

Sound energy and waves

Pitch Perception. Roger Shepard

Music 170: Wind Instruments

August Acoustics and Psychoacoustics Barbara Crowe Music Therapy Director. Notes from BC s copyrighted materials for IHTP

Syllabus: PHYS 1300 Introduction to Musical Acoustics Fall 20XX

Analysing Room Impulse Responses with Psychoacoustical Algorithms: A Preliminary Study

INSTRUCTION SHEET FOR NOISE MEASUREMENT

Music for the Hearing Care Professional Published on Sunday, 14 March :24

Welcome to Vibrationdata

Our Perceptions of Music: Why Does the Theme from Jaws Sound Like a Big Scary Shark?

AUD 6306 Speech Science

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

LESSON 1 PITCH NOTATION AND INTERVALS

Amplitude and Loudness 1

Getting Started with the LabVIEW Sound and Vibration Toolkit

MAutoPitch. Presets button. Left arrow button. Right arrow button. Randomize button. Save button. Panic button. Settings button

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

Linear Time Invariant (LTI) Systems

1aAA14. The audibility of direct sound as a key to measuring the clarity of speech and music

Pitch is one of the most common terms used to describe sound.

Determination of Sound Quality of Refrigerant Compressors

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Music Complexity Descriptors. Matt Stabile June 6 th, 2008

y POWER USER MUSIC PRODUCTION and PERFORMANCE With the MOTIF ES Mastering the Sample SLICE function

... read The Art of Tap Tuning by Roger H. Siminoff (Hal Leonard Publishing).

A FUNCTIONAL CLASSIFICATION OF ONE INSTRUMENT S TIMBRES

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

Tempo and Beat Tracking

Making music with voice. Distinguished lecture, CIRMMT Jan 2009, Copyright Johan Sundberg

A Need for Universal Audio Terminologies and Improved Knowledge Transfer to the Consumer

Lecture 7: Music

Create It Lab Dave Harmon

Spectral Sounds Summary

Chapter 2 Auditorium Acoustics: Terms, Language, and Concepts

Transcription:

PART 2 Sound Pressure Sound Pressure Levels (SPLs) Sound consists of pressure waves. Thus, a way to quantify sound is to state the amount of pressure 1 it exertsrelatively to a pressure level of reference. As the pressure level of reference we usually use the standard threshold of hearing in terms of sound pressure, which is only p 0 = 2 10-5 Pa. We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is p atm = 101.325 kpa 10 5 Pa i.e. p 0 / p atm 2 10-10. The power carried by a travelling wave is proportional to the square of the amplitude, i.e.: 2 P p. Therefore, we can write: P 2 I 10 log = 10 A p p log = 10 log I 0 P = 20 log 2. 0 A p 0 p 0 Thus, we can make a comparison in db of two sounds, in terms of Sound Pressure Levels (SPLs), by: p 2 20 log. p1 The following table will help us to understand the definition of the SPL above: p 2 /p 1 Comparison in SPL (db) p 2 /p 1 Comparison in SPL (db) 1 20 log1 = 0 10 3 20 log10 3 = 60 2 20 log2 6.02 10 6 20 log10 6 = 120 10 20 log10 = 20 10 12 20 log10 12 = 240 0.1 20 log10-1 = -20 10-12 20 log10-12 = -240 1 The pressure is to be understood as the amplitude of the pressure wave. 2 The standard atmosphere, 1atm = 760 mmhg = 101.325 kpa = 1013.25 mb. The millibar is 1mb =100 Pa.

The three basic characteristics of sound. Sounds may generally be characterized by pitch (τονικό ύψος), loudness, and quality or timbre (ποιότης). These are defined below. Pitch or the ear s response to the Frequency (τονικό ύψος) Pitch is the attribute of the auditory sensation by which sounds can be ordered on a musical scale. For most practical purposes, pitch is just the frequency or to be more precise the perceived pitch of a sound is just the ear's response to its frequency 3. For example, middle C in equal temperament (συγκερασµός) = 261.6 Hz. The pitch perception of the human ear is understood to operate basically by the place theory 4, with some sharpening mechanism necessary to explain the remarkably high resolution of human pitch perception. The place theory and its refinements provide plausible models for the perception of the relative pitch of two tones, but do not explain the phenomenon of perfect pitch. A unit used to measure pitch is the cent 5 which is defined so that one equal tempered semitone (συγκερασµένο ηµιτόνιο) is 100 cents. Consequently, one equal tempered octave (συγκερασµένη οκτάβα) is 1200 cents. For the human ear, the just noticeable difference in pitch is about 5 cents. Although for most practical purposes, the pitch of a sound can be said to be simply a measure of its frequency, there are circumstances in which a constant frequency sound can be perceived to be changing in pitch. One of most consistently observed "psychoacoustic" effects is that a sustained high frequency sound (> 2 khz) which is increased steadily in intensity will be perceived to be rising in pitch, whereas a sustained low frequency sound (< 2 khz) will be perceived to be dropping in pitch 6. For example, with an increase of sound intensity from 60 to 90 db, Terhardt found that the pitch of a 6 khz pure tone was perceived to rise over 30 cents. A 200 Hz tone was found to drop about 20 cents in perceived pitch over the same intensity change. Studies with the sounds of musical instruments show less perceived pitch change with increasing intensity. Rossing [3] reports a perceived pitch change of around 17 cents for a change from 65 db to 95 db. This perceived change can be upward or downward, depending upon which harmonics (generally also overtones) are predominant. For example, if the majority of the intensity comes from harmonics which are above 2 khz, the perceived pitch shift will be upward. The perception of the pitch of short pulses differs from that of sustained sounds of the same measured frequency. If a short pulse of a pure tone is decaying in amplitude, it will be perceived to be higher in pitch than an identical pulse which has steady amplitude. Interfering tones or noise can cause an apparent pitch shift. Further discussion of these and other perceptual aspects of pitch may be found in Ref. [3]. Perfect pitch or absolute pitch («απόλυτο ους» 7 ) is the ability of some persons to recognize the pitch (frequency) without any given frequency of reference. In other words, as if the person can recognize a pitch like the eye discerns the colour of an object. Most persons apparently have only a sense of relative pitch and can recognize a musical interval 8, but not an isolated pitch. Rossing [3] suggests that less than 0.01% of the population appear to be able to recognize absolute pitches, whereas over 98% of the population can do the corresponding visual task of recognizing colours with no colour standard present. Sound Loudness versus Sound Intensity Sound loudness, L, is a subjective term describing the strength of the ear's perception of a sound. To obtain the subjective sound loudness, the objective sound intensity must be factored by the subjective ear's sensitivity, s, which is a function of frequency. Τhe equal loudness curves depict the variation of the average human ear s sensitivity. If 1000 Hz is chosen as a standard frequency, then each equal loudness curve can be referenced to the db level at 1000 Hz. This is the basis for the measurement of sound loudness in phons. If a given sound is perceived to be as loud as a 60 db tone at 1000 Hz, then it is said to have a loudness of 60 phons, i.e.: 60 phons perceived as loud as {60 db, 1000 Hz}, 3 Notice that only periodic or nearly periodic sounds have pitch: we cannot ascribe a pitch to noise. 4 For details see paragraph «Inner Ear Place Theory Organ of Corti Basilar Membrane etc» below. 5 For details see paragraph «Cents, Equal Tempered Octave, Equal Tempered Semitone etc» below. 6 Sometimes called «Stevens s rule» after an early investigator, this psychoacoustic effect has been extensively investigated. 7 Απόλυτο αυτί είναι δηλαδή η αναγνώριση του τονικού ύψους (συχνότητας) χωρίς να δίνεται συχνότητα αναφοράς. 8 For details see paragraph «Musical Scales, Musical Interval etc» below.

i.e. schematically we can write: L(f) = I(f) s(f), (L in phons and I in db). That is, we could define s(f) as the fraction L(f) / I(f) and measure it in phons/db. For example, a sound of {12 db, 4 khz} is perceived by the human sound sensory system the same as loud as a sound of {20 db, 1kHz}. Then, we say that the sound {12 db, 4 khz} has a loudness of 20 phons. The characterization of sound loudness with phons has the problem that this scale is not proportional to our feeling of sound loudness. This means that the human sound perception system has the following idiotropy or quirk («rule of thumb»): the sound intensity must be increased by a factor of 10, for the sound to be perceived as twice as loud! A common way of stating it is that «it takes 10 violins to sound twice as loud as one violin». Schematically: 10 times STIMULUS 2 times FEELING (Sound Intensity) (Sound Loudness) Although this rule is widely used, it must be emphasized that it is an approximate general statement. 10 times STIMULUS (Sound Intensity) 2 times FEELING (Sound Loudness) This «rule of thumb» is applicable only to adding loudness of identical sounds. If a second sound is enough separated in frequency to be outside the critical band 9 of the first, then this rule does not apply. The rule is not precise even for the increase of the same sound. 9 For details see paragraph «Adding Loudness, Critical Band and Critical Band Measurement etc» below.

To express the above stated idiotropy, the sone scale was created to provide such a linear scale of sound loudness. Now, it is usually presumed that the standard range for orchestral music is about 40 to 100 phons. If the lower end of that range (40 phons) is arbitrarily assigned a loudness of 1 sones, then 50 phons would have a loudness of 2 sones, 60 phons would be 4 sones, etc. The mathematical relation between phon and sone and some characteristic steps of the phon- and sone- scales are presented in the following table: Dynamic Level Phons Sones fff 100 64... 90 32 Phon = 40 + 10 log 2 (Sone) f 80 16 phon 40... 70 8 Sone = 2 10 p 60 4... 50 2 ppp 40 1 For example a sound of 8 sones is perceived by the human sound perception system twice as loud as a sound of 4 sones, and half as loud as a sound of 16 sones etc. 1000 100 Sones versus Phons The figure on the right hand describes the above mentioned relation of sones versus phons. Question: up to what extend especially very loud and very soft sounds is this figure experimentally verified? sones 10 1 0.1 0.01-50 0 50 100 150 phons A clearer view of the equal loudness curves which represent the sensitivity of the ear is given in the icon on the left. Another example for the perception of loudness: a sound of {110 db, 30 Hz} is perceived by the human sound sensory system the same as loud as a sound of {90 db, 1 khz}. Then, we say that the sound {110 db, 30 Hz} has a loudness of 90 phons.

Quality or Timbre or Chroma (ποιότης) Sound quality or timbre or tonal colour (χρώµα) describes those characteristics of sound which allow the ear to distinguish sounds which have the same pitch and loudness. Timbre is mainly characterized by the harmonic content and the dynamic properties of the sound such as vibrato/tremolo and the attack-decay envelope. Some investigators report that it takes a duration of about 60 ms to recognize the timbre of a tone, and that any tone shorter than about 4 ms is perceived as an atonal click. It is suggested that it takes about a 4 db change in mid or high harmonics to be perceived as a change in timbre, whereas about 10 db of change in one of the lower harmonics is required. Before defining harmonic content, let us be reminded of some properties of the resonant frequencies. Well, any object has - in general - its resonant frequencies, and: (1) It is easy to get an object to vibrate at its resonant frequencies, hard to get it to vibrate at other frequencies. (2) A vibrating object will pick out its resonant frequencies from a complex excitation and vibrate at those frequencies, essentially "filtering out" other frequencies present in the excitation. (3) Most vibrating objects have many resonant frequencies. The lowest resonant frequency of a vibrating object is called its fundamental frequency. As mentioned before, most vibrating objects have more than one resonant frequency and those used in musical instruments typically vibrate at harmonics of the fundamental. A harmonic is defined as an integer multiple of the fundamental frequency, i.e. the nth harmonic is such that f n = n f 1, where f 1 is the fundamental frequency. Harmonic content is the number and relative intensity of the harmonics present in the sound and - for sustained sounds it is the most important of the characteristics of timbre. An overtone is any resonant frequency above the fundamental frequency. Thus, an overtone may or may not be an harmonic. The term harmonic content may be extended to include overtones which are not harmonics. Many of the instruments of the orchestra, those utilizing strings and air columns, produce the fundamental frequency and harmonics. Vibrating strings, open cylindrical air columns, and conical air columns will vibrate at all harmonics of the fundamental. One-sideclosed cylinders will vibrate with only odd harmonics of the fundamental. In all categories specified above, the overtones can be said to be harmonic. However, sound sources such as membranes or other percussive sources may have resonant frequencies which are not whole number multiples of their fundamental frequencies. They are said to have some non-harmonic overtones.

Notice that it is always possible to characterize a periodic function in terms of harmonics. This is done by the use of the famous Fourier Series, where the periodic function is expressed as a sum of harmonics. In the case of aperiodic functions a generalisation of the Fourier series is used i.e. the Fourier Transform where the sum is replaced by an integral and as a result the aperiodic function is expressed as an integral where also non-harmonics contribute to the function representation 10. The recognition of different vowel sounds of the human voice is largely accomplished by analysis of the harmonic content by the inner ear. Vibrato/Tremolo. Vibrato means periodic changes in the pitch of the tone i.e. vibrato could be called frequency modulation (FM). Tremolo means periodic changes in the amplitude or loudness of the tone i.e. tremolo could be called amplitude modulation (AM) of the tone. Actually, in the voice or the sound of a musical instrument both are usually present to some extent. Vibrato is considered to be a desirable characteristic of the human voice if it is not excessive. It can be used for expression and adds a richness to the voice. If the harmonic content of a sustained sound from a voice or wind instrument is reproduced precisely, the ear can readily detect the difference in timbre because of the absence of vibrato. More realistic synthesized tones will add some type of vibrato and/or tremolo to produce a more realistic tone. Attack and Decay. (επίθεσις-φθίσις) Attack (decay) is how sound enters (leaves) or gets born (dies). References [1] Basic source of information was the site http://hyperphysics.phy-astr.gsu.edu/hbase/sound/ and related links. [2] Private Communication: Toufektsis Orestis, Graz Musical Academy. Ορέστης [3] http://www.sfu.ca/sonic-studio/handbook/phon.html [4] See also next paragraphs as indicated in the footnotes. Created by C. Simserides for Sound Properties: part2. Comments should be addressed to csimseri@ifn-magdeburg.de 10 See paragraph «Fourier Series and Fourier Transform etc» below.