Quantitative multidimensional approach of technical pianistic level

Similar documents
The role of the Alexander technique in musical training and performing

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

From quantitative empirï to musical performology: Experience in performance measurements and analyses

Finger motion in piano performance: Touch and tempo

Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

Temporal coordination in string quartet performance

However, in studies of expressive timing, the aim is to investigate production rather than perception of timing, that is, independently of the listene

A PRELIMINARY STUDY ON THE INFLUENCE OF ROOM ACOUSTICS ON PIANO PERFORMANCE

About Giovanni De Poli. What is Model. Introduction. di Poli: Methodologies for Expressive Modeling of/for Music Performance

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Reinhard Gentner, Susanne Gorges, David Weise, Kristin aufm Kampe, Mathias Buttmann, and Joseph Classen

Experiment on adjustment of piano performance to room acoustics: Analysis of performance coded into MIDI data.

Chords not required: Incorporating horizontal and vertical aspects independently in a computer improvisation algorithm

A MULTI-PARAMETRIC AND REDUNDANCY-FILTERING APPROACH TO PATTERN IDENTIFICATION

TMEA ALL-STATE AUDITION SELECTIONS

Hartt School Community Division Clarinet Audition Teacher Resource Packet

Maintaining skill across the life span: Magaloff s entire Chopin at age 77

A Novel System for Music Learning using Low Complexity Algorithms

Robert Alexandru Dobre, Cristian Negrescu

Director Musices: The KTH Performance Rules System

SENIOR SCHOOL MUSIC COURSE OVERVIEW

CS229 Project Report Polyphonic Piano Transcription

Peak experience in music: A case study between listeners and performers

A study of the influence of room acoustics on piano performance

THE SOUND OF SADNESS: THE EFFECT OF PERFORMERS EMOTIONS ON AUDIENCE RATINGS

Book: Fundamentals of Music Processing. Audio Features. Book: Fundamentals of Music Processing. Book: Fundamentals of Music Processing

Multidimensional analysis of interdependence in a string quartet

Speech and Speaker Recognition for the Command of an Industrial Robot

A prototype system for rule-based expressive modifications of audio recordings

The Mathematics of Music and the Statistical Implications of Exposure to Music on High. Achieving Teens. Kelsey Mongeau

Doubletalk Detection

Project Summary EPRI Program 1: Power Quality

Frequencies. Chapter 2. Descriptive statistics and charts

SCORE ANALYZER: AUTOMATICALLY DETERMINING SCORES DIFFICULTY LEVEL FOR INSTRUMENTAL E-LEARNING

On the contextual appropriateness of performance rules

Zooming into saxophone performance: Tongue and finger coordination

Semi-automated extraction of expressive performance information from acoustic recordings of piano music. Andrew Earis

Banff Sketches. for MIDI piano and interactive music system Robert Rowe

CSC475 Music Information Retrieval

xlsx AKM-16 - How to Read Key Maps - Advanced 1 For Music Educators and Others Who are Able to Read Traditional Notation

Exploring Piano Masterworks 3

Practice makes less imperfect: the effects of experience and practice on the kinetics and coordination of flutists' fingers

Maintaining skill across the life span: Magaloff s entire Chopin at age 77

Advanced Orchestra Performance Groups

Music Representations

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

GSA Applicant Guide: Instrumental Music

Controlling Musical Tempo from Dance Movement in Real-Time: A Possible Approach

Modeling memory for melodies

Can the Computer Learn to Play Music Expressively? Christopher Raphael Department of Mathematics and Statistics, University of Massachusetts at Amhers

Mirror neurons: Imitation and emulation in piano performance

A Bayesian Network for Real-Time Musical Accompaniment

Building a Better Bach with Markov Chains

Lab P-6: Synthesis of Sinusoidal Signals A Music Illusion. A k cos.! k t C k / (1)

Keyboard Theory and Piano Technique

CURRICULUM COURSE GUIDE

PRACTICAL APPLICATION OF THE PHASED-ARRAY TECHNOLOGY WITH PAINT-BRUSH EVALUATION FOR SEAMLESS-TUBE TESTING

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

A STATISTICAL VIEW ON THE EXPRESSIVE TIMING OF PIANO ROLLED CHORDS

Checks for Understanding State Performance Indicators Assessments

Toward an analysis of polyphonic music in the textual symbolic segmentation

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

GSA Applicant Guide: Instrumental Music

Piano Teacher Program

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Clark County School District Las Vegas, Nevada

A Computational Model for Discriminating Music Performers

Salt on Baxter on Cutting

SWING, SWING ONCE MORE: RELATING TIMING AND TEMPO IN EXPERT JAZZ DRUMMING

Hartt School Community Division Oboe Audition Teacher Resource Packet

Music Processing Introduction Meinard Müller

Chapter 5. Describing Distributions Numerically. Finding the Center: The Median. Spread: Home on the Range. Finding the Center: The Median (cont.

CURRICULUM COURSE GUIDE

Music Genre Classification and Variance Comparison on Number of Genres

PIANO GRADES: requirements and information

Visual Encoding Design

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

Audio. Meinard Müller. Beethoven, Bach, and Billions of Bytes. International Audio Laboratories Erlangen. International Audio Laboratories Erlangen

Bichordal Triad Pitch Collection Etudes

THE MAGALOFF CORPUS: AN EMPIRICAL ERROR STUDY

Timing In Expressive Performance

An Empirical Comparison of Tempo Trackers

TMEA ALL-STATE OBOE MASTERCLASSES Before we begin with the selections, following are general suggestionsfor preparation:

Florida Performing Fine Arts Assessment Item Specifications for Benchmarks in Course: Chorus 2

Subjective Similarity of Music: Data Collection for Individuality Analysis

Smart Pianist Manual

Page 4 Lesson Plan Exercises Score Pages 50 63

From Score to Performance: A Tutorial to Rubato Software Part I: Metro- and MeloRubette Part II: PerformanceRubette

Music Database Retrieval Based on Spectral Similarity

Analysis and Clustering of Musical Compositions using Melody-based Features

Hidden Markov Model based dance recognition

**This audition is part of your 2nd Marking Period Grade**

Collaboration in the choral context: The contribution of conductor and choir to collective confidence

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

ECG Denoising Using Singular Value Decomposition

The roles of expertise and partnership in collaborative rehearsal

ECONOMICS 351* -- INTRODUCTORY ECONOMETRICS. Queen's University Department of Economics. ECONOMICS 351* -- Winter Term 2005 INTRODUCTORY ECONOMETRICS

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

by Staff Sergeant Samuel Woodhead

Transcription:

International Symposium on Performance Science ISBN 978-94-90306-01-4 The Author 2009, Published by the AEC All rights reserved Quantitative multidimensional approach of technical pianistic level Paul Molin LEAD CNRS, University of Burgundy, France The purpose of this study is to examine whether it is possible to define quantitatively the technical level of a pianist by summarizing the individual characteristics of the pianist s technical ability in a few at most three independent variables. These variables could be used in competitions and examinations as well as by piano teachers to measure students progress, or by the students themselves in their daily work. Keywords: pianistic level; principal component analysis; pianist motor skill; correlation of difficulties; MIDI A statistical approach to measuring the biomechanical characteristics of piano playing was used by Altenmüller and Jabusch (2005) to measure pianist s focal dystonia, then by the same authors (2007, 2009) to measure the development of adults and children s motor skill, using the Musical Instrument Digital Interchange (MIDI) recording format. An ordinary MIDI file contains a succession of events, each event being characterized by three parameters: occurrence time of the event (T), duration (D), and pitch (P). From these parameters, many characteristics (tone overlap, equality of produced sound velocity, equality of rhythm, etc.) can be computed, each variable giving an insight of the technical level of a pianist. The advantages and disadvantages of MIDI coding are widely discussed by Clarke (2004). Keyboard technical skill and how to improve it was studied early, for example by J. S. Bach (Klavierübung) and D. Scarlatti (Exercizi), and C. P. E Bach wrote a treatise Essai sur la véritable manière de jouer les instruments à clavier in 1762. A systematic study of piano playing difficulties was completed by Alfred Cortot (1928), with the author recommending a detailed daily work based on five-finger exercises, scales, arpeggios, trills, double notes, and octaves, with daily key transposition. This well known book is the basis of the daily training of pianists. Our purpose is, from a sample of these typical training patterns, to ex-

72 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG tract the qualities and flaws of a pianist, and summarize them by synthetic graphs and synthetic variables. Participants METHOD Eighteen pianists (mean age 22.5 years, ranging from 12-59), from middle level music conservatories to professional level participated in the experiment. They had played piano for a mean of 14.5 years (range=4-52) and received either course credit or payment for their participation. Materials The pianists performed on a classical upright piano (Yamaha U2) with MIDI interface. MIDI data were collected on a DELL laptop PC, by using the Cubase SX program. All statistic programs were written in the Matlab programming language by members of the LEAD-CNRS laboratory. Procedure The pianists were submitted to a series of exercises containing the most common classical difficulties in piano playing (scales, trills, and arpeggios). In order to obtain a photograph of the pianist, a few attempts only (up to 5) were authorized for each exercise. The tempo of each exercise was free. The pianists were allowed 20 minutes for warming up and getting used to the piano prior to the actual performances. Not all the pianists used this warm up time. An operator was always present to guide the participants, verify the fingering, and record the performances. The detailed exercises were as follows: Four scales (Eb major, E major, Bb major, Eb minor) were requested from participants, played on four octaves twice without interruption, hands together at a pitch distance of one octave. The choice of scales was motivated by the fact that these scales have a very natural fingering, so all pianists used the same fingering. There were two easy scales (Eb major, E major), one more difficult (Bb major), and a difficult scale (Eb minor). All the possible combinations of fingers (1-2, 1-3, 4-5) were used for the trill exercise. Eight arpeggios (C major, C major 4/6, Db major, Db major 4/6, C# minor, C# minor 4/6, Bb minor, Bb minor 4/6), played on four octaves twice without interruption, hands together at a pitch distance of one octave.

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 73 Fingering was verified by the operator to be 1-2-3 for the right hand and 1-4-2 for the left hand for the natural arpeggio state, 1-2-4 for right hand and 1-3-2 for left hand in the upward direction, and the reverse for the down direction. RESULTS Defining and computing the relevant variables For all exercises, the first indices of quality are a high tempo and few false notes. But these are not the only qualities of a good piano technique; for scales and arpeggios, the main word characterizing the quality of playing is homogeneity. This means rhythmic homogeneity for each hand, rhythmic homogeneity between hands, homogeneity of velocity (intensity of key depressing force) for each hand, velocity homogeneity between hands, and overlap homogeneity (legato homogeneity). Of course, in an artistic performance of a piece of music, this homogeneity is not observed; all the discrepancies in an artistic performance are voluntary acts, guided by the artistic sensibility. But the homogeneity required in the exercises is the way to gain the control of the movements, in order to play with expression. We then retained seven variables: (1) tempo (TPO), (2) tempo coefficient of variation (CVTPO), (3) number of false notes (FN), (4) intra-hand velocity standard deviation (STDVEL), (5) intra-hand overlap standard deviation (STDOVL), (6) lag time between hands (LAGLR), and (7) velocity difference between hands (DVELLR). For trills, only the first five variables were used, since the exercise was performed hand-by-hand. Why a free tempo? In the experiments of Jabusch et al. (2004, 2007, 2009), the authors fixed the tempo of the exercises (scales) and allowed the subjects to repeat them 10-15 times. They then computed a mean scale for each subject. Our point of view is slightly different. An important part of the technical level is the pianist s awareness of his own skill e.g. I will not begin a scale at a metronomic tempo of 160 if I know that I will not finish the scale. We then left the subjects free to choose their tempos and considered the tempo variable as a level variable among others. Principal component analysis (PCA) (Hotelling 1933) for scales We present complete results and graphs for the Eb major scale. Table 1 shows the correlation matrix of the seven variables for the scale. It is not surprising

74 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG Table 1. Correlation matrix between the seven quality variables for Eb major scale. Tempo False Inter hand Intra hand CV tempo Overlap Time lag notes velocity variations velocity STD STD between hands (TPO) (FN) (DVELLR) (STDVEL) (CVTPO) (STDOVL) (LAGLR) TPO 1-0.179-0.610-0.165-0.468-0.503-0.198 FN -0.179 1-0.024 0.422 0.682 0.187 0.686 DVELLR -0.610-0.024 1 0.291 0.128 0.516 0.134 STDVEL -0.165 0.422 0.291 1 0.220 0.242 0.724 CVTPO -0.468 0.682 0.128 0.220 1 0.540 0.432 STDOVL -0.503 0.187 0.516 0.242 0.540 1 0.159 LAGLR -0.198 0.686 0.134 0.724 0.432 0.159 1 that the tempo variable is negatively correlated with all other variables: intuitively, tempo is a positive index of quality, while the other variables are negatives. Some other correlations, intuitively expected, can be noticed: for example, the high correlation (0.682) between the false notes and CV. Tempo variables can be understood as a false note induces a stress for the pianist, then an irregularity of tempo. In the same way, the high correlation between the CV tempo and the time lag can be understood as a false note in one hand does not affect the other hand, and then increases the time lag between hands. Figure 1 shows the graphic results of the Eb major PCA, which is consistent with respect to the correlation matrix. The left part of the figure is the correlation circle, which represents the correlations between variables and principal components. The first two principal components account for about 70% of the total variance of the data, and thus the bi-dimensional representation is reliable. As expected, the tempo variable is opposed to all others. It is very interesting to see that the correlation between tempo and false notes is low (-0.179). A good pianist can do many wrong notes a known joke of Alfred Cortot: if you picked all my false notes in a concerto, it could be a second concerto. In order to interpret the graph of subjects, let us describe some of them: pianists 3, 4, and 9 are beginning the second cycle of studies in a regional conservatory, pianist 18 is a concert pianist, pianist 2 is a piano teacher, and pianist 1 is an amateur (the author). All other pianists are second or third cycle students in a regional conservatory. Then the first principal component

INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON PERFORMANCE SCIENCE 75 Table 2. Metronomic and adjusted tempos (scores) for the Eb major scale. Subject 1 2 3 4 5 7 9 10 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 Tempo 86.7 79.2 65.9 31.0 36.7 71.7 54.5 41.8 35.6 64.8 85.5 94. 6 71.7 64. 2 143.3 Score 63.0 59.8 26.7 1.7 16.6 50.0 13.8 25.1 7.1 30.1 62.7 64. 0 49.2 38. 7 125.0 Figure 1. Principal component analysis of the Eb major scale. The right panel represents the variables and the left panel the subjects. can be easily interpreted as the technical level axis. The second axis can describe more precisely the flaws of the pianist: for example, a pianist far above the second axis is characterized by many false notes, a bad synchronism between hands, but a good legato (little overlap STD). DISCUSSION The PCA analysis appears to be a good visualization tool for a synthetic presentation of the technical level of a pianist. The results for trills and arpeggios are similar to those of scales. All the default variables are positively correlated, and all negatively correlated with the tempo variable. As we stated in the section on PCA above, the principal components cannot be used as they are, but they suggest an overall evaluation in a form opposing the tempo with all the other variables. For example, for the Eb major scale, the first principal component has the following equation: PC1=-0.3595*TPO+0.3991*FN+0.2860*DVELLR+ 0.3682*STDVEL+0.4331*CVTPO+0.3668*STDOVL+ 0.4147*LAGLH

76 WWW.PERFORMANCESCIENCE.ORG All the coefficients have the same magnitude. The idea is then to give the results to the musician in the form of an adjusted tempo, very understandable by musicians: TPOADJ=PC1/-.3595=TPO-1.1102*FN-0.7956*DVELLR -1.0244*STDVEL-1.2049*CVTPO-1.0205*STDOVL -1.1537*LAGLH This formula gives the results in Table 2. Under this system, a pianist who plays fast and has many flaws gets the same score as a pianist who plays slower but with good legato, good rhythmic homogeneity, few false notes, etc. Acknowledgments I wish to thank Konrad Maria Engel for kindly accepting to lend his talent to our experiments, to Eckart Altenmüller and Hans Christian Jabusch for fruitful help and discussions, to Chloé Fourier and Michel Cara the indefatigable operators, and to Véronique Assadas for her stylistic help. Address for correspondence Paul Molin, LEAD CNRS, University of Burgundy, Esplanade Erasme, Dijon, Côte d Or 21000, France; Email: paul.molin@u-bourgogne.fr References Bach C. P.E. (1762). Essai sur la Véritable Manière de Jouer les Instruments à Clavier. Paris: CNRS Editions 2002. Clarke E. F. (2004). Empirical methods in the study of performance. In E. F. Clarke and N. Cook (eds), Empirical Musicology (pp. 77-102).Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cortot A. (1928). Principes Rationnels de la Technique Pianistique. Paris: Salabert. Hotelling H. (1933). Analysis of a complex of statistical variables with principal components. Journal of Educational Psychology, 24, pp. 417-441. Jabusch H.C., Alpers H., Kopiez R., and Altenmüller E. (2009). The influence of practice on development of motor skills in pianists: A longitudinal study in a select motor task. Human Movement Science, 28, pp. 74-84. Jabusch H.C., Vauth H., and Altenmüller E. (2004). Quantification of focal dystonia in pianists using scale analysis. Movement Disorders, 19, pp. 171-180. Jabusch H.C., Yong R., and Altenmüller E. (2007). Biographical predictors of musicrelated motor skills in children pianists. In A. Williamon and D. Coimbra (eds.), Proceedings of ISPS 2007 (pp.363-368). Utrecht, The Netherlands: European Association of Conservatoires (AEC).