AN IDENTIFICATION OF SYNAESTHETIC ABILITY: CONSISTENCY OF SYNAESTHETIC EXPERIENCE TO VOWELS AND DIGITS

Similar documents
INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Radiating beauty" in Japan also?

Quantify. The Subjective. PQM: A New Quantitative Tool for Evaluating Display Design Options

COGNITIVE INTERFERENCE IN THE PERCEPTION OF PITCH AND LOUDNESS IN A FIVE-NOTE MUSICAL PATTERN DISSERTATION. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

Coloured Vowels: Wittgenstein on Synaesthesia and Secondary Meaning

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

1. BACKGROUND AND AIMS

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

THE COLOR OF MUSIC: CORRESPONDENCE THROUGH EMOTION

Rusudan Japaridze SYNESTHETIC METAPHORS IN WILLIAM BUTLER YEATS POETRY

The quality of potato chip sounds and crispness impression

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

Co-simulation Techniques for Mixed Signal Circuits

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

MUSICAL MOODS: A MASS PARTICIPATION EXPERIMENT FOR AFFECTIVE CLASSIFICATION OF MUSIC

Guidelines for Manuscript Preparation for Advanced Biomedical Engineering

Stable aesthetic standards delusion: Changing artistic quality by elaboration

K Use kinesthetic awareness, proper use of space and the ability to move safely. use of space (2, 5)

Brief Report. Development of a Measure of Humour Appreciation. Maria P. Y. Chik 1 Department of Education Studies Hong Kong Baptist University

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Environment Expression: Expressing Emotions through Cameras, Lights and Music

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Research Methodology for the Internal Observation of Design Thinking through the Creative Self-formation Process

TYPE OF CONTRIBUTION (1) [ARIAL CAPITAL LETTERS 9 pt]

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

THE SENSATION OF COLOUR

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

23/01/51. Gender-selective effects of the P300 and N400 components of the. VEP waveform. How are ERP related to gender? Event-Related Potential (ERP)

Colour-influences on loudness judgements

BBC Television Services Review

Graduate Bulletin PSYCHOLOGY

Brain-Computer Interface (BCI)

Surprise & emotion. Theoretical paper Key conference theme: Interest, surprise and delight

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Research & Development. White Paper WHP 228. Musical Moods: A Mass Participation Experiment for the Affective Classification of Music

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

Composer Identification of Digital Audio Modeling Content Specific Features Through Markov Models

Research Master Thesis. Academic year 2011/2012, Term: 3-4. Supervisor: Prof. dr. Koen van Eijck. Second assessor: Dr. Peter Achterberg. S.

S. S. Stevens papers,

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Empirical Aesthetics. William Seeley, Bates College

High School Photography 1 Curriculum Essentials Document

Chapter Two: Long-Term Memory for Timbre

AUD 6306 Speech Science

With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long!

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Common assumptions in color characterization of projectors

Pitch Perception. Roger Shepard

Module PS4083 Psychology of Music

Embodied music cognition and mediation technology

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Memory and learning: experiment on Sonata KV 331, in A Major by W. A. Mozart

California Content Standard Alignment: Hoopoe Teaching Stories: Visual Arts Grades Nine Twelve Proficient* DENDE MARO: THE GOLDEN PRINCE

An Indian Journal FULL PAPER ABSTRACT KEYWORDS. Trade Science Inc.

JOURNAL OF PHARMACEUTICAL RESEARCH AND EDUCATION AUTHOR GUIDELINES

Exploring touch: A review of Matthew Fulkerson s The First Sense

Musical learning and cognitive performance

Electronic Musicological Review

Synaesthetic Effects Can Produce an Immersive Visual Music. Chao-Chun Wu

Machine Vision System for Color Sorting Wood Edge-Glued Panel Parts

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Covert Painting Simulations Influence Aesthetic Appreciation of Artworks

Doubletalk Detection

hprints , version 1-1 Oct 2008

Mixing Metaphors. Mark G. Lee and John A. Barnden

Dial A440 for absolute pitch: Absolute pitch memory by non-absolute pitch possessors

On Musical Preference. Kendrick K woczalla. Ball State University

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

Effect of room acoustic conditions on masking efficiency

2. Measurements of the sound levels of CMs as well as those of the programs

VISUAL MILL LAB. SECTION 1: Complete the following tests and fill out the appropriate sections on your Visual Mill Color Deficit Worksheet.

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Subjective evaluation of common singing skills using the rank ordering method

Real-time QC in HCHP seismic acquisition Ning Hongxiao, Wei Guowei and Wang Qiucheng, BGP, CNPC

PROTOTYPING AN AMBIENT LIGHT SYSTEM - A CASE STUDY

(Syn)aesthetics: Redefining Visceral Performance. by Josephine Machon. A review. by Paul Woodward

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

UNDERSTANDING TINNITUS AND TINNITUS TREATMENTS

On time: the influence of tempo, structure and style on the timing of grace notes in skilled musical performance

Natural Scenes Are Indeed Preferred, but Image Quality Might Have the Last Word

The Effects of Web Site Aesthetics and Shopping Task on Consumer Online Purchasing Behavior

Compose yourself: The Emotional Influence of Music

CHOICE OF WIDE COLOR GAMUTS IN CINEMA EOS C500 CAMERA

GUIDELINES FOR THE PREPARATION OF WRITTEN ASSIGNMENTS

Analysis on the Value of Inner Music Hearing for Cultivation of Piano Learning

V Congreso Internacional de Sinestesia, Ciencia y Arte. Alcalá la Real, Jaén mayo 2015

Informational Masking and Trained Listening. Undergraduate Honors Thesis

Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription

Behavioral and neural identification of birdsong under several masking conditions

Murdoch redux. Colorimetry as Linear Algebra. Math of additive mixing. Approaching color mathematically. RGB colors add as vectors

ACT-R ACT-R. Core Components of the Architecture. Core Commitments of the Theory. Chunks. Modules

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

Introduction. Part 1. Evaluating Color. Color Communication/Terms. Colorist Qualifications. Color Equipment/Instruments. Specifications/Settings

Experiments on tone adjustments

An ecological approach to multimodal subjective music similarity perception

Transcription:

AN IDENTIFICATION OF SYNAESTHETIC ABILITY: CONSISTENCY OF SYNAESTHETIC EXPERIENCE TO VOWELS AND DIGITS Sabine Schneider and Christian Kaernbach Institut für Allgemeine Psychologie, Universität Leipzig Seeburgstr. 1 0, D-03 Leipzig, Germany Email: erz95alr@studserv.uni-leipzig.de Christian@Kaernbach.de Abstract The present study investigates the consistency of synaesthetic attribution. By means of a local press campaign we recruited 50 participants reporting synaesthesia. Of those, 19 reported mainly lexical synaesthesia, and 11 reported occasional lexical synaesthesia besides other synaesthetic experiences. By comparing repeated colour selection (RGB-colour space) attributed to alphanumeric signs (vowels and Arabic digits) for synaesthetes and a control group it was found that the mainly lexical synaesthetes showed a high consistency in responding over a short (one week) as well as a long (three month) time span. In comparison, the control group and the occasional lexical synaesthetes demonstrated less consistent responding when retested after one week. Synaesthesia, which means syn = together and aesthesis = perception or sensation, is a vivid cross-modal perception, defined as an experience in one modality as a function of stimulation in a different modality (e.g. Baron-Cohen et al., 197, 1993). A person experiencing synaesthesia is called synaesthete, typically experiencing synaesthesia on a daily basis, beginning in early childhood (e.g. Grossenbacher & Lovelance 001). Synaesthete s sensory reaction to stimuli like, for instance, letters and numbers is special. By their own description lexical synaesthetes perceive spoken or written letters and numbers always involuntarily and insuppressibly together with colours. These synaesthetic assignments are highly individual, they vary from person to person. Some synaesthetes perceive multidimensional sensations, e.g. coloured shapes elicited by music. These multidimensional sensations are interesting and fascinating, but it is difficult to investigate them objectively. Therefore, we decided to focus on a single and frequent dimension of synaesthetic experience. The following section we firstly describe a survey of the dimensions of synaesthesia, which was conducted to isolate the major dimensions of synaesthesia for subsequent experiments. Secondly we reported a test of consistency of synaesthetic attributions. The aim of this test was to find an objective measure for the stability of the synaesthetic experience of each individual. The results allow a first differentiation between self-reported synaesthetes and nonsynaesthetes by psychophysical methods.

Survey of individual dimensions of synaesthetic ability In order to find individuals experiencing synaesthesia, the media were informed about our project. Nationwide there was a lot of resonance. Following publications in major local and national newspapers and leading German magazines (e.g. Spiegel and Süddeutsche Zeitung) and interviews on TV and radio (e.g. Arte and Deutschlandfunk), people from all parts of the country got in touch with us. 50 subjects took part in qualitative interviews aimed to identify the dimensions involved in their individual synaesthetic experience. All subjects reported synaesthesia from their early childhood. To investigate the dimensions of synaesthesia systematically we inquired of the participants about their synaesthetic sensations. The aim was to find criteria that would allow us to select a homogeneous group of synaesthetes from all participants. The results of the initial interviews are shown in Figure 1 below. number 0 35 30 5 0 15 5 dimensions of secondary sensation: colour body graph shape motion surface 0 other modality situation emotion pain voice noise instruments music tone space of time digit letter stimuli Figure 1. Dimensions of Synaesthesia. Number of synaesthetes reporting a certain secondary sensation elicited by the stimuli shown on the abscissa. Multiple choices of the modality of the secondary sensation were possible. This diagram shows that the experience of synaesthesia can be triggered by various stimuli (e.g. letters, tones, or even pain). This secondary sensation can involve various dimensions (e.g. colour plus form plus surface). The diagram shows also that a very common form of synaesthesia is coloured hearing, which implies that the subject experiences colours when hearing various kinds of auditory signals, such as spoken letters, music, or instrument sounds (Argelander, 197; Bleuler & Lehmann, 11; Marks, 1975). A prominent kind of synaesthetic experience is linked to vowels (Argelander, 197; Marks, 1975). Argelander as well as the interviewed synaesthetes described vowels as acoustically clearer than consonants. If vowels and consonants occur together, the vowel sound is assumed to dominate in the production of synaesthetic photisms. According to our survey, the most frequently experienced synaesthetic link exists between letters (esp. vowels) and digits on the one hand and colours on the other hand. Therefore we designed an experiment investigating this lexical synaesthetic link. Based on their self-report, 19 participants reporting mainly lexical synaesthesia and 11 participants reporting occasional lexical synaesthesia (often in addition to other synaesthetic experiences) were selected to take part in the following test.

Investigation of stability of synaesthetic connection: The correspondence of colour to digits and vowels The main purpose of this experiment was to measure the assignment of a specific colour to each of 1 alphanumeric signs. The existence of synaesthesia is well documented (Cytowic & Wood, 19; Marks, 1975), but the degree of consistency in the individual synaesthetic experience has not been adequately investigated. Early investigators were especially interested in common combinations (e.g. red and a). From a short survey among the citizens of Leipzig in 10 Fechner, a synaesthete by himself, reported some colour-letter associations to be more frequent than others (e.g. yellow and e) (Fechner, 10, 13). Francis Galton (13) was the first who described the experiences of synaesthetes systematically. He found that seers, as he called them, are invariable in their description of the precise tint and hue of the colour of their coloured alphabets. Similar to Fechner s approach, this early study relied on verbal introspective reports of the synaesthetes. Argelander (197) and Marks (1975) used colourmatching tasks to investigate synaesthetic sensations with a small number of broad colour categories like yellow, red, etc., neglecting the highly specific and differentiated character of synaesthetic sensation. A more recent study confirmed the high consistency of synaesthetic associations between primary perception and secondary synaesthetic sensations (Baron-Cohen et al., 1993). To specify the degree of consistency, 13 synaesthetes and 13 non-synaesthetes were compared. The participants had to attach a colour word to over one hundred items (e.g. days of the week, months, seasons) and it was found that in a retest one year later the synaesthetes had chosen nearly the same colours for all one hundred items (consistency 9%). Consistency in nonsynaesthetes retested after one week was only 5%. Like most other studies this study employed methods of verbal colour report, which are constrained by the limitations of the subject s language to accurately describe a synaesthetic episode. Additional evidence of the high consistency of synaesthetic experience was given by a test of Svartdal & Iversen (199). They tested the assignment of colours to vowels and consonants. Their subjects could respond within a given set of choices by Munsell colour chips. Using colour-matching tasks to investigate the synaesthetic assignments, Cytowic & Wood (19) found the answers of synaesthetes are more stable over a long time span. One of the weaknesses of the studies reported above is the restricted range of colour responses subjects can choose from. Head (1995) conducted initial work to provide a more precise measurement of colour matching. He used a continuous printed colour pallet on which subjects marked their responses with a pen. The palette was taken from Bouma (1971) and did not contain all possible colours. In line with this idea we wanted to develop a test with a finely-graded response domain. The aim of the study was to expand and further improve the response quality, by allowing participants to indicate colour matches on a continuous and complete three-dimensional colour palette. These include the greys, browns, and black omitted in the Head (1995) palette. The palette was implemented by using a colour-preference tuning program on a computer. Thus, the data would directly be available for further experiments with the same subjects. Hypothesis. We expected synaesthetes to prove higher degrees of consistency than control subjects. Furthermore, the results of the test should confirm the self-reports of the participants: If they assigned themselves to the mainly lexical synaesthetes they should show better results in the test of consistency compared to participants calling themselves occasional lexical synaesthetes or the control group of non-synaesthetes.

Methods Participants. Synaesthetic participants (n=30, 3 female and 7 male; age range 1 to 75) were selected on the grounds of self-reported sign-colour synaesthesia in the initial interview (19 reporting mainly lexical and 11 reporting occasional lexical synaesthesia). A comparison group of non-synaesthetes was included. Control participants without synaesthesia were found locally and comprised 30 undergraduates (3 female and 7 male; age range 17to 35). Procedure/Stimuli. The synaesthetes were asked to express their inner colours with the help of a computer colour-preference tuning program. The control subjects were asked to mix a colour that fits best to the vowel or digit. The stimulus set comprised 1 alphanumeric signs: digits, 5 vowels, and 3 diphthongs. These stimuli were randomly chosen in terms of colour and sequence. They appeared on a light-grey monitor (size approximately x 5cm). The monitor was placed in a darkened room with controlled soft background illumination. The colour of the letter or digit was continuously adjustable by three pairs of keys for the dimensions saturation, brightness and hue. In order to support subject s orientation in colour space a coloured ruler for every dimension was located on the right side of the monitor. A pointer indicated the respective value. In addition to that the ruler indicated which colours would emerge by manipulation of this dimension. The experimental screen is shown in Figure below. There was no time limit for this test. During the first test, subjects were not aware that there would follow a retest. Subjects were retested after one week. A further unannounced retest was performed after three months. Figure. Screen shot of colour-preference tuning program. A colour version of this screen shot can be found at www.uni-leipzig.de/cognition/synaesthesia/screenshot.gif Data Analysis. Each session yielded for each subject 1 RGB vectors corresponding to their colour responses to the 1 stimuli. Comparing two sessions, the Euclidic distance for each of 1 pairs of RGB vectors was computed and averaged. The results come out in percent, with 0% corresponding to the maximal distance varying one dimension alone (R, G or B- colour). Due to the number of dimensions, a maximal distance of 173% (0% x 3) is possible. Pairs of randomly distributed values in the RGB colour cube result in a mean distance of 7%. The distribution of such random distances is indicated in Figure 3 by the white bars: random performance (Monte-Carlo simulation). Genuine synaesthetes in our sense are subjects with a small average distance. This indicates that they mixed similar colours for the same alphanumeric sign in both experimental sessions.

Results and Discussion The results of the test of consistency are shown in Figure 3. Figure 3a shows the consistency after one week. Apparently, mainly lexical synaesthetes show a much higher degree of consistency than both occasional lexical synaesthetes and control subjects. The mean value of all synaesthetes taken together (n=30) is 37,5%±0,%, the mean value of the control group (n=30) is significantly higher 59,0±11,% (-tailed t-test: t(9) = -,; p < 0.001). Would the mean have been calculated only across mainly lexical synaesthetes, the difference would have been even more significant. number 0 mainly lexical synaesthetes occasional lexical synaesthetes 0-5 -15 0-5 30-35 0-5 50-55 0-5 distance (%) 70-75 0-5 90-95 Figure 3.a. Consistency measure based on retesting after one week: Distribution of distances in the RGB colour cube for repeated signcolour matching for mainly (N=19) and occasional (N=11) lexical synaesthetes and control subjects (N=30). Simulation see text. controls random performance (Monte-Carlo simulation) number 0 0-5 -15 0-5 30-35 0-5 50-55 0-5 distance (%) 70-75 0-5 90-95 Figure 3. b. Consistency measure based on retesting after three months: Distribution of distances in the RGB colour cube for repeated signcolour matching for synaesthetes (N=17) and control subjects (N=17). Data for occasional lexical synaesthetes can not be shown because of subject drop-out. Figure 3b shows the consistency after three months. Only 17 synaesthetes took part in this second retest. It is remarkable that these are exclusively from the group of the mainly lexical synaesthetes. All synaesthetes had been invited without prior warning, and none of them knew the outcome of the first consistency test. Nonetheless, occasional lexical synaesthetes showed no interest in a further lexical retest, some of them stating that this test didn t fit their kind of sensations. After three months, the mean value of the synaesthetes (n=17) is 33,±1,5%, (note: after one week the mean value for these 17 subjects is 9.%). We restricted the number of control subjects equally to 17. The mean value of the control group after three months is 1,0±1,1% (t(1) = -5,; p < 0,001) (note: after one week the mean value for these 17 subjects is 5.9%). Apparently, synaesthetes were significantly better at repeating a colour response for the same alphanumeric sign on an unannounced retest.

The consistency of occasional lexical synaesthetes after one week did not differ much from that of controls. Both groups exhibit performance slightly above randomness. This indicates that their assignments of colour values to the items was not carried out in a random manner. We assume that they followed general assignment trends or memory effects after the oneweek period. Only two subjects of the control group stand out of the distribution. For these individuals the classification as non-synaesthetes is questionable. In these cases additional tests may provide further information. Those of the synaesthetes that classified themselves as mainly lexical synaesthetes (n=19) showed very stable sign-colour correspondences (mean value after one week: 5%). These results confirm nicely the self-report of the participants. The low consistency of occasional lexical synaesthetes does not contradict their synaesthetic disposition, but reveals the different focus of their synaesthesia. Although classified as synaesthetes this subgroup was excluded from further experiments in the lexical domain, as here a stable assignment is the precondition. The results of this study are encouraging. At present 19 synaesthetes with stable signcolour correspondences are available. The number of subjects is still increasing. Conclusion The aim of this study was to investigate synaesthetic ability. While the latter can be highly complex and multidimensional, we focussed on a single dimension which was found frequently in our group of synaesthetes in order to facilitate the design of future group experiments. We intended to establish a measure of the stability of synaesthetic experience in order to recruit a homogeneous test group for further cognitive experiments. In our interviews, vowels and digits were found to be the most common stimulus domains whereas colour was the most frequented response domain. These assignments were reported to be very stable over time. Based on this survey a new computer test with a continuous three-dimensional response domain was designed to measure the degree of the stability of sign-colour synaesthesia. The results of the test confirm the self-report of the participants: When they assigned themselves as mainly lexical synaesthetes they showed better results in the test of consistency compared to participants calling themselves occasional lexical synaesthetes. Classical studies on synaesthesia rely on self-report/introspection. Future research might be advanced by defining synaesthetic ability by objective measures. A first step in this direction is the investigation of stability we presented. More progress should result from developing further objective methods to assess synaesthetic sensations offering additional evidence for the reality of synaesthesia as a phenomenon above memory, metaphor or imagery. The test of stability presented in our study is useful as an indication of how genuine the subject s synaesthesia is, but as Cytowic (199) observes, these studies do little to clarify the underlying mechanisms of synaesthesia itself. They can, however, provide a basis to build up a homogeneous group of approved synaesthetes as a precondition to further studies.

References Argelander, A. (197). Das Farbenhören und der synästhetische Faktor der Wahrnehmung. Jena: G. Fischer. Baron-Cohen, S., Harrison, J., Goldstein, L.H. & Wyke, M. (1993). Coloured speech perception: Is Synaesthesia what happens when modularity breaks down? Perception,, 19-. Baron-Cohen, S., Wyke, M.A., & Bannie, C. (197). Hearing words and seeing colours: an experimental investigation of a case of synaesthesia. Perception, 1, 71-77. Bleuler, E. & Lehmann, K. (11). Zwangsmäßige Lichtempfindungen durch Schall und verwandte Erscheinungen auf dem Gebiet anderer Sinnesempfindungen. Leipzig: Fues Verlag. Bouma, P.J. (1971). Physical Aspects of colour. nd Edition. Netherlands: Machmillon. Cytowic, R.E. (199). Synesthesia: A Union of the Senses. New York: Springer Verlag, 3-35. Cytowic, R.E. & Wood, F.B. (19). Synesthesia: II. Psychophysical relations in the synesthesia of geometrically shaped taste and colored hearing. Brain and Cognition, 1, 3-9. Fechner, G.T. (13). Vorschule der Ästhetik, Vol.. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Haertel. Fechner, G.T. (10). Professor Fechners Assoziationsstatistik. Aufruf des Akademisch- Philosophischen Vereins Leipzig. Wissenschaftliche Beilage der Leipziger Zeitung Nr. (11. Januar), Galton, F. (13). Inquiries into Human Faculty and its Development. London: Macmillan. Grossenbacher, P.G. & Lovelance, C.T. (001). Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 3-1. Head, P.A., de T. (1995). Coloured Hearing; A Case Study. Unpublished manuscript. Marks, L.E. (1975). On colored-hearing synaesthesia: cross-modal translation of sensory dimensions. Psychological Bulletin,, 303. Svartdal, F. & Iversen, T. (199). Consistency in synesthetic experience to vowels and consonants: Five case studies. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 30, 0-7.