Selecting Serious or Satirical, Supporting or Stirring News? Selective Exposure to Partisan versus Mockery Online News Videos

Similar documents
UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

At Odds: Laughing and Thinking? The Appreciation, Processing, and Persuasiveness of Political Satire

Zondag met Lubach: makes breaking news whole again? perceived funniness in this relationship

When Political Comedy Turns Personal: Humor Types, Audience Evaluations, and Attitudes

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) The Causes and Consequences of Affinity for Political Humor Boukes, M.

The Effects of Web Site Aesthetics and Shopping Task on Consumer Online Purchasing Behavior

International Journal of Communication 11(2017), /

Where the word irony comes from

POLITICAL MEME HUMOR AND ITS EFFECT ON VIEWS OF POLITICIANS AND POLICIES. Introduction

Centre for Economic Policy Research

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

The Fox News Eect:Media Bias and Voting S. DellaVigna and E. Kaplan (2007)

Laughing or learning with the Chief Executive? The impact of exposure to presidents jokes on message elaboration

Klee or Kid? The subjective experience of drawings from children and Paul Klee Pronk, T.

Chapter Two: Long-Term Memory for Timbre

A dual-processing approach to the effects of viewing political comedy

The Relevance Framework for Category-Based Induction: Evidence From Garden-Path Arguments

DOES MOVIE SOUNDTRACK MATTER? THE ROLE OF SOUNDTRACK IN PREDICTING MOVIE REVENUE

The Roles of Politeness and Humor in the Asymmetry of Affect in Verbal Irony

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

The Impact of Media Censorship: Evidence from a Field Experiment in China

Affective response to a set of new musical stimuli W. Trey Hill & Jack A. Palmer Psychological Reports, 106,

Student Performance Q&A:

When Do Vehicles of Similes Become Figurative? Gaze Patterns Show that Similes and Metaphors are Initially Processed Differently

AP Language and Composition Hobbs/Wilson

Improving music composition through peer feedback: experiment and preliminary results

Introduction to Rhetoric (from OWL Purdue website)

Non-Reducibility with Knowledge wh: Experimental Investigations

DEMOGRAPHIC DIFFERENCES IN WORKPLACE GOSSIPING BEHAVIOUR IN ORGANIZATIONS - AN EMPIRICAL STUDY ON EMPLOYEES IN SMES

Sundance Institute: Artist Demographics in Submissions & Acceptances. Dr. Stacy L. Smith, Marc Choueiti, Hannah Clark & Dr.

Validity. What Is It? Types We Will Discuss. The degree to which an inference from a test score is appropriate or meaningful.

Effect of Compact Disc Materials on Listeners Song Liking

Master thesis. The effects of L2, L1 dubbing and L1 subtitling on the effectiveness of persuasive fictional narratives.

Why Music Theory Through Improvisation is Needed

Theories and Activities of Conceptual Artists: An Aesthetic Inquiry

Karen Hutzel The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio REFERENCE BOOK REVIEW 327

Expanding Perceptions of African-Americans Political Habits: A Study of Expectancy Violation. Theory and Humor. Master s Thesis

The contribution of material culture studies to design

Author Instructions for submitting manuscripts to Environment & Behavior

Fran s School of Dance: The Dancing through Life Campaign

History Admissions Assessment Specimen Paper Section 1: explained answers

Fairfield Public Schools English Curriculum

Running head: FACIAL SYMMETRY AND PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS 1

Introduction One of the major marks of the urban industrial civilization is its visual nature. The image cannot be separated from any civilization.

Interdepartmental Learning Outcomes

What Are You Really Buying? FJU Students Opinions on Eslite Bookstore and its Adoption on Cultural Commodification Strategy

Timbre blending of wind instruments: acoustics and perception

Radiating beauty" in Japan also?

PUBLIC SCHOOLS OF EDISON TOWNSHIP DIVISION OF CURRICULUM AND INSTRUCTION. Chamber Choir/A Cappella Choir/Concert Choir

High School Photography 1 Curriculum Essentials Document

INFLUENCE OF MUSICAL CONTEXT ON THE PERCEPTION OF EMOTIONAL EXPRESSION OF MUSIC

Truthiness and Second-Level Agenda Setting: Satire News and Its Influence on Perceptions of Television News Credibility

BBC Trust Review of the BBC s Speech Radio Services

in the Howard County Public School System and Rocketship Education

Frightfully funny: combining threat and humour in health messages for men and women

Sight and Sensibility: Evaluating Pictures Mind, Vol April 2008 Mind Association 2008

The Influence of Visual Metaphor Advertising Types on Recall and Attitude According to Congruity-Incongruity

observation and conceptual interpretation

WEB APPENDIX. Managing Innovation Sequences Over Iterated Offerings: Developing and Testing a Relative Innovation, Comfort, and Stimulation

The Influence of Open Access on Monograph Sales

Department of MBA, School of Communication and Management Studies, Nalukettu, Kerala, India

How Do We React When Our Favorite Characters Are Taken Away? An Examination of a Temporary Parasocial Breakup

Speech Recognition and Signal Processing for Broadcast News Transcription

The Impact of Humor in North American versus Middle East Cultures

Can scientific impact be judged prospectively? A bibliometric test of Simonton s model of creative productivity

On time: the influence of tempo, structure and style on the timing of grace notes in skilled musical performance

HOW TO WRITE HIGH QUALITY ARGUMENTS

What Can Experimental Philosophy Do? David Chalmers

Patterns of difference in participants visual depictions of Clayoquot Sound

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

King s Research Portal

Image and Imagination

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Introductory Comments: Special Issue of EJOP (August 2010) on Humor Research in Personality and Social Psychology

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

AGGRESSIVE HUMOR: NOT ALWAYS AGGRESSIVE. Thesis. Submitted to. The College of Arts and Sciences of the UNIVERSITY OF DAYTON

Anthropology and Philosophy: Creating a Workspace for Collaboration

Effects of Musical Training on Key and Harmony Perception

Brief Report. Development of a Measure of Humour Appreciation. Maria P. Y. Chik 1 Department of Education Studies Hong Kong Baptist University

The Sony channel New Channel Evaluation

EDITORIAL POLICY. Open Access and Copyright Policy

CONQUERING CONTENT EXCERPT OF FINDINGS

River Dell Regional School District. Visual and Performing Arts Curriculum Music

Have you seen these shows? Monitoring Tazama! (investigate show) and XYZ (political satire)

A Comprehensive Critical Study of Gadamer s Hermeneutics

Age differences in women s tendency to gossip are mediated by their mate value

Introduction to Satire

REVIEW ARTICLE BOOK TITLE: ORAL TRADITION AS HISTORY

PPM Rating Distortion. & Rating Bias Handbook

KINDS (NATURAL KINDS VS. HUMAN KINDS)

a story or visual image with a second distinct meaning partially hidden behind it literal or visible meaning Allegory

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

Master of Arts in Psychology Program The Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences offers the Master of Arts degree in Psychology.

Carlo Martini 2009_07_23. Summary of: Robert Sugden - Credible Worlds: the Status of Theoretical Models in Economics 1.

THE IMPLEMENTATION OF INTERTEXTUALITY APPROACH TO DEVELOP STUDENTS CRITI- CAL THINKING IN UNDERSTANDING LITERATURE

POVs, HMWs, and Experience Prototypes

Modeling memory for melodies

Running head: THE EFFECT OF MUSIC ON READING COMPREHENSION. The Effect of Music on Reading Comprehension

Discourse analysis is an umbrella term for a range of methodological approaches that

Color Reproduction Complex

Transcription:

Running Head: SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS Selecting Serious or Satirical, Supporting or Stirring News? Selective Exposure to Partisan versus Mockery Online News Videos Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & *Lavis, S. (2017). Selecting serious or satirical, supporting or stirring news? Selective exposure to partisan versus mockery news online videos. Journal of Communication, 67, 54-81.

Running Head: SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 1 Selecting Serious or Satirical, Supporting or Stirring News? Selective Exposure to Partisan versus Mockery News Online Videos* * The authors would like to acknowledge Kate T. Luong for her assistance during the formative stages of this project, and the undergraduate assistants Travis Filicky, Megan Huber and Park Lukich for their help with searching for and preparing satirical materials. Abstract The present investigation combines cognitive dissonance theory with entertainment-education frameworks to study selection and effects of news. Selective exposure to satirical and partisan news was examined with online clips to test hypotheses on overcoming resistance to persuasive messages. An experiment (n = 146) presented news choices, varied in stance (conservative vs. liberal) and format (serious partisan news vs. satirical news). Results show political interest fosters selection of serious partisan news. Clips with partisan alignment were more frequently selected; only for the satire news clips, Democrats did not exhibit such confirmation bias. Selecting satire news affected internal political efficacy, and selecting online news clips induced attitude reinforcement according to message stance.

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 2 Selecting Serious or Satirical, Supporting or Stirring Messages? Selective Exposure to Partisan versus Mockery News Online Videos Numerous current media embed political messages in entertainment formats (see Holbert, 2005, for an overview). Ranging from soft news to fictional drama about politics, from satire news to political references in late night comedy, a wide variety of shows and messages convey political information and views while seemingly appealing first and foremost to entertainment needs along the lines of amusement and emotional involvement. Scholars have expressed hopes that embedding political information into entertaining formats can help to involve otherwise unengaged citizens more in the political process (e.g., Moy, Xenos, & Hess, 2005). At the same time, political polarization among citizens causes concerns and is said to partially result from selective exposure to mediated messages on political matters, which is fostered by the internet (Bennett & Iyengar, 2008). Political messages embedded in entertainment may permeate such confirmation bias that is widely demonstrated for traditional political messages. The current study will tackle this proposition, based on theoretical lens that merges cognitive dissonance theory with the entertainment-education framework. In the following, we review the scholarly discussion of political entertainment, before introducing both entertainmenteducation and cognitive dissonance theory. Hypotheses derived from these perspectives will be examined in an investigation on selective exposure to serious partisan news and satirical news, presented through an online video site featuring clips on three different political issues. It is the first study to observe media users selections of serious and satirical news when accessible simultaneously; thus it reflects real-life media choice environments with plenty of TV and online channels in which entertainment and informative messages compete for attention (Prior, 2007). Additionally, it is also the first study to examine the confirmation bias with online videos.

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 3 Political Entertainment There is wide consensus among scholars that political values and information are commonly featured in formats that are traditionally associated with entertainment fictional movies, fictional TV series, as well as entertainment show events (e.g., Holbert, 2005). This phenomenon may be a cause either for hope or concern on the one hand, such political information embedded in entertainment could serve as a gateway and trigger greater interest in political topics and discourse among otherwise disengaged or alienated segments of the electorate (e.g., Bartsch & Schneider, 2014; Feldman & Young, 2008; Xenos & Becker, 2009). On the other hand, it may contribute to trivialization of political topics as a matter of amusement or peripheral importance, or possibly increase alienation and cynicism (Balmas, 2014; Baumgartner & Morris, 2006; Guggenheim, Kwak, & Campbell, 2011). There is consensus, however, that such political entertainment is not without effects. It has been shown to affect political knowledge (e.g., Young, 2004), as well as political attitudes (e.g., Mutz & Nir, 2010). Specifically, the wide popularity of satire news (e.g., Mitchell, Gottfried, Kiley, & Matsa, 2014) has garnered much scholarly attention: Viewers acquire political information from watching these shows (e.g. Hardy, Gottfried, Winneg, & Jamieson, 2014) and may benefit in their internal political efficacy (Baumgartner & Morris, 2006; Becker, 2011), thus assess their own ability to understand and influence politics more positively. Both these outcomes are highly desirable in terms of fostering political engagement, which underlines satire news exposure s societal relevance and importance for communication science. Thus, the present study examines selection and effects of satire news, including impacts on attitudes and internal political efficacy. Satire is such an amorphous genre [ ] no two scholars define it in the same words (Feinberg, 1967, p. 18). Political entertainment scholars also noted that it is difficult to define

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 4 (Holbert, Tchernev, Walther, Esralew, & Benski, 2013). In simple terms, satire is an artistic form, chiefly literary and dramatic, in which human or individual vices, follies, abuses, or shortcomings are held up to censure by means of ridicule, [ ] irony, parody, [ ] sometimes with an intent to inspire social reform (Satire, 2016). For political entertainment, scholars have juxtaposed political satire with traditional forms of political messages such as opinion-editorials or fact-oriented news (e.g., Boukes, Boomgaarden, Moorman, & de Vreese, 2015; Holbert et al., 2013), and have further differentiated it into juvenalian satire with harsh attacks, versus horatian satire with gentler humor and wit. Everyday satire, however, will often come as a mix of harsher and gentler ridicule. The present work will use an empirically based conceptualization in which recipients categorize messages as fitting satire (see method section). Overcoming Resistance Through Entertainment Contexts The branch of political communication that focuses on political entertainment is a flourishing research field interestingly, it appears related to the recent surge in research about entertainment-education in health communication (de Graaf, Sanders, & Hoeken, 2016). In these persuasive efforts, health information is embedded in entertaining, often narrative contexts to reach audience segments that otherwise avoid health education messages. The hope is that the entertainment context will veil the persuasive intent of the health communication and thus reduce recipients reactance (Kreuter, Green, Cappella, Slater, Wise, Storey, et al., 2007). Similarly, political messages embedded in entertainment may reach recipients who do not attend to political content otherwise or who resent its persuasive purpose (see also Boukes et al., 2015; Holbert et al., 2013). Along these lines, Nabi, Moyer-Gusé, and Byrne (2007) argued that humorous social commentary in late-night shows might be processed with less scrutiny, yet it might also be more easily discounted. Accordingly, entertaining and humorous political messages might permeate

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 5 more easily than traditional political messages with a serious tone, which are widely shown to be subject to a confirmation bias. Hence, embedding persuasion messages into entertainment might make them far more effective because recipients will be less likely to suspect and guard against such persuasion (Dal Cin, Zanna, & Fong, 2004), regardless of the context of health or politics. Yet intriguing differences between the domains of health and political communication should be noted: While there is little debate that persuasive messages promoting healthier behaviors are beneficial to the individual and society as a whole, no such normative consensus exists in the realm of political persuasive messages. Unlike health information, political persuasive communication is typically partisan in nature or campaigns for a particular policy that has not reached consensus in the electorate, so entertainment-education in the political context is akin to what critics might call entertainment-propaganda. Then again, political persuasion that aims to increase political knowledge, deliberation, and engagement pursues outcomes generally deemed positive. An additional difference between persuasion in the health versus in the political context is that the former pertains to personal decisions, behaviors, and even longevity outlooks, whereas the latter pertains to holding correct and socially acceptable opinions with societal but less personal implications. Hence, health messages may generally be more likely to hit close to home whereas political messages are not as deeply personal and instead public affairs matters. This difference could result in different selection and processing patterns for health and political messages, respectively, which in turn shape entertainment-embedded persuasion effects. Additionally, political satire in particular may be rather implicit in its meaning and thus open to interpretations that suit the recipients (Holbert, 2005; LaMarre, Landreville, & Beam, 2009). Regardless of what persuasive outcome a political message embedded in entertainment pursues, there may be much to be learned from the research on health entertainment-education.

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 6 To assess hopes that political messages might reach the electorate better when interjected in entertainment, it is worthwhile to inspect what health entertainment-education research has found. For this purpose, we draw on a review of this work by de Graaf et al. (2016): It is remarkable that none of the narrative persuasion studies considered what has been prominently discussed as a key advantage of entertainment-education that is its ability to overcome recipients avoidance of messages that challenge existing attitudes and behaviors and that are easily identified as persuasive in nature (Dal Cin et al., 2004). The related empirical work has consistently used forced-exposure experimental designs, which cannot capture whether entertainment messages can overcome avoidance better than non-entertainment messages. Given this gap in the literature, the present work will examine both the selection and subsequent persuasive effects of messages embedded in entertainment versus non-entertainment formats. Additionally, only a limited set of research (per de Graaf et al. s review of 42 studies, Table 2) allows inferences on whether persuasive claims are indeed more effective when embedded in entertainment. This set of 18 studies provided control groups with roughly the same claims (through advocacy, evidence-based or educational messages) that experimental groups received through some narrative. The clear majority of this set of studies found equivalent results for experimental and control groups, suggesting that the entertainment context did not render the messages more persuasive. On the other hand, many reviewed studies (13) did not provide control groups with the same claims in a non-entertaining context and thus cannot shed light on whether embedding persuasion in entertainment is a superior approach it is not surprising then that experimental groups in these studies were generally more influenced than so-called control groups, which were not even presented with the claims and naturally scored lower on targeted outcomes. And even though the evidence so far does not suggest that entertainment-embedded

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 7 persuasion is more effective, prior work neglected what theorists believe to be the key strength of entertainment-education overcoming avoidance of the persuasive messages in the first place. Selective Exposure to Political Information The goal of overcoming avoidance of certain messages actually has a long tradition, both in health and political communication research. Going back to Festinger s (1957) theory of cognitive dissonance, which has been applied to both contexts, it has long been argued that recipients prefer messages that align with their pre-existing attitudes while avoiding attitudechallenging content. Although this proposition was not consistently supported in earlier political communication research (see Donsbach, 2009, for a review), recent work has yielded consensus on such confirmation bias (Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015). Moreover, the increased ease of message selection in the internet era is thought to allow recipients to block out counterattitudinal messages even more effectively (Bennett & Iyengar, 2008), for instance, via social media (Messing & Westwood, 2014) and automated filtering algorithms (Pariser, 2011). This trend raises concerns for a functional democracy, which is based on an informed, deliberate citizenry. In light of these concerns of increasingly biased information exposure in the internet era, the ability to embed political messages into entertainment contexts may give reason to hope to engage citizens more with counterattitudinal views, for a more balanced opinion formation and greater discourse in society. In other words, can entertainment contexts overcome the confirmation bias that has been consistently demonstrated for traditional political messages? Only a few studies have considered what may motivate viewers to attend to political entertainment formats. For instance, Young (2013) applied a uses-and-gratifications perspective to explore why college students choose or avoid to watch satire news shows. While not exactly pertaining to motivations, Holbert et al. (2013) found political TV satire viewing correlated to

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 8 exposure to satirical sitcoms, exposure to liberal cable news programming, and affinity for political humor. Only one study so far speaks explicitly to the question of whether media users are more likely to choose counterattitudinal political messages if they are presented as entertaining: Stroud and Muddiman (2013) proposed that it can both be argued that satirical news will foster greater confirmation bias and that serious news will foster greater confirmation bias. They conducted an experiment with college students who were exposed to a website with either eight serious news texts or eight satirical news texts. In both the serious news condition and the satirical news condition, the participants saw two articles on four topics health care, immigration, the political parties, and the economy such that there was a conservative-leaning message and a liberal-leaning message for each topic. The results showed that participants in the serious news condition chose more counterattitudinal articles than those in the satirical news condition, 1.85 on average compared to 1.24. Hence, they concluded that a greater confirmation bias was evident when media users were choosing from satire news. Current Investigation The discussion above marries entertainment-education frameworks with cognitive dissonance theory, to aid with increasing interest in political information and exposure to attitude-challenging messages. Next, hypotheses will be derived and tested empirically. Our investigation will allow participants to selectively attend to messages that are presented as entertaining satire or informative news (to examine selections) while keeping content that is actually presented as comparable as possible (to examine effects). It will shed light on whether political messages framed as entertainment attract recipients with low political interest (as proposed, for example, by Baum, 2002). It has been argued that political entertainment could

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 9 serve as a gateway to engage segments of society that are otherwise politically unengaged (e.g., Xenos & Becker, 2009). The first hypothesis serves to test this proposition: H1: Compared to recipients with higher political interest, recipients with low political interest select satirical news more frequently. The present study also extends work on selective exposure to political information by examining selections on an online video site a context that has not been specifically studied before through the following hypothesis: H2: Online news clips with a political stance that aligns with media users partisanship are more frequently selected (confirmation bias). Yet at the heart of the present research is the question of whether political messages permeate the confirmation bias differently when embedded in entertainment, compared to the widely demonstrated for confirmation bias for traditional political messages. To address this issue, the present study extends the research on selective exposure to satirical messages, specifically the investigation by Stroud and Muddiman on the question Does selective exposure to satirical news differ from selective exposure to serious news? (2013, p. 273). Their work used text stimuli, whereas the current study has participants choose from video clips (based on text leads) because political entertainment appears to be predominantly consumed via audiovisual stimuli (although prominent satire magazines and websites exist). Satire news shows and late-night comedy are at the forefront of the scholarly discussion of political entertainment and attract large television audiences. To mimic the great ease with which media users can now select messages online, the study presents audiovisual stimuli through an online video site. In line with Stroud and Muddiman (2013), we also follow an approach used earlier by Knobloch-Westerwick and Meng (2009) in which opposite-stance messages were offered for

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 10 selection, such that message choices could be categorized as either attitude-consistent or counterattitudinal per preexposure attitude measures. A crucial contrast to Stroud and Muddiman (2013) is the fact that the present study offered both partisan and satirical news simultaneously as choices. Stroud and Muddiman (2013) varied serious versus satirical news as a between-subjects factor and presented half of their participants a serious news website and the other half with a satirical news website. In contrast, the current experiment conceptualizes the choice between serious partisan news and satirical news as a within-subjects factor by making both news and satire options available at the same time. After all, media users in everyday situations can freely pick from shows like The Daily Show and the like on the one hand, and from a variety of news sources some partisan on the other simultaneously. Additionally, the present work examines impacts of selective exposure on political attitudes and political efficacy, while Stroud and Muddiman (2013) only looked at political tolerance toward other partisans. Theory and related empirical evidence on whether selective exposure to satirical news differs from selective exposure to serious news derive contradictory predictions. On the one hand, satire sources can be considered low-credibility sources that make counterattitudinal messages appear easily refuted, resulting in a reduced confirmation bias per Lowin s (1967) approach-avoidance model. This model, however, has not found much empirical support (e.g., Westerwick, Kleinman, & Knobloch-Westerwick, 2013, Knobloch-Westerwick, Mothes, Johnson, Westerwick, & Donsbach, 2015). Additionally, models from entertainment-education (e.g., Kreuter et al., 2007; Slater & Rouner, 2002) suggest that attitude-challenging messages, which would otherwise be avoided, instigate less reactance if framed as entertainment. Further, some empirical work has found that satire viewers of opposite stances perceive the same satirical message as supporting their views (LaMarre, Landreville, & Beam, 2009), indicating that satire

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 11 may be ambiguous enough to please any political preference. Hence, several theoretical perspectives and empirical work suggest that satirical news messages which challenge recipients views will be avoided less than serious news messages. On the other hand, Stroud and Muddiman (2013) found in their study with serious and satirical news texts that participants chose more counterattitudinal messages in the serious news context than in the satire news context, while attitude-consistent choices showed no difference. As satire can be particularly hostile (Holbert et al., 2013), recipients might be even more conscientious in avoiding attitude-challenging materials that are satirical. Further, messages that humiliate a liked person or group should be found less amusing (e.g., Zillmann & Cantor, 1972). In light of the conflicting theoretical perspectives and evidence, we will test competing hypotheses on whether the confirmation bias is strong for informational or entertaining contexts: H3a: The confirmation bias postulated in H2 is more pronounced for serious partisan news clips than for satirical news clips. H3b: The confirmation bias postulated in H2 is more pronounced for satirical news clips than for serious partisan news clips. Beyond selective exposure, we will also examine subsequent effects by testing H4 and H5 outlined below. For the purpose of studying message effects, much care will be taken to make the satirical news and serious news messages beyond the targeted experimental differentiation as equivalent as possible. This approach will avoid issues with unequivocal control conditions that the narrative persuasion research faces, as discussed above. In light of the discussion of whether political news and satire news in particular help or hinder broader political views such as political cynicism and alienation (e.g., Guggenheim, Kwak, & Campbell, 2011), the present investigation will examine whether internal political

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 12 efficacy is affected by selecting and viewing satirical news. Scholars studying political entertainment have taken particular interest in internal political efficacy (e.g., Becker, 2014) because it is a key factor in political engagement and behavior (Kenski & Stroud, 2006). Several studies (Baumgartner & Morris, 2006; Becker, 2011) found satire news exposure positively related to internal political efficacy. Hence, H4 postulates such impact for selective exposure. H4: Selective exposure to satirical news increases internal political efficacy. Lastly, while several investigations have demonstrated that selective exposure along the lines of a confirmation bias strengthens preexisting views (e.g., Knobloch-Westerwick, 2012), research on user-generated satire online video impacts on attitudes has not shown such impacts (Rill & Cardiel, 2013). Hence, the current study tests again whether attitude shifts may occur from exposure to serious partisan news and satire news presented in online clips. H5: Selective exposure to (a) serious partisan and (b) satirical online news clips reinforces political attitudes. Method Overview A lab experiment with 146 participants served to test the hypotheses, while stimuli were pretested with 102 participants. In the main experiment, after baseline measures about political attitudes and efficacy and a short distractor task, participants were presented with an online video site. In total, they were shown three overview pages on which they could choose to watch videos. Each overview page provided eight selections of news videos on a single topic (climate change, gun control, or immigration), with four presented as serious partisan news clips and four as satirical news clips. For the topics of gun control and climate change, participants made an initial choice, watched a corresponding video clip, and then returned to the results page to make a

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 13 second choice. They then completed this process for the second topic (either gun control or climate change), and finally were presented with a last overview page with eight video choices for the topic of immigration, where only an initial choice is made without a corresponding video. After this, the participant completed a series of posttest and demographic questions. Participants One hundred forty-six participants were recruited from undergraduate communication courses at a large Midwestern university and received either course credit or extra credit for participation in the main experiment. Two thirds of the participants were female (67.1%) and were predominantly aged between 18 and 25 (M = 21.05, SD = 2.99). A separate sample of 102 students served to pretest the stimuli content, in return for extra credit, and had a similar demographic makeup of gender (62.7% female) and age (M = 21.75, SD = 1.69). Procedure The main experiment was conducted as a computerized lab session and was presented in four separate parts the baseline questionnaire on attitudes, then a distracter task, the selective exposure task framed as research on Navigation and Evaluation of Video Clip Site Result Pages, and finally post-exposure measures. This setup served to veil the purpose of the research to avoid demand characteristics. Participants were seated in cubicles with a laptop and headphones. After general verbal instructions, participants took the computerized session. First, baseline attitudes measures about the target topics (climate change, gun control, and immigration) were presented along with five distractor topics. Participants next indicated their attitude importance and certainty for each topic and completed political efficacy items. Second, in the distractor task, participants evaluated nine black-and-white patterns that were allegedly considered for website designs.

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 14 Third, participants read a short instructional page before beginning the selective exposure task. Participants were presented with an overview page from a video clips webpage (see Figure 1 for an example screen shot) and instructed to select the video which seemed most interesting to them. The overview page depicted eight leads for either climate change or gun control, with both the topic and the arrangement of the eight leads randomly generated to eliminate order effects. Upon making a selection, they were taken to a page which played the corresponding video. Participants watched the video in its entirety, or until they wished to proceed (their time on the page was unobtrusively tracked), and were able to rate the video (from one to 10 stars). Afterwards, they returned to the list of videos available from an overview page where their first choice was marked as watched and were able to make a second choice. Participants were informed after their second selection that they had reached a time limit for that portion and were taken to the next overview page (the topic out of climate change and gun control not yet presented) without a second video. Here, they repeated the same procedure (made an initial choice, watched the corresponding video, made a second choice without video exposure) and were finally asked to choose from an overview page of videos on immigration. With this final choice, three different topics were included, aiding with the breadth of message topics and increasing the possible range in the key dependent measures (zero to five). However, to avoid participant fatigue, they did not actually watch a video on immigration and were informed they had reached a time limit. At this point, the selective exposure task was completed. In the fourth part, participants completed another set of questions on political attitudes on the issues and their political efficacy. Additional measures pertained to media consumption, attitudes towards news and political satire, general interest in politics, and demographics including partisanship. Lastly, they were thanked for their participation.

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 15 Experimental Manipulation Stimuli pretest. Stimuli pretest procedure and measures are presented in Appendix A. Video leads and source cues. In the main experiment, for each of the three topics (gun control, climate change, and immigration) online news video selections were made from an overview page with eight choices (see example in Figure 1), which included two leads for each of four categories: conservative partisan news, conservative satire news, liberal partisan news, and liberal satire news. Thus, the video choices were presented as either serious partisan news or satirical news, and as either having a conservative or a liberal stance. On the overview page, each available video clip was identified as either news or satire along with a serious partisan news source cue or a satire news cue. The ideological stance of each video was not explicitly mentioned; instead, it was indicated by a source cue and a lead (see appendices for stimuli pretest details, specifically Appendices B and C). Video captions and source cues. The video stimuli were created for the climate change and gun control topics using publically available footage from C-SPAN. Thus, similar to popular satire formats, actual news footage was used; the ridiculing commentary was incorporated in news ticker style, in line with actual visual TV news satire show elements (Baym & Jones, 2013). In the serious news version, these video captions featured news-style texts. To keep the length of the research session manageable while avoiding respondent fatigue, the third topic of immigration did not actually feature video materials; instead, after making an alleged choice, respondents received a message that time limitation would not allow them to watch the video. For climate change, a roundtable broadcast on July 6, 2014 was utilized which involved a moderator-led discussion between spokespeople for both a liberal and conservative group (C- SPAN, 2014a). This roundtable discussion was edited down to a 2:32 minute video, ensuring

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 16 equal time dedicated to both spokespeople. For gun control, two separate interviews were edited together into a single video clip with break cuts, similar in style to clip sequences used in both satirical news-style programs and serious partisan news shows. These clips were taken from interviews with a gun control activist and gun rights activist both broadcast during 2014, on July 2 (C-SPAN, 2014c) and December 13 (C-SPAN, 2014b), respectively. These were edited down for time and equal time devoted to each pundit, and packed together into a 2:59 minute video. These two baseline videos were then manipulated to create four versions of each that corresponded with the four categories of stimuli available: conservative serious news, conservative satire news, liberal serious news, and liberal satire news. This manipulation used a lower third graphic on screen (see screen shot in Figure 2) with a source logo shown throughout. For the satire news versions, the source logos were either Mock the Week or The Spoof, for partisan news versions, the logos were either Fox News or MSNBC, aligned with the targeted political stance of the clip and the associated lead, such that the Fox News logo appeared with the conservative clip and the MSNBC logo appeared in the liberal clip. Further, video captions (see Appendix D) served to present the clip as a serious news clip or as a satirical news clip, either leaning conservative or leaning liberal. Captions for these four categories were developed by the research team and pretested (see appendices for details). The captions were displayed to the right of the logo sequentially, and during each video, a full-screen title card also appeared which included the source logo and the lead which was selected from the overview page. The appearance of captions was linked to the content of the underlying video clip, such that humorous captions which disparaged specific individuals in the clip appear in tandem with their on-screen presence. Display times of the title card and the captions were uniform between videos on the same topic, such that the first caption appeared and disappeared

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 17 at the same timestamp in each gun control video, as did the second caption, and so on. This resulted in eight videos for each topic which were identical with one another but for our specific manipulations (there were two videos for each condition by topic, differing only because the title card text referred to the caption which the participant selected on the overview). Measures in Main Study Partisanship. Partisanship was captured at the end of the session, along with demographics: (1) Generally speaking, do you usually think of yourself as a Republican, a Democrat, an Independent, or something else? If neither Republican nor Democrat was picked, the next question was (2) Do you think of yourself as closer to the Republican Party or to the Democratic Party? with a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = closer to Republican Party to 7 = closer to Democrat Party; participants with scores below 4 were categorized as Republicanleaning while those with scores above 4 as Democrat-leaning. For all Republican and Republican-leaning participants (n = 61, 42%), partisanship was coded -1. For all Democrat or Democrat-leaning participants (n = 70, 48%), partisanship was coded 1. Ultimately, 15 participants (10%) could not be categorized, as they picked 4 on question (2) and were thus placed into a neutral category (coded 0). Selective exposure. The five clip choices each individual participant made were recorded and categorized for measures of selective exposure to (a) liberal partisan news (26% of the choices, or M = 1.30 of the five individual selections, SD = 1.02), (b) conservative partisan news (27% of the choices, or M = 1.36, SD = 1.06), (c) liberal satire news (22% of the choices, M = 1.12, SD = 1.02), and (d) conservative satire news (25%, or M = 1.23, SD =.85). Further measures were derived as follows: Consonant partisan news exposure was equivalent to (a) for Democrat-leaning participants and equivalent to (b) for Republican-leaning

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 18 participants; and vice versa for dissonant partisan news. Then consonant satire news exposure was equivalent to (c) for Democrat-leaning participants and equivalent to (d) for Republicanleaning participant, and vice versa for dissonant satire news. Political attitudes. Respondents were asked to provide their attitudes towards a list of issues on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly oppose, 7 = strongly support) before and after the selective exposure task. Attitude measures regarding the three target topics (climate change; gun control; immigration) were embedded in five distractor topics. The target items were governmental measures to combat climate change, decrease gun control, and deport illegal immigrants. To derive a condensed attitude measure, two of the items were reverse-coded to always have higher scores indicate a more liberal attitude and then all pre-selective exposure attitude measures were collapsed, and so were the post-selective exposure measures (see descriptives in Table 1). Internal political efficacy. Four items ascertained internal political efficacy (adopted from Niemi, Craig, & Mattei, 1991) on an 11-point scale, with 0 = strongly disagree and 10 = strongly agree, both prior to and following the selective exposure task. Thus, two condensed internal political efficacy measures were collected, one for internal efficacy before and another for internal efficacy after the selective exposure task (descriptives reported in Table 1). Clip ratings. After viewing a clip, participants rated it on a 10-star scale, similar to giving feedback on many popular websites (e.g., IMDB.COM uses 10 star ratings). On average, the gun control clip received M = 5.5 stars (SD = 2.2) and the climate change clip received M = 5.47 stars (SD = 2.2), which plausibly suggests that the stimuli quality was convincing. The satire versions received average ratings close to the mid-point of the scale (for gun control M = 5.3, SD = 2.2; for climate change M = 4.9, SD = 2.1), attesting to their convincing appeal. Results

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 19 Selective Exposure Partisan news vs. satire news. The first hypothesis suggested that, compared to recipients with higher political interest, recipients with low political interest select satirical news more frequently. An ANOVA with the number of times serious partisan news clips and the number of times satire news clips were selected as repeated measures examined H1. It also controlled for partisanship (Democrat-leaning, Republican-leaning, or neutral), political interest, and biological sex of the participants. Participants tended to select serious news more often, M = 2.66 (SD = 1.21), than satire news, M = 2.34 (SD = 1.21), F(1, 139) = 3.62, p =.059, η² =.025. In line with H1, lower political interest led to picking satire news clips more frequently, F(1, 139) = 5.28, p =.023, η² =.037, B = -.10. No other effects in this analysis were significant. Confirmation bias. Regarding H2 about a general confirmation bias, an ANOVA with number of conservative versus liberal choices as repeated measures and partisanship (Republican- vs. Democrat-leaning) as between-group factor was conducted. An interaction emerged between message stance and partisanship, F(1, 129) = 14.31, p <.001, η² =.10: Republicanleaning participants picked more conservative clips, M = 2.97 (SD = 1.08), than liberal clips, M = 2.03 (SD = 1.08), which was significantly different (p =.001) per subsequent test with Sidak correction. Democrat-leaning participants picked fewer conservative clips, M = 2.23 (SD = 1.14) than liberal clips, M = 2.77 (SD = 1.14), which differed at p =.044. Thus H2 was supported. Confirmation bias for partisan vs. satire news. The third hypothesis pertained to two competing suggestions: The confirmation bias will be either more pronounced for serious partisan news clips than for satirical news clips (H3a) or, to the contrary, more pronounced for satirical news clips than for serious partisan news clips (H3b). To test H3, the ANOVA model above was extended with a within-subjects factor for partisan vs. satire news, such that now four

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 20 different selective exposure measures were included: (a) liberal partisan news, (b) conservative partisan news, (c) liberal satire news, and (d) conservative satire news. Further, political interest was included as a covariate. This analysis showed again the impact of political interest on selection of partisan vs. satire news through an interaction between the within-subjects factor for format and the covariate, F(1, 128) = 4.67, p =.033, η² =.035, per H1. It also revealed a confirmation bias per H2 through an interaction between partisanship and message stance, F(1, 128) = 14.48, p <.001, η² =.102. The three-way interaction between partisanship, message stance, and message format fell short of significance, p =.103. Subsequent tests with Sidak correction showed that the partisans differed in choosing conservative partisan news, with Republican-leaning picking them more frequently than Democrat-leaning participants (M = 1.66, SD = 1.05, vs. M = 1.13, SD = 1.03, p =.004); when choosing liberal partisan news, the partisans differed with Democrat-leaning picking them more frequently than Republican-leaning participants (M = 1.57, SD = 1.15, vs. M = 1.06, SD =.81, p =.005). Further, Republican-leaning participants exhibited a confirmation bias both when choosing from serious news (by picking conservative partisan news, M = 1.67, SD = 1.05, more often than liberal partisan news, M = 1.07, SD =.81, p =.007) and when choosing from satire news (by picking conservative satire news, M = 1.31, SD =.87, more often than liberal satire news, M =.97, SD =.91, p =.045). However, Democrat-leaning participants had only a significant confirmation bias when choosing from serious partisan news, by picking liberal partisan news more than conservative partisan news (M = 1.57, SD 1.15, vs. M = 1.13, SD = 1.03, p =.028). Their satire news choices had no significant confirmation bias, p =.530. Hence, although the findings speak more for H3a than for the competing hypothesis H3b, the related three-way interaction fell short of significance. Political Efficacy Shift

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 21 Regarding H4 that selective exposure to satirical news increases internal political efficacy, a regression analysis utilized the post-selective exposure internal efficacy measure as criterion while using the selections of conservative and liberal satire clips as predictors while controlling for biological sex, political interest, partisanship, and baseline efficacy. It yielded that both the liberal satirical news selections (beta =.079, p =.007) and conservative satirical news selections (beta = -.081, p =.007) influenced efficacy, albeit in opposite ways, while the control variables partisanship (beta = -.066, p =.023), political interest (beta =.113, p =.003) and, of course, baseline internal political efficacy (beta =.855, p <.001) were also significant influences. Hence, H4 did not find straightforward support, because internal political efficacy was influenced differently by different-stances satire. Attitude Reinforcement Regarding H5, it was examined whether the use of the online video site with serious and satirical news had impacts on political attitudes. Mediation analyses (Preacher & Hayes, 2008) were conducted to assess whether the pre-exposure attitudes were reinforced through the video clips chosen. The pre-exposure attitudes, averaged across the three target topics, served as the predictor variable (X), with higher scores indicating more liberal views. The post-exposure attitudes, again averaged across topics, served as the dependent variable (Y) with higher scores indicating more liberal views, while controlling for biological sex and political interest. The first mediation analysis differentiated exposure by both stance and format for the mediating variables but did not find any significant mediation effects that would speak to H5a and H5b specifically. However, when combining all selected liberal-leaning video clips (M1) and all satire clips (M2) across all topics to serve as two mediators, interesting insights emerged. The more liberal the pre-exposure attitudes were, the more liberal-stance videos were selected, coeff. =.18 (S.E. =

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 22.07), p =.010. In turn, the more liberal-stance videos were selected, the more the post-exposure attitudes tended toward a more liberal attitude, coeff. =.13 (S.E. =.07), but this impact was only marginally significant, p =.076. Most importantly, this analysis demonstrated that selective exposure reinforced attitudes: A significant indirect effect of pre-exposure attitude via liberal news video selections on post-exposure attitudes emerged, with a point estimate at.023 and a confidence interval.003 to.066. The only other significant effect emerged because the preexposure attitudes had, of course, a significant direct effect on the post-exposure attitudes, coeff. =.77 (S.E. =.06), p <.001. The number of satire choices had no impact on attitudes. These findings suggest that, in line with H5, political messages uniformly influenced attitudes. This pattern applied regardless of (serious or satirical) format. Discussion With satirical presentations of current news being widely popular, especially among younger audiences, it is relevant to examine how satire news is selected by and influences viewers, in comparison to serious partisan news. After all, how individuals learn about current public affairs very much shapes societal and political discourse. Satire news, as a form of embedding political information into entertaining formats, may serve as a gateway to engage individuals who are otherwise agnostic about politics more with the political process. Importantly, satire recipients might even engage more with counterattitudinal views and broaden their understandings of political issues. However, the downside may be that satire news is undermining people s sense of being able to influence politics and causing viewers to become more cynical about it, therefore adding to the polarization of attitudes in the electorate. Specific hypotheses related to these concerns were derived from cognitive dissonance theory and entertainment-education frameworks. H1 was supported because participants with

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 23 lower political interest picked satire news more often than people with greater interest in politics. Hence, satire news could potentially serve as a gateway of engaging more citizens in the political discourse it does not merely attract those who are already very involved with politics. For future research, it will be of great interest to observe actual subsequent choices after news satire viewing does such exposure then channel people actually into following up with related traditional news reports, into consuming more public affairs news, or do they possibly end up attending more traditional entertainment or soft news? For H2 on a confirmation bias in online news selections, clear support emerged hence, the rather robust evidence on Festinger s (1957) classic proposition that people prefer messages that support preexisting views was extended here to the context of selecting news videos online, although it should be noted that participants chose the videos based on brief text leads, while anticipating to see the related video. At the heart of our investigation were the two competing hypotheses H3a and H3b on whether the confirmation bias is weaker or stronger in a satire context. Some indication emerged in favor of H3a--that the confirmation bias is weaker for satirical news--because Democratleaning participants did not discriminate between conservative and liberal satire news clips. But the related interaction fell short of significance (p =.10), thus we do not have a clear basis for accepting H3a and rejecting H3b. It should be noted that findings from an above-mentioned study by Stroud and Muddiman (2013) diverged from the presently found indication, as their data showed that people selecting from serious news did not show a confirmation bias (in contrast to numerous studies that found a confirmation bias in news exposure, see review by Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015) while people selecting from satire news did. Possibly, when their participants were asked about political tolerance before that news text selection task, they were alerted to pursue a more balanced news diet. For the present experiment, the following

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 24 interpretation is offered for why Democrat-leaning participants did not shy away from counterattitudinal satire while Republican-leaning participants did: The recent prominent satirical news shows (such as The Daily Show ) generally attracted a more liberal audience (Mitchell et al., 2014) and may thus cater more toward liberal attitudes, which can lead Democrat-leaning recipients to think that they will not encounter much counterattitudinal material whenever they turn to satire. Republican-leaning recipients, on the other hand, may not generally find much attitude-consistent satire and, as a result, scrutinize and discriminate more when it comes to satire news choices. As a result, Democrat-leaning recipients may also discount satire content more easily (Nabi et al., 2007) and thus discriminate less between attitudeconsistent and -challenging satire. When addressing H4 regarding impacts of selective exposure to satire news on internal political efficacy, significant effects emerged, but they were more complicated than anticipated. Liberal satire news exposure fostered internal efficacy, whereas conservative satire news exposure undermined it. With conservativism emphasizing traditional values and authorities, critiques of leading figures in politics may create a sense of low efficacy among conservativeleaning media users, while the opposite might occur for the liberal-minded. Further research is needed to explore this idea of differential impacts of satire exposure depending on ideological preference. Lastly, analysis regarding H5 did not show differential attitudinal impacts but both serious partisan news exposure and satire news exposure bolstered existing views without detectable differentiation by format. Some strengths and limitations of the present study should be outlined: By considering and actually observing selectivity, it extends an important angle in political entertainment research that only Stroud and Muddiman (2013) took thus far. Indeed, a major contribution is the

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 25 observation of selections of entertainment-embedded versus traditional persuasive messages that were both accessible at the same time. Even though many scholars (Dal Cin et al., 2004; Slater & Rouner, 2002) argued that entertainment can overcome avoidance of persuasive messages more effectively than non-entertainment formats, no empirical work in health or political communication research has actually examined this proposition to our knowledge. The related hypothesis H3a, however, did not receive solid support general superiority of entertainmentframed persuasion due to overcoming avoidance cannot be inferred from the present evidence. Alongside the lack of effectiveness that forced-exposure designs in narrative persuasion research yielded (discussed above per de Graaf et al. s (2016) review), this observation unfortunately undermines the high hopes for entertainment-based persuasion (e.g., Kreuter et al., 2007). But further research is needed, in light of the current s work limitations: Only a small set of messages could be utilized, which means they cannot represent the vast diversity of political satire that comes in many shapes (e.g., print or audiovisual, horation or juvenalian satire, etc.) or disentangle impacts of different satire types. Our experimental stimuli were constructed for clean variation of political stance and serious vs. satirical news, which is pivotal for testing hypotheses on exposure effects. This approach aligns with many audiovisual satire programs that add commentary, occasionally subversive text (e.g. "The Colbert Report's" The Word feature), to regular news footage. It is one style utilized in this amorphous genre (Feinberg, 1967, p. 18). Future research should consider whether these results hold up for other satirical formats (e.g. presenter-based commentary). Further, recipients could not avoid political messages entirely future research could include non-political options to choose from. But the study used three topics, which is preferable to the common focus on just one topic serving as context of researching political ridicule. Additionally, as many studies in this line of research, the present

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 26 work relies on a student sample and has not examined delayed effects regarding how long attitudinal or efficacy impacts last or ultimately affect actual behavior. Future work should extend to more diverse samples and capture persuasion processes in more nuanced fashion, e.g. by looking at absorption, funniness perceptions, etc. (Boukes et al., 2015) as mediators and at delayed measures beyond short-term impacts. For instance, it is possible that the more disparaging the humor, the more recipients may engage in downward social comparison (Wills, 1981) with political actors and derive greater political efficacy. Some ideas pursued in the present work may go well beyond political communication and satire contexts. The use of humor may generally foster recipients efficacy, possibly through downward social comparison. Humor may generally help any message to stand out from the clutter of messages that compete for attention and encourage recipients to engage more with messages that might challenge their views than they otherwise would (Eisend, 2009). We are not aware of any research that has rigorously tested whether the addition of humor makes it more likely that a message will be attended to, so there is room for extending our research approach to commercials, public service announcements, fictional entertainment, etc. What is the bottom line of the present evidence? Satire news matters. It matters for political attitudes and their extremity and may indeed contribute to polarization. It matters for people s internal political efficacy, which subsequently shapes their engagement and interest in politics and the political discourse. Media users utilize satire news as well as serious news selectively, mostly bolstering preexisting views while the confirmation bias certainly applies to online videos on serious partisan news, it might be weakened for some recipients when they choose from satire news. And there is hope that political entertainment gets those involved who are otherwise agnostic regarding politics and boosts their political efficacy. No kidding!

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 27 References Balmas, M. (2012). When fake news becomes real: Combined exposure to multiple news sources and political attitudes of inefficacy, alienation and cynicism. Communication Research, 41, 430-454. doi: 10.1177/0093650212453600 Bartsch, A., & Schneider, F. M. (2014). Entertainment and politics revisited: How non escapist forms of entertainment can stimulate political interest and information seeking. Journal of Communication, 64, 369-396. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12095 Baum, M. A. (2002). Sex, lies, and war: How soft news brings foreign policy to the inattentive public. American Political Science Review, 96, 91-109. doi: 10.1017/S0003055402004252 Baumgartner, J., & Morris, J. S. (2006). The Daily Show effect candidate evaluations, efficacy, and American youth. American Politics Research, 34, 341-367. doi: 10.1177/1532673X05280074 Baym, G., & Jones, J. P. (2013). News parody and political satire across the globe. Routledge. Becker, A. B. (2011). Political humor as democratic relief? The effects of exposure to comedy and straight news on trust and efficacy. Atlantic Journal of Communication, 19, 235-250. doi: 10.1080/15456870.2011.622191 Becker, A. B. (2014). Playing with politics: Online political parody, affinity for political humor, anxiety reduction, and implications for political efficacy. Mass Communication and Society, 17, 424-445. doi: 10.1080/15205436.2014.891134 Bennett, W. L., & Iyengar, S. (2008). A new era of minimal effects? The changing foundations of political communication. Journal of Communication, 58, 707-731. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00410.x

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 28 Boukes, M., Boomgaarden, H. G., Moorman, M., & de Vreese, C. H. (2015). At Odds: Laughing and Thinking? The Appreciation, Processing, and Persuasiveness of Political Satire. Journal of Communication, 65, 721-744. doi: 10.1111/jcom.12173 C-SPAN (2014, July 6). Washington Journal series: Extreme weather and climate change. Retrieved from http://www.c-span.org/video/?320307-4/washington-journal-climatechange C-SPAN (2014, December 13). Washington Journal series: Gun rights. Retrieved from http://www.c-span.org/video/?323256-4/washington-journal-larry-pratt-gun-rights C-SPAN (2014, October 21). Washington Journal series: Pro-gun control efforts in campaign 2014. Retrieved from http://www.c-span.org/video/?322216-3/washington-journalchelsea-parsons-progun-control-efforts-campaign-2014 Dal Cin, S., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2004). Narrative persuasion and overcoming resistance. In E. S. Knowles & J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion (pp. 175-191). Mawah, NJ: Erlbaum. de Graaf, A. D., Sanders, J., & Hoeken, H. (2016). Characteristics of narrative interventions and health effects: A review of the content, form, and context of narratives in health-related narrative persuasion research. Review of Communication Research, 4, 88-131. doi: 10.12840/issn.2255-4165.2016.04.01.011 Donsbach, W. (2009). Cognitive dissonance theory A roller coaster career: How communication research adapted the theory of cognitive dissonance. In T. Hartmann (Ed.), Media choice: A theoretical and empirical overview (128-149). New York: Routledge. Eisend, M. (2009). A meta-analysis of humor in advertising. Journal of The Academy of Marketing Science, 37, 191-203.

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 29 Feinberg, L. (1967). Introduction to satire. Ames: Iowa State University Press. Feldman, L., & Young, D. G. (2008). Late-night comedy as a gateway to traditional news: An analysis of time trends in news attention among late-night comedy viewers during the 2004 presidential primaries. Political Communication, 25, 401-422. doi: 10.1080/10584600802427013 Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Guggenheim, L., Kwak, N., & Campbell, S. W. (2011). Nontraditional news negativity: The relationship of entertaining political news use to political cynicism and mistrust. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 23, 287-314. doi: 10.1093/ijpor/edr015 Hardy, B. W., Gottfried, J. A., Winneg, K. M., & Jamieson, K. H. (2014). Stephen Colbert's civics lesson: How Colbert Super PAC taught viewers about campaign finance. Mass Communication and Society, 17, 329-353. doi: 10.1080/15205436.2014.891138 Holbert, R. L. (2005). A typology for the study of entertainment television and politics. American Behavioral Scientist, 49, 436-453. doi: 10.1177/0002764205279419 Holbert, R. L., Tchernev, J. M., Walther, W. O., Esralew, S. E., & Benski, K. (2013). Young voter perceptions of political satire as persuasion: A focus on perceived influence, persuasive intent, and message strength. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 57, 170-186. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2013.787075 Kenski, K., & Stroud, N. J. (2006). Connections between internet use and political efficacy, knowledge, and participation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 50, 173-192. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem5002_1 Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2012). Selective exposure and reinforcement of attitudes and

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 30 partisanship before a presidential election. Journal of Communication, 62, 628-642. Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2015). Choice and preference in media use: Advances in selectiveexposure theory and research. New York: Routledge. Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Meng, J. (2009). Looking the other way: Selective exposure to attitude-consistent and counter-attitudinal political information. Communication Research, 36, 426-448. Knobloch-Westerwick, S., Mothes, C., Johnson, B. K., Westerwick, A., & Donsbach, W. (2015). Political online information searching in Germany and the U.S.: Confirmation bias, source credibility, and attitude impacts. Journal of Communication, 65, 489-511. Kreuter, M. W., Green, M. C., Cappella, J. N., Slater, M. D., Wise, M. E., Storey, D., et al. (2007). Narrative communication in cancer prevention and control: A framework to guide research and application. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 3, 221-235. LaMarre, H. L., Landreville, K. D., & Beam, M. A. (2009). The irony of satire: Political ideology and the motivation to see what you want to see in The Colbert Report. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 14, 212-231. doi: 10.1177/1940161208330904 Lowin, A. (1967). Approach and avoidance: alternate modes of selective exposure to information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1-9. Messing, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2014). Selective exposure in the age of social media: Endorsements trump partisan source affiliation when selecting news online. Communication Research, 41, 1042-1063. doi: 10.1177/0093650212466406 Mitchell, A., Gottfried, J., Kiley, J., & Matsa, K. A. (2014). Political polarization & media habits. Retrieved April 14, 2015, from http://www.journalism.org/2014/10/21/politicalpolarization-media-habits/

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 31 Moy, P., Xenos, M. A., & Hess, V. K. (2005). Communication and citizenship: Mapping the political effects of infotainment. Mass Communication & Society, 8, 111-131. doi: 10.1207/s15327825mcs0802_3 Mutz, D. C., & Nir, L. (2010). Not necessarily the news: Does fictional television influence realworld policy preferences? Mass Communication and Society, 13, 196-217. doi: 10.1080/15205430902813856 Nabi, R. L., Moyer-Gusé, E., & Byrne, S. (2007). All joking aside: A serious investigation into the persuasive effect of funny social issue messages. Communication Monographs, 74, 29-54. doi: 10.1080/03637750701196896 Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efficacy in the 1988 National Election Study. American Political Science Review, 85, 1407-1413. doi: 10.2307/1963953 Pariser, E. (2011). The filter bubble: What the internet is hiding from you. New York: Penguin. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior research methods, 40, 879-891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 Prior, M. (2007). Post-broadcast democracy: How media choice increases inequality in political involvement and polarizes elections. New York, N.Y.: Cambridge University Press. Rill, L. A., & Cardiel, C. L. (2013). Funny, ha-ha: The impact of user-generated political satire on political attitudes. American Behavioral Scientist, 57, 1738-1756. doi: 10.1177/0002764213489016 Satire. (2016). In Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved August 11, 2016, from https://www.britannica.com/art/satire

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 32 Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. (2002). Entertainment-education and elaboration likelihood: Understanding the processing of narrative persuasion. Communication Theory, 12, 173 191. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2002.tb00265.x Stroud, N. J., & Muddiman, A. (2013). Selective exposure, tolerance, and satirical news. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 25, 271-290. doi: 10.1093/ijpor/edt013 Westerwick, A., Kleinman, S., & Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2013). Turn a blind eye if you care: Impacts of attitude consistency, importance, and credibility on seeking of political information and implications for attitudes. Journal of Communication, 63, 432-453. Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245 271. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.90.2.245 Xenos, M. A., & Becker, A. B. (2009). Moments of Zen: Effects of The Daily Show on information seeking and political learning. Political Communication, 26, 317-332. doi: 10.1080/10584600903053569 Young, D. G. (2004). Late-night comedy in election 2000: Its influence on candidate trait ratings and the moderating effects of political knowledge and partisanship. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 48, 1-22. doi: 10.1207/s15506878jobem4801_1 Young, D. G. (2013). Laughter, learning, or enlightenment? Viewing and avoidance motivations behind The Daily Show and The Colbert Report. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 57, 153-169. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2013.787080 Zillmann, D., & Cantor, J. R. (1972). Directionality of transitory dominance as a communication variable affecting humor appreciation. Journal of personality and social psychology, 24, 191-198. doi: 10.1037/h0033384

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 33 Table 1: Descriptive Statistics of Measures in Main Experiment Measure M SD Cronbach s Alpha Political Attitudes Before Selective Exposure Climate change 5.01 1.58 - Gun control 3.12 1.77 - Immigration 3.88 1.57 - Condensed Attitudes (3 items above, 2 reversed) 4.92 1.37.73 Political Attitudes After Selective Exposure Climate Change 4.90 1.49 - Gun Control 3.45 1.89 - Immigration 3.68 1.58 - Condensed Attitudes (3 items above, 2 reversed) 4.67 1.43.75 Internal Political Efficacy Before Selective Exposure 5.86 2.20.86 After Selective Exposure 5.87 2.19.90

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 34 Figure 1: Example Screenshot of Overview Page with Video Clip Search Results

SELECTING SERIOUS OR SATIRICAL NEWS 35 Figure 2: Example Screenshot of Satire News Clip