Loudness of pink noise and stationary technical sounds

Similar documents
Rhona Hellman and the Munich School of Psychoacoustics

Loudness and Sharpness Calculation

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

ADVANCED PROCEDURES FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC NOISE EVALUATION

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

Experiments on tone adjustments

Soundscape and Psychoacoustics Using the resources for environmental noise protection. Standards in Psychoacoustics

Colour-influences on loudness judgements

Implementing sharpness using specific loudness calculated from the Procedure for the Computation of Loudness of Steady Sounds

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Predicting annoyance judgments from psychoacoustic metrics: Identifiable versus neutralized sounds

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

Psychoacoustics. lecturer:

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Characterization of sound quality of impulsive sounds using loudness based metric

Loudness of transmitted speech signals for SWB and FB applications

Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Fan Noise

Progress in calculating tonality of technical sounds

Binaural Measurement, Analysis and Playback

Sound design strategy for enhancing subjective preference of EV interior sound

INTER-NOISE AUGUST 2007 ISTANBUL, TURKEY

Largeness and shape of sound images captured by sketch-drawing experiments: Effects of bandwidth and center frequency of broadband noise

EFFECTS OF REVERBERATION TIME AND SOUND SOURCE CHARACTERISTIC TO AUDITORY LOCALIZATION IN AN INDOOR SOUND FIELD. Chiung Yao Chen

DIFFERENCES IN TRAFFIC NOISE MEASUREMENTS WITH SLM AND BINAURAL RECORDING HEAD

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England

Determination of Sound Quality of Refrigerant Compressors

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Analysing Room Impulse Responses with Psychoacoustical Algorithms: A Preliminary Study

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones

Using the BHM binaural head microphone

Relation between the overall unpleasantness of a long duration sound and the one of its events : application to a delivery truck

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

JOURNAL OF BUILDING ACOUSTICS. Volume 20 Number

Absolute Perceived Loudness of Speech

The quality of potato chip sounds and crispness impression

A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL STUDY OF LOW AMPLITUDE SUPERSONIC AIRCRAFT NOISE AND OTHER TRANSIENT SOUNDS

Basic Considerations for Loudness-based Analysis of Room Impulse Responses

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

2. Measurements of the sound levels of CMs as well as those of the programs

MASTER S THESIS. Sound Quality Evaluation of Floor Impact Noise Generated by Walking. Payman Roonasi

INTER-NOISE AUGUST 2007 ISTANBUL, TURKEY

Interior and Motorbay sound quality evaluation of full electric and hybrid-electric vehicles based on psychoacoustics

Generating the Noise Field for Ambient Noise Rejection Tests Application Note

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

The importance of recording and playback technique for assessment of annoyance

Calculation of Unsteady Loudness in the Presence of Gaps Through Application of the Multiple Look Theory

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Hybrid active noise barrier with sound masking

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

Robert Alexandru Dobre, Cristian Negrescu

Concert halls conveyors of musical expressions

Getting Started with the LabVIEW Sound and Vibration Toolkit

Good playing practice when drumming: Influence of tempo on timing and preparatory movements for healthy and dystonic players

Calibration of auralisation presentations through loudspeakers

Investigation of Digital Signal Processing of High-speed DACs Signals for Settling Time Testing

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Tinnitus Assessment Appointment

IP Telephony and Some Factors that Influence Speech Quality

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

EE-217 Final Project The Hunt for Noise (and All Things Audible)

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

Predicting Performance of PESQ in Case of Single Frame Losses

The influence of Room Acoustic Aspects on the Noise Exposure of Symphonic Orchestra Musicians

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations

DETECTING ENVIRONMENTAL NOISE WITH BASIC TOOLS

Binaural summation of loudness: Noise and two-tone complexes

An Introduction to the Spectral Dynamics Rotating Machinery Analysis (RMA) package For PUMA and COUGAR

FC Cincinnati Stadium Environmental Noise Model

Sound Quality of Wind Turbines

Department of Electrical & Electronic Engineering Imperial College of Science, Technology and Medicine. Project: Real-Time Speech Enhancement

A QUERY BY EXAMPLE MUSIC RETRIEVAL ALGORITHM

Multiband Noise Reduction Component for PurePath Studio Portable Audio Devices

CTP 431 Music and Audio Computing. Basic Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology (GSCT) Juhan Nam

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Pitch perception for mixtures of spectrally overlapping harmonic complex tones

Spectrum analysis and tone quality evaluation of piano sounds with hard and soft touches

PSYCHOACOUSTICS & THE GRAMMAR OF AUDIO (By Steve Donofrio NATF)

Study on the Sound Quality Objective Evaluation of High Speed Train's. Door Closing Sound

Effect of room acoustic conditions on masking efficiency

PsySound3: An integrated environment for the analysis of sound recordings

MIE 402: WORKSHOP ON DATA ACQUISITION AND SIGNAL PROCESSING Spring 2003

Next Generation Software Solution for Sound Engineering

SOUND LABORATORY LING123: SOUND AND COMMUNICATION

APPLICATION OF A PHYSIOLOGICAL EAR MODEL TO IRRELEVANCE REDUCTION IN AUDIO CODING

Investigation into Background Noise Conditions During Music Performance

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Perceptual thresholds for detecting modifications applied to the acoustical properties of a violin

Musicians Adjustment of Performance to Room Acoustics, Part III: Understanding the Variations in Musical Expressions

Do Zwicker Tones Evoke a Musical Pitch?

Temporal control mechanism of repetitive tapping with simple rhythmic patterns

How to Obtain a Good Stereo Sound Stage in Cars

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 (3), March /2000/107(3)/1589/9/$ Acoustical Society of America 1589

ENGINEERING COMMITTEE Interface Practices Subcommittee AMERICAN NATIONAL STANDARD ANSI/SCTE Composite Distortion Measurements (CSO & CTB)

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Replicability and accuracy of pitch patterns in professional singers

9.35 Sensation And Perception Spring 2009

Transcription:

Loudness of pink noise and stationary technical sounds Josef Schlittenlacher, Takeo Hashimoto, Hugo Fastl, Seiichiro Namba, Sonoko Kuwano 5 and Shigeko Hatano,, Seikei University -- Kichijoji Kitamachi, Musashino-shi, Tokyo 0-000 JAPAN, Technische Universität München,5 Osaka University ABSTRACT Although their basic concept is similar, the current standards for the calculation of loudness, ANSI S.-007 and DIN 5 (99), produce significantly different results for many kinds of sounds. While their values for pure tones can be explained by the equal loudness contours of their times, they also show huge discrepancies for broadband sounds. For this reason, extended psychoacoustic experiments were made in order to find target values for the loudness of pink noise at various levels. Furthermore, the performance of the algorithms was investigated by subjective tests on several technical sounds at a large scale of loudness levels. In all cases, the method of adjustment was used. The results suggest that DIN 5 (99) predicts loudness very well. Its estimations are within the interquartile range of the subjective evaluations. Keywords: Loudness, Pink Noise, Method of Adjustment. INTRODUCTION Pure tones are the probably most studied artificial sounds and targeted by many psychoacoustic experiments. Although broadband noise is more important for real problems, there exists comparatively few data about its loudness. Especially, there are few experiments where the loudness judgment was made by directly comparing complex sounds with khz pure tone. At first glance, this lack does not seem to be disturbing as loudness can be calculated by several procedures, namely DIN 5 (99), which is a refinement of ISO 5 B (975), and ANSI S.-007. All are based on the same model, however, the first and the third differ in their implementations. They target different versions of the equal loudness contours caused by a revision of ISO. Moreover, while DIN 5 (99) uses the Bark scale for critical bands, ANSI S.-007 realizes them with equivalent rectangular bandwidths (ERB) resulting in different models for masking and influence of bandwidth. This leads to significant discrepancies in their outcomes for broadband sounds. In the case of pink noise it is a shift of as much as 5 db []. That s why 0 participants were asked during this work to adjust the loudness of pink noise to the reference one of a khz pure tone in order to find clear target values according to the definition. Further subjective tests scrutinize the validity of the loudness algorithms with respect to technical sounds whereupon this work focuses on stationary ones. 5 josef.schlittenlacher@mytum.de hashimot@st.seikei.ac.jp fastl@mmk.ei.tum.de qzw000@nifty.com kuwano@see.eng.osaka-u.ac.jp hatano@st.seikei.ac.jp

. EXPERIMENTS. Equipment and stimuli The diotic sounds were reproduced via PC and fed via audio interface, amplifier and free-field equalizer [] to Beyer dynamic DT- (5 Ω) headphones. The transfer function of the first part of the setup, measured at the input of the equalizer, is flat up to 0 khz and shows an attenuation of less than db at 0 khz. The experiments took place in a sound-proof room of approximately 7 square meters. Pink noise was produced by HEAD acoustic s Artemis, covering 0 third octaves with cut-off frequencies of 0 Hz and 0 khz. Technical sounds were recorded with the binaural system Squadriga and filtered to get a monaural free field equivalent recording that can be judged by the algorithms. A frame that is as stationary as possible without distortions was chosen. All stimuli had a duration of second with a fall and rise time of 5 ms.. Procedure The method of adjustment was used. The standard stimulus (Ss) and comparison stimulus (Sc) were separated by second with a silent time of seconds until the pair started again. The participant could adjust the level of the Sc via the mouse while it was played and one second after. He or she could listen to the pair as often as needed until he/she thought that Sc and Ss were equally loud. The described procedure was repeated eight times whereas the adjustment started from the sound pressure level of Sc obviously louder or softer than that of Ss. The order of ascending (A) and descending (D) was as following: ADDADAAD. Before experiments, two trials of training were made, one ascending and one descending. In order to be permitted to the experiments, each participant had to pass an audiometry test, checking the hearing capability at frequencies from 5 Hz to khz in octave steps according to the JIS [].. Methods of computation The calculated loudness of pink noise was obtained with software provided by the group for technical acoustics of TU München (www.mmk.ei.tum.de/~kes/loudnessmeter) respectively the Cambridge University Hearing Group (hearing.psychol.cam.ac.uk/demos/demos.html). Calculations for technical noises were done by own Matlab code, fulfilling all examples of the appendix of ANSI S.-007 and also performing correctly for DIN 5 (99). Under assumption of stationarity, the input is a fast Fourier transformation (FFT) of a windowed frame in the middle of the signal. For DIN 5 (99), third octave levels were gained by summing all samples within its limiting frequencies and adding levels reduced by 0 db to the neighboring third octaves as suggested by DIN 5/A (00) because the FFT is much steeper than real thirdoctave filters.. Loudness of pink noise The loudness of pink noise was studied using the khz pure tone at 9 loudness levels from to phon at a spacing of 5 phon and in quasirandom order, whereas 7 phon always served as the first and tenth level. After that, pink noise was used as Ss at levels of and and the khz pure tone should be adjusted. As these series altogether took between one and three hours, the participant took some breaks between. The 0 participants were aged to with a median of.5, 5 of them female and 5 male. They were familiar with the method because they had completed a similar preliminary experiment. Figure illustrates the results with the abscissa showing sound pressure levels of the pink noise and the ordinate its loudness level as determined by the corresponding khz pure tone. Interquartile ranges are approximately 5 db, which is in the same order of magnitude as that of a similar experiment done by Zwicker for uniform exciting noise []. Intraindividual differences, which are not shown at the figure, are typically around db and seldom significantly larger. As shown by the horizontal red line, which is shifted slightly upwards and represents the second run of 7 phon, participants were able to reproduce their results very well. This is not only true for the average but also for most individuals. The vertical black line with the pure tone as Sc is lower than expected. A psychological reason could be the fact that the participant concentrates more on the adjustable stimulus and in this case focuses on frequencies around khz. DIN 5 (99) does not meet the target values exactly but its calculations always are within

5 0 75 level of khz pure tone (db) 70 5 0 55 50 5 0 5 0 0 5 0 5 0 5 50 55 0 5 70 75 level of pink noise (db) Figure Level of a khz pure tone that is as loud as a pink noise of 0 third octaves. Circles indicate medians, whiskers the interquartile range, the upper dashed line values as calculated by ANSI S.-007, the lower that of DIN 5 (99) the interquartile range. This means that at least 5 % of the evaluations lie on the other side than the remaining 75 %. It works best around sone and again good at very high loudness. By contrast, ANSI S.-007 performs worse. It is always higher than the other standard which already predicts too loud in a wide range. However, it must be mentioned that its output becomes better for low levels and it cannot be excluded that it is very good for faint sounds that have not been part of the experiment. This is notably because ANSI S.-007 pays much attention to the absolute threshold of quiet..5 Loudness of technical sounds A second experiment investigated the loudness of stationary technical sounds. They were chosen to cover a large extent of the audible range as well as various possible sources of annoyance. They go from a notebook fan noise at medium rotation and. sone to a hair dryer of almost 5 sone and are listed at Table. Table Technical sounds of the second experiment no. sound LL (phon) Notebook fan, medium. Notebook fan, high 5.9 Sedan gasoline interior.0 Hatchback gasoline interior.9 5 Urban ambient noise at night.9 Sedan diesel interior 7.0 7 Vacuum cleaner 7.9 Hair dryer Because of the results of the previous experiment, the technical sound was chosen as Sc and the khz pure tone as Ss. Each sound was reproduced at a typical loudness level. The trials of.

participants yield 9 evaluations per sound. All participants took part in the pink noise experiment before. The results are represented by Figure, with the ordinate showing the real loudness as given by the khz pure tone, which the noise was adjusted to, and the abscissa showing the point of subjective equality (PSE) as it would be calculated by the algorithms. The diagonal line marks a perfect computation. Inter- and intrapersonal differences are in the same range as at the previous experiment. 0 0 N (sone) of khz Ss 5.5 sound sound 7 sound sound sound sound sound sound 5.5 5 0 N (sone(gf)): PSE by DIN 5 (99) Figure Loudness of technical sounds: The ordinate shows the loudness as given by the fixed khz pure tone, the abscissa the PSE as calculated by DIN 5 of 99 (left) and ANSI S.-007 (right). Circles and whiskers indicate medians and interquartile ranges, respectively. N (sone) of khz Ss 5.5 sound 7 sound sound sound sound sound sound 5 sound.5 5 0 N (sone(gf)): PSE by ANSI S. 007 Both standards predict the loudness of all sounds higher than it actually is. Their performance is in accord with that found for pink noise, meaning DIN 5 (99) is closer to the results. Furthermore it estimates the values of the vehicle interior noises, vacuum cleaner and hair dryer very well and is almost all times within the interquartile range. This accuracy is remarkable because the input was based on a rather simple FFT and in terms of DIN 5/A (00) most of the sounds would be considered just almost stationary. Although ANSI S.-007 suggests higher values, the relations among its calculations are correct. The notebook fan noises are overestimated most. A reason could be that they show strong spectral components, in particular the medium one has got a dominant one at. khz. As it is very close to the reference stimulus, the participant could tend to not evaluate the total loudness but that of the component masked by noise. The urban ambient noise is judged a bit irregularly. It may be caused by the most difficult recording conditions, neither in laboratory nor at least a closed car which in turn could lead to more time variance.. CONCLUSIONS Extended psychoacoustic experiments, which have found target values for the loudness of pink noise at various levels, have shown that the estimations of DIN 5 (99) are close to the subjective evaluations. It is always within the interquartile range. This is still true for many technical sounds which are only almost stationary. As the standard meets the experimental output for that many sounds, it can be expected that it also determines specific loudness very well. Its graphical diagrams seem to represent a good model for the main loudness within a critical band. By contrast, the outcomes of ANSI S.-007 are too high for all tested sounds, indicating that it needs further refinement. Nevertheless, the algorithm is an interesting approach as it provides many details at intermediate steps. Pure tones are adequate to show many aspects of loudness, for example frequency dependency. However, it is also very important to consider the psychoacoustic facts of broadband noises which are more similar to environmental sounds.

REFERENCES [] H. Fastl, F. Völk and M. Straubinger. Standards for calculating loudness of stationary or timevarying sounds, Proc. Inter-Noise (Ottawa 009) [] Hugo Fastl and Eberhard Zwicker, Psychoacoustics. Facts and Models (Springer, Berlin, New York, 007) [] JIS T 0 (000). Audiometers [] E. Zwicker. Über psychologische und methodische Grundlagen der Lautheit, Acustica,, 7-5 (95) [5] ANSI S.-007: Procedure for the Computation of Loudness of Steady Sounds [] DIN 5 (99): Berechnung des Lautstärkepegels und der Lautheit aus dem Geräuschspektrum Verfahren nach E. Zwicker (March 99) [7] DIN 5/A (000): Berechnung des Lautstärkepegels und der Lautheit aus dem Geräuschspektrum Verfahren nach E. Zwicker Änderung : Berechnung der Lautheit zeitvarianter Geräusche [] ISO (00): Acoustics Normal equal-loudness-level contours [9] ISO 5 (975): Method for calculating loudness level 5