The comparison of two rhythm syllable systems on the meter recognition abilities of second and third grade students

Similar documents
Rhythm Syllable Systems

Rhythm and Tonal Patterns

The effect of male timbre vocal modeling in falsetto and non-falsetto on the singing and pitch accuracy of second grade students

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RHYTHM APTITUDE AND RHYTHM ACHIEVEMENT OF FIRST, SECOND, AND THIRD GRADE STUDENTS. Kelly Kristine Harding

EFFECTS OF ORFF-SCHULWERK PROCESS OF IMITATION ON ELEMENTARY STUDENTS READING FLUENCY

Central Valley School District Music 1 st Grade August September Standards August September Standards

Effects of physical movement in rhythm instruction in early instrumentalist development

Choral Sight-Singing Practices: Revisiting a Web-Based Survey

Boogies and Blues. By Marilyn Lowe with Michael Brill. In cooperation with Edwin E. Gordon

THE EFFECTS OF AUDIATION ON THE MELODIC ERROR DETECTION ABILITIES OF FOURTH AND FIFTH GRADE BAND STUDENTS RICHARD D. BECKMAN II

Danville School District #118 Fine Arts Music Curriculum and Scope and Sequence Third Grade - First Quarter

The effects of range of notes, gender, and songs with text or without text on the vocal accuracy of first, second and third grade students

PERFORMING ARTS Curriculum Framework K - 12

Choir Scope and Sequence Grade 6-12

Music. on Scale and. Specificc Talent Aptitude: Visual Arts, Music, Dance, Psychomotor, Creativity, Leadership. Performing Arts,

Reading and Writing Music Notation

Developing Musicianship through Improvisation: An Eclectic Teaching Module for Pre-school Children Preeyanun Promsukkul

Rhythmic Characteristics of Improvisational Drumming Among Preschool Children

Examining the Effect of Aural Preparation on Second Grade Students

Course Syllabus. SchMu Spring Semester 2014 Methods in Elementary Music Semesters Hours: 3

Improvisation Is Elementary: It s all about Readiness!

THE EFFECT OF REPERTOIRE ON THE SINGING ACHIEVEMENT OF SECOND GRADE STUDENTS. Christine Elyse Hadfield

First Grade General Music Curriculum

The effects of harmonic accompaniment on the music achievement, aptitude, and musical effectiveness of first grade children

Recruitment and retention : the influence of general music teachers methodology on secondary music ensembles

Grade 4 General Music

Introduction to Music Moves for Piano Book 4

California Subject Examinations for Teachers

READINESS FOR BAND: WHAT DO BEGINNER BAND STUDENTS NEED TO KNOW? Angela G. Cascione

Differentiated Approaches to Aural Acuity Development: A Case of a Secondary School in Kiambu County, Kenya

University of Western Ontario Don Wright Faculty of Music Kodaly Summer Music Course KODÁLY Musicianship Level I SYLLABUS

CONCERT ORCHESTRA AND SYMPHONIC ORCHESTRA

Title: Audiation, Improvisation, and Music Learning Theory. Author(s): Christopher Azzara

LYRASIS Members and Sloan Foundation

COURSE TITLE: CONCERT CHOIR GRADES 9-12 LENGTH: FULL YEAR SCHOOLS: RUTHERFORD HIGH SCHOOL RUTHERFORD, NEW JERSEY DATE:

Music Moves for Piano

CURRICULUM. Grade 3 Music TEACHER WORKBOOK

Syllable systems : four students' experiences in learning rhythm.

General Music. The following General Music performance objectives are integrated throughout the entire course: MUSIC SKILLS

Rhythmic Notation Unit Plan

WESTFIELD PUBLIC SCHOOLS Westfield, New Jersey

WaunAkee. MIDDLe SCHOOL Vocal MUSIC. TeacherS MS.Roberts and Mrs. Thomas

THE EFFECTS OF COMPUTER MUSIC LEARNING ACTIVITIES ON THE TONAL APTITUDES OF CANADIAN STUDENTS ALLAN F. ANDERSON

Honors Music Theory South Carroll High School : Fall Semester

MILLSTONE TOWNSHIP SCHOOL DISTRICT MUSIC CURRICULUM GRADE: FIRST

Chamber Orchestra Course Syllabus: Orchestra Proficient Joli Brooks, Jacksonville High School, Revised August 2016

Newport Public Schools Curriculum Framework. Subject: Classroom Music Standard #1 Grade Level Grade 1 Standard Benchmarks Suggested Resources/

Chorus 6- Beginning Level

MUSC 103 Materials and Design Wesleyan University Fall 2012, T/R 9:00 10:20

Grade-Level Academic Standards for General Music

The Hidden Curriculum in the Instrumental Music Class

Pine Hill Public Schools Curriculum

Saint Patrick High School

AP Music Theory Syllabus CHS Fine Arts Department

Concert Band and Wind Ensemble

2017 Revised August 2015 Developed August 2013

AP MUSIC THEORY SUMMER ASSIGNMENT AP Music Theory Students and Parents,

Geneva CUSD 304 Content-Area Curriculum Frameworks Grades 6-12 Choral Music

Keyboard Area Handbook for Undergraduate and Graduate Students in Applied Keyboard Courses

Shrewsbury Borough School Visual and Performing Arts Curriculum 2012 Music Grade 1

Title: The Application of Gordon s Empirical Model of Learning Sequence to Teaching the Recorder. Author(s): June C. McDonald

AUDITION PROCEDURES:

MUSIC CURRICULUM GUIDELINES K-8

OSPI-Developed Performance Assessment. A Component of the Washington State Assessment System. The Arts: Music. Cartoon Soundtrack.

Music NORTH WARREN CLUSTER GRADE K - 3

COURSE OF STUDY UNIT PLANNING GUIDE INSTRUMENTAL MUSIC GRADE LEVEL: 4-5 REVISED AUGUST 2017 ALIGNED TO THE NJSLS FOR VISUAL AND PERFORMING ARTS

Grade One General Music

Key Assessment Criteria Being a musician

Audiation: Ability to hear and understand music without the sound being physically

Jessicah R. Saldana. Songs and Chants

Stafford Township School District Manahawkin, NJ

K12 Course Introductions. Introduction to Music K12 Inc. All rights reserved

Elementary Music Curriculum Objectives

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

MUSICAL KEYBOARDING 1-4

A comparison of the Kodaly method and the traditional method to determine pitch accuracy in grade 6 choral sight-singing

Praxis Music: Content Knowledge (5113) Study Plan Description of content

The Nature of Children's Singing Voices: Characteristics and Assessment

UNIVERSITY OF WATERLOO Music Department- Conrad Grebel University College Music Fundamentals of Music Theory FALL 2013

Introduction to Music Moves for Piano Book 5

Student Performance Q&A:

Grade 3 General Music

Course Overview. Assessments What are the essential elements and. aptitude and aural acuity? meaning and expression in music?

MUS 173 THEORY I ELEMENTARY WRITTEN THEORY. (2) The continuation of the work of MUS 171. Lecture, three hours. Prereq: MUS 171.

Beat - The underlying, evenly spaced pulse providing a framework for rhythm.

CARLISLE AREA SCHOOL DISTRICT Carlisle, PA Elementary Classroom Music K-5

Integrating Orff, Kodály, and Eurhythmics with Integrity

Woodlynne School District Curriculum Guide. General Music Grades 3-4

Indiana Music Standards

Chapter Five: The Elements of Music

AP Music Theory Syllabus

Intermediate Piano Syllabus and Course Outline

Bi-Borough Music Curriculum

Most Sincerely,

Strand 1: Music Literacy

Greenwich Music Objectives Grade 4 General Music

Oakview and Red Bank Elementary Music: First-Second Grade Standards Based Report Card Rubric

CURRICULUM FOR CONCERT BAND GRADES 9-12

Geneva CUSD 304 Content-Area Curriculum Frameworks Grades 6-12 Choral Music

International School of Kenya

Transcription:

Rowan University Rowan Digital Works Theses and Dissertations 5-16-2003 The comparison of two rhythm syllable systems on the meter recognition abilities of second and third grade students Richard D. Beckman II Follow this and additional works at: http://rdw.rowan.edu/etd Part of the Elementary Education and Teaching Commons Recommended Citation Beckman, Richard D. II, "The comparison of two rhythm syllable systems on the meter recognition abilities of second and third grade students" (2003). Theses and Dissertations. 1259. http://rdw.rowan.edu/etd/1259 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Rowan Digital Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Rowan Digital Works. For more information, please contact LibraryTheses@rowan.edu.

THE COMPARISON OF TWO RHYTHM SYLLABLE SYSTEMS ON THE METER RECOGNITION ABILITIES OF SECOND AND THIRD GRADE STUDENTS by Richard D. Beckman II A Thesis Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the Master of Arts Degree of The Graduate School at Rowan University 2003 Approved by Date Approved essor yb 00 5 I I

ABSTRACT Richard D. Beckman II The Comparison of Two Rhythm Syllable Systems on the Meter Recognition Abilities of Second and Third Grade Students 2003 Thesis Advisor: Dr. Kristyn L. Kuhlman Master of Arts: Subject Matter Teaching Music Graduate Division of Rowan University The purpose of this study was to investigate the efficacy of rhythm syllable systems in developing students' aural discrimination abilities. The problem was to examine the effect of rhythm syllable systems on students' abilities to aurally identify the meter of rhythm patterns. A secondary problem was to examine the interaction effects of music aptitude and rhythm syllable systems. The sample for this study included 107 students in second and third grade general music classes. Classes remained intact and were randomly assigned to the control group, the time-based syllable group, and the function-based syllable group. Subjects were administered the Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation prior to the start of the treatment period to identify level of music aptitude and a student questionnaire to identify prior rhythmic activity experience. During the treatment period,

the students learned the same rhythm patterns: instruction differed only in the type of rhythm syllable system used. The time-based treatment group used Kodaly rhythm syllables, the function-based treatment groups used Gordon syllables, and the control group continued to use a neutral syllable. At the conclusion of the 14 week treatment period, all subjects were administered a Meter Identification Test in which they were expected to aurally identify patterns as being in duple or triple meter. An ANOVA revealed no significant differences among groups with regard to treatment and no interaction effects between treatment and level of rhythm aptitude. The researcher concluded that there were no differences in abilities of subjects who received rhythm instruction that utilized the function-based syllable system and those who used a time-based syllable system. Furthermore, subjects who used either syllable system failed to demonstrate greater meter identification abilities than students who used no syllable system as part of rhythm instruction.

MINI ABSTRACT Richard D. Beckman II The Comparison of Two Rhythm Syllable Systems on the Meter Recognition Abilities of Second and Third Grade Students 2003 Thesis Advisor: Dr. Kristyn L. Kuhlman Master of Arts: Subject Matter Teaching Music Graduate Division of Rowan University The purpose of this study was to investigate students' aural abilities with regard to rhythm syllable systems. An ANOVA revealed no significant differences among groups with regard to treatment and no interaction effects between treatment and level of rhythm aptitude. The researcher concluded that there were no differences in abilities of subjects who received rhythm instruction that utilized the function-based syllable system and those who used a time-based syllable system. Furthermore, subjects who used either syllable system failed to demonstrate greater meter identification abilities than students who used no syllable system as part of rhythm instruction.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am indebted to the following people who played an invaluable role in the completion of this thesis. Dr. Kristyn Kuhlman, my Graduate Advisor, whose excellent guidance, teaching and support were critical to the completion of this thesis. Dr. Kuhlman's vast amount of patience and kindness were also greatly appreciated. Dr. Lili Levinowitz, who has given me a solid foundation in music education through her courses at both the undergraduate and graduate level. To all of my students, they have taught me as much as I have taught them over the years. My parents, Richard and Elaine, who have always stressed the importance of education and provided me with love and encouragement. My wife, Diane, who was incredibly supportive throughout the writing process. Diane probably knows more about rhythm syllables then she ever wanted to know. Our baby, Jordan Kristine, who was born during this time. She provided me with the much needed drive to write this thesis and complete it on time. Lastly, I would like to thank my dog Cooper, who always knew when a walk would help clear my mind and focus my thoughts. Thank you for all of your love, help and encouragement. ii

TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS... L IS T O F T A B L E S... ii iv CHAPTER ONE Introduction and Purpose of the Study... 1 Intro du ctio n... 1 P ro b le m s... 5 CHAPTER TW O Related Research... 7 The Palmer Study... 7 Comparison between the Palmer study and the present stu dy... 8 The Bacon Study... 9 Comparison between the Bacon Study and the present study... 10 CHAPTER THREE Design of Study... 12 S a m p le... 12 P ro cedu res... 12 A n a ly sis... 14 CHAPTER FOUR Results and Interpretations... 15 Analysis of Variance for M eter Identification Test... 15 Interpretations... 17 iii

CH APTER FIVE Summ ary and Conclusions... 18 Purpose and Problem... 18 D e sig n... 18 R e su lts... 19 Conclusions...19 APPEND IX A... 21 APPEND IX B... 24 APPEND IX C... 29 APPEND IX D... 31 BIBLIOGRAPHY... 35 iv

LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations for Meter Recognition Test by treatment group... 15 Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and ANOVA Summary table for Meter Recognition Test... 16 v

Chapter One Introduction and Purpose of the Study Introduction The association of a set of rhythm syllables with a specific note value or rhythm pattern is common pedagogical practice in music education today. This practice is supported by music educators and music learning theorists who maintain that a rhythm syllable system is necessary in order to learn rhythm patterns efficiently and effectively (Bluestine, 2000). According to Gordon (1997), failure to implement a rhythmic syllable system makes it increasingly difficult for students to discriminate among patterns they need to learn. He further states that when more and more patterns are introduced and learned, the patterns "begin to sound alike to students unless they can organize the patterns by syllable names" (p. 97). Schleuter (1997) concurs, stating that the syllables become the 'words' for remembering and manipulating the rhythm patterns. A survey of music education textbooks reveals the diversity of rhythm syllable systems created specifically to help students learn rhythm. Generally, these systems are presented to the reader without bias or preference, providing little objective evidence regarding each systems' efficacy in developing students' rhythmic vocabulary. Without knowledge of research or current theories and knowledge about music learning, a teacher's choice of rhythm syllable system is often no more than the result of his or her familiarity with a certain system. According to Schlueter (1997), most teachers teach the way they were taught as children and "they seldom examine or question traditional 1

methods and techniques of instruction with regard to current theories and knowledge about music learning " (p. 20). Among the most popular of these systems are the Orffword chant, Kodaly syllables, and Gordon syllables. Generally the syllable systems are associated with the creator or proponent of the system such as Orff or Kodaly. The syllable systems, however, can be more appropriately categorized into the following classifications: (a) eurhythmic words, (b) mnemonic words, (c) numbering systems, (d) time-based syllables and (e) beatfunction syllables (Bluestine, 2000). Of these systems, the timed-based syllable system as represented by Kodaly is one of the most commonly utilized systems. This rhythm system is part of The Kodaly Concept which was created by Zoltan Kodaly, a Hungarian composer with an interest in music education. Kodaly adapted a rhythmic syllable system created by Emil Cheve for use in his Kodaly Concept (Mark, 1996). The Kodaly syllable system meets all of the requirements of a time-based syllable system in that certain syllables are associated with the durational value of notes. Each note value, such as a quarter or eighth note, is assigned a syllable regardless of how the note functions within the context of the rhythm pattern. These syllable assignments remain consistent in duple, triple, and uneven meters. For example, a quarter note is assigned with the syllable Ta and a pair of eighth notes are assigned with the syllables Ti Ti. In a time signature of 4/4, two quarter notes followed by four eighth notes would be chanted Ta - Ta - Ti Ti - Ti 7i (Figure 1). The same sounding pattern written in a time signature of 4/2 as two half notes followed by four quarter notes would be chanted Ta+a - Ta+a - Ta Ta - Ta Ta (Figure 2). The rhythm syllables remain consistent regardless of the meter. 2

tj J n n J J J J J J Ta Th Ti Ti T Ti wa Tw T«To To TA Figure 1 Figure 2 In triple meter with a time signature of 3/4, three quarter notes would be chanted Ta Ta Ta (Figure 3). The same sounding pattern written in a time signature of 3/8 with three eighth notes would be chanted Ti Ti Ti (Figure 4). Although students may not yet have learned to read rhythm notation, they are learning to associate a specific written note value with a syllable. Examples of the Kodaly syllable system for the patterns used in this study are found in Appendix A. Ta TR To 11 1 Figure 3 Figure 4 The time-based rhythm syllable system is contrasted by the beat function-based rhythm syllable system, a syllable system which takes into consideration how the notes function within a rhythm pattern regardless of value or duration. In a function-based rhythm syllable system the syllables are associated with the strong and weak beats of the rhythm pattern. The terms 'macrobeats' and 'microbeats' were coined by Gordon to describe the strong and weak beats in rhythm patterns. Gordon refers to the strong beat as the 'macrobeat" and the weak beat as the 'microbeat' (Gordon, 1997). In a functionbased syllable system the macrobeats are labeled with the same syllable, and the microbeats are labeled according to their function within a rhythm pattern and within a specific meter. This unique feature makes the function-based system the only syllable system to meet the criteria set forth by Schleuter (1997) that different but related syllables should be used for duple, triple, and unusual meters. The differences in syllables among 3

meters enables the student to easily discriminate among meters, both by differences in the feel of the meters as well as the syllables associated with meters. A function-based rhythm syllable system was developed by Gordon and described in his book, Learning Sequences In Music: Skill, Content, And Patterns (1997). In Gordon's function-based rhythm syllable system, the macrobeat is always labeled Du while the microbeats change dependent on whether the pattern is in duple or triple. In duple meter the microbeats would be labeled Du De. For example, in duple with a meter signature of 4/4, two quarter notes followed by four eighth notes would be chanted Du - Du - Du De - Du De (Figure 5). The same sounding pattern written in a meter of 4/2 would use the same syllables Du - Du - Du De - Du De (Figure 6) because the functions of the beats have not changed although the notation has. 4J J!: ~ i }J \- J, J J, nil nlu nlu ne D ni ee flfe fl t Figure 5 Figure 6 Adhering to Schleuter's criteria for syllable systems, Gordon's syllables for triple meter differ from the syllables for duple meter. The macrobeat is still labeled Du however the remaining microbeats are now labeled Da and Di. In triple meter with a time signature of 3/4, three quarter notes are chanted Du Da Di (Figure 7). The same sounding pattern written in a meter of 3/8 is still chanted Du Da Di because the function of the macro and microbeats has not changed (Figure 8). Further examples of the Gordon function-based syllable system for the patterns used in this study are found in Appendix A. SI SI nlj Jfu D i flj J 1) - ni, lh ni flt ni ni Figure 7 Figure 8 4

Few studies exist which compare the efficacy of time-based and function-based rhythm syllable systems. A five-month study conducted by Palmer (1976) investigated the effectiveness of a time-based syllable and a function-based syllable system on the rhythm reading achievement of fourth grade students. Palmer compared Richard's system, a time-based rhythm solfege system based on Kodaly, with a function-based system using syllables created by Gordon. Although no significant difference in students' rhythmic performances was found between syllable systems (p >.05), Palmer reported results favoring the function-based system through analysis of the gain scores. A similar twelve-week study was conducted by Bacon (1998) who investigated the use of the '1 e + a' mnemonic syllable system and the Gordon function-based rhythm syllable system. Beginning instrumental students were taught the same rhythm patterns in duple and triple meters but differed only in the counting approach. At the end of the treatment period, each student was administered a meter recognition test and performed four rhythm etudes on his or her instrument. Significant differences (p <.05) in performance of triple meter were found, the results favoring the students who used the Gordon rhythm syllables and the students in the control group who used no syllable system over the students who used the ' 1 e + a' syllable system. Problems Schleuter states that the purpose of syllable systems is to label the musical sounds, not the notation (1997), yet the studies by Palmer and Bacon focused on the efficacy of a rhythm syllable system on students' rhythm and meter performance through the use of reading notation. These studies did not investigate students' ability to aurally identify the musical sound of meters, and, in fact, there is a lack of information regarding the 5

effectiveness of rhythm syllable systems in developing aural identification of meters. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to investigate students' aural abilities with regard to rhythm syllable systems. The problem of the study is to examine the effect of rhythm syllable systems on students' abilities to aurally identify meter of rhythm patterns. A secondary problem is to examine the interaction effects of music aptitude and rhythm syllable systems. 6

Chapter Two Related Research The Palmer Study The purpose of this study was to compare effectiveness of the Richards and Gordon rhythm syllables in developing the rhythm reading abilities of fourth-grade students. The Richards rhythm syllable system is based upon Kodaly's rhythm syllable system in which the time values of notes are labeled with specific syllables. The researcher also compared the Richards and Gordon syllables to using no syllable system. The subjects for this study consisted of 136 fourth grade students from three elementary schools located in Florida. The researcher formed a Richards group (n = 48), a Gordon group (n = 50) and a control group (n = 38). Each group consisted of two intact general music classes. Students in the control group were from one elementary school: the remaining two schools each had one Richards group and one Gordon group. Subjects were administered the Musical Aptitude Profile (MAP) (Gordon, 1995) to determine level of music aptitude. Prior achievement of the subjects' rhythm reading and rhythm performance abilities were measured through a series of four pre-tests. Subsections of Colwell' s Music Achievement Test (MA4T), the MATIIH and Gordon's Iowa Test of Musical Literacy (ITML) were used to measure rhythm reading achievement. Rhythm performance achievement was measured by Palmer's self-designed test. Subjects' rhythm performances were recorded and evaluated on a five-point rating scale by a panel of three independent judges. Following the pre-testing, treatment was 7

administered for five months by the researcher. During this time, the Richard's group received instruction using Richard's time-based syllables, the Gordon group received instruction using Gordon's function-based syllables, and the control group received no special instruction. Each group met three times a week for 20 minutes, totaling 60 minutes a week. At the end of the treatment period, the subjects were re-evaluated with the same tests used for pre-testing. Palmer used an ANCOVA to analyze the differences in the gain scores of the subject's pre and posttests with music aptitude serving as a covariate. Although no significant differences were found at the p >.05 level, Palmer stated that the Gordon group had a slight increase of pre-test to post-test gain scores in performance achievement. The researcher concluded that although the Gordon syllable system was statically better in developing subject's rhythm performance achievement then the Kodaly syllables, other factors may have added to the results such as the 'Hawthorne' effect. Palmer also stated that the Kodaly and Gordon rhythm syllables were better then using no syllable system at all. Comparison between the Palmer study and the present study The Palmer study examined the effects of the Kodaly and Gordon syllable systems on subjects' rhythm reading performance and achievement. The present study examines the effect of rhythm syllables on subjects' abilities to aurally identify meters. Subjects are not required to read rhythm notation, but rather to associate syllables with the sound of rhythm patterns for the purpose of identifying meter. 8

Both studies included the use of a music aptitude test to measure subjects' rhythm aptitude prior to the start of the study as well as a researcher-designed performance assessment. Both studies also compare the interactive effects of music aptitude with differences between the Kodaly, Gordon, and control groups. The Bacon Study The purpose of this study was to compare the use of the 'le + a' counting system, Gordon rhythm syllables, and no syllable system in student performance achievement on an instrument. The researcher also sought to compare the different counting systems with regard to students' abilities to identify meter. For this study, subjects consisted of 81 beginning band students who were divided into three groups: the ' Ie + a' group (n = 25), the Gordon syllable group (n = 32) and the control group (n = 24) which used no counting system. The students' homogenous lesson groups were kept intact when assigned to a treatment group. The subjects for the twelve-week study were from a suburban middle school in central Michigan. Subjects were administered the MAP (Gordon, 1995) prior to the start of the treatment period to insure that level of music aptitude was normally distributed among the groups. Throughout the treatment period, instruction for each group was identical except for use of the counting system. Each group met for two 42-minute classes a week, totaling 24 classes during the treatment period. During each class, the researcher administered five to ten minutes of rhythm instruction in the beginning of the lesson. The remainder of the lesson time was spent on normal beginning instrumental instruction which was taught by the students' regular instrumental instructors. At the end of the treatment period, the 9

students were subjected to a researcher-designed meter recognition test and a performance test of four rhythm etudes. The rhythm etudes consisted of a duple meter prepared etude, a triple meter prepared etude, a duple meter unprepared etude, and a triple meter unprepared etude. The rhythm etudes were recorded by the researcher and rated by two independent judges using a five-point continuous rating scale. At the conclusion of the treatment period, the results of the meter recognition test were subjected to a one-way ANOVA. No significant differences were found between treatments for meter recognition. Results for the combined scores of the students' performance on rhythm etudes were also subjected to a one-way ANOVA. No significant differences were found as a result of treatment on the combined performances of etudes. However, an ANOVA was calculated on each individual etude and revealed significant results (p <.05) favoring the Gordon syllables and no syllables over the 'le + a' counting system on the triple meter prepared, triple meter unprepared, and combined triple meter etudes. Bacon concluded that "the results of this study indicate that Gordon or neutral rhythm syllables, when used in an aural/oral presentation teaching style, may be a better teaching system than what is typically used with popular method books." The researcher also suggests that rhythm aptitude may be a more significant factor in student performance than the use of a syllable system. Comparison between the Bacon study and the present study The Bacon study compared the effects of the 'le + a' counting system and the Gordon rhythm syllable system on subjects' abilities to discriminate meters and performance 10

achievement. Similar to the Bacon study, the present study examines the effects of Kodaly and Gordon rhythm syllable systems on the subjects' abilities to aurally discriminate meters. However, the subjects in the Bacon study were administered a meter recognition test which consisted of unfamiliar songs that were performed in duple and triple meter. In the present study, the meter identification test consists of rhythm patterns chanted on a neutral syllable. The subjects in the present study will be expected to apply the syllables they learned during the treatment period to familiar and unfamiliar rhythm patterns. A one-way ANOVA was used in the Bacon study to compare the treatment with the subjects' post-test score on the meter recognition test. In the present study, a 3 x 3 twoway ANOVA will be used to compare treatment and post-test scores but also examine interaction effects between music aptitude and treatment. 11

Chapter Three Design of Study Sample The sample for the current study consisted of 107 second and third grade students from general music classes in the Cherry Hill Public Schools located in Southern New Jersey. The subjects included in the study came from diverse ethnic and social backgrounds. Procedures The sample included three classes of third grade students and three classes of second grade students, all of whom had been taught general music by the researcher during the school year. For three months prior to the start of the 14-week treatment period, all of the classes were taught the duple and triple rhythm patterns included in this study (Appendix A) on the neutral syllable bah in their weekly music class. Prior to the start of the study, the researcher sent a letter to the following people in the Cherry Hill Public Schools requesting permission to perform the research on the subjects: the principals of Johnson and Sharp Elementary Schools, the district's Supervisor of the Arts, and the Assistant Superintendent of Instruction and Professional Development (Appendix B). The subjects' general music classes were kept intact and randomly assigned to the control group (n = 36), the time-based treatment group (n = 35), or the function-based 12

treatment group (n = 36). All classes were administered the Intermediate Measures of Music Auditation (Gordon, 1982) to determine the subject's level of rhythmic aptitude. Subjects were designated as having a high, average or low music aptitude based on the norms listed in the test manual. Because differences in students' rhythm performances have been found based on piano experience (Kuhlman, 1996), a researcher-designed questionnaire (Appendix C) was also administered to all subjects for the purpose of obtaining information which may impact subjects' rhythm achievement. Duple and triple meter rhythm patterns selected for use in the study are from Jump Right In: the instrumental series (Grunow, Gordon & Azzara, 2000). According to the authors, these patterns were chosen because they are found in the majority of folk song literature commonly used in general music classes. The patterns were taught to all groups during the first ten minutes of each music class. The remainder of the class was spent on regular music classroom activities mandated by the school music curriculum. The teaching process of the rhythm pattern was identical for all groups with the exception of the system of rhythm syllable used. The researcher aurally presented a pattern using the neutral syllable bah to all groups and students echoed the pattern back. During the echo process, students were randomly selected to provide solo performances. The purpose of the solo performances was to provide a way for the researcher to aurally evaluate the student performance and familiarity of the patterns. After a pattern became familiar to the subjects and they were able to perform it consistently using bah, the researcher aurally taught the pattern using rhythm syllables. The time-based treatment group used Kodaly time-based rhythm syllables. The function-based treatment group used Gordon's function-based rhythm syllables. The control group continued to learn the patterns on the neutral syllable bah for the remainder of the treatment period. Both duple 13

and triple rhythm patterns were taught in each class period using a metronome set to 86 beats per minute to provide a steady pulse during the rhythm pattern training. Starting during week four of the treatment process, the researcher occasionally presented a pattern on bah and asked the students to perform the pattern with the proper rhythm syllables. In order to prepare students for the testing situation, the researcher periodically chanted a pattern using bah and asked students to identify if the pattern was in duple or triple meter. At the conclusion of the treatment period, the researcher administered a self-designed Meter Identification Test (MIT, Appendix D). The test consisted of a CD recording of the investigator chanting duple and triple rhythm patterns. A metronome set at 84 beats per minute was used in the recording of the MIT to provide a steady pulse during the performance of the patterns. Twenty-four patterns were included in the test: 16 of the patterns were familiar and taught during the study, the remaining eight were unfamiliar. Students were expected to recognize the sound of the patterns and encode the syllables learned during the treatment process to determine if the pattern was in duple or triple meter. The students recorded answers by circling the correct word, 'duple', 'triple' or '?' after listening to each pattern. The "?" was used to discourage the subjects from guessing "duple" or "triple" on the patterns that they could not identify as duple or triple meter with certainty. Analysis A 3 x 3 two-way ANOVA was used to investigate differences among the control group, timed based treatment, and function-based treatment group on the subjects' abilities to identify meter (Best & Kahn, 2003). It also identified interaction effects between treatment and levels of rhythm aptitude. 14

Chapter Four Results and Interpretations The 24-item Meter Identification Test yielded the following results as shown in Table 1. The mean score for the control group was 14.138. The mean score for the function-based treatment group was 16.111, and the mean score for the time-based treatment group was 13.628. Analysis of Variance for Meter Identification Test Table 1 Mean scores of MIT by treatment group Treatment n Mean SD Function-based High 13 17.153 5.225 Medium 19 15.315 2.849 Low 4 16.500 1.914 Total 36 16.111 3.823 Time-based High 2 18.000 2.828 Medium 25 13.480 3.583 Low 8 13.000 3.251 Total 35 13.628 3.565 Control High 3 14.000 3.000 Medium 16 13.312 3.419 Low 17 14.941 4.099 Total 36 14.138 3.719 15

Results from the Student Questionnaire revealed that an almost equal number of subject's with piano experience comprised each group: function-based group (n = 7); time-based group (n = 8); control group (n = 6). Thus, possible confounding effects due to piano experience were internally controlled; therefore piano experience was not considered in further analyses. A 3 x 3 two-way ANOVA was used to investigate differences among the control group, timed-based treatment, and function-based treatment group on the subjects' abilities to identify meter. Results of the analysis of variance revealed no significant differences among treatment groups at the.05 level of significance (Table 2). Results also revealed that there was no significant interaction between level of music aptitude and treatment. Table 2 3 x 3 Analysis of variance of MIT scores by treatment group and rhythmic aptitude Source of Sum of Variance dc' Square Mean F p APTITUDE 2 49.609 24.805 1.818.168 TREATMENT 2 56.426 28.213 2.067.132 APTITUDE * TREATMENT 4 39.672 9.918.727.576 Error 98 1337.416 13.647 Total 107 24470.000 16

Interpretations The problem of this study was to examine the effect of different syllable systems on students' meter discrimination abilities, specifically examining the time-based and function-based syllable systems. Results of the study revealed no difference in abilities of subjects who received rhythm instruction that utilized the function-based syllable system and those who used a time-based syllable system. Furthermore, subjects who used either syllable system failed to demonstrate greater meter identification abilities than students who used no syllable system as part of rhythm instruction. 17

Chapter Five Summary and Conclusions Purpose and Problems The purpose of this study was to investigate the efficacy of rhythm syllable systems in developing students' aural discrimination abilities. The problem was to examine the effect of rhythm syllable systems on students' abilities to aurally identify meter of rhythm patterns. A secondary problem was to examine the interaction effects of music aptitude and rhythm syllable systems. Design The sample for this study included 107 students in second and third grade general music classes. Classes remained intact and were randomly assigned to the control group, the time-based syllable group and the function-based syllable group. Each group consisted of one second grade and one third grade class and included at least 35 subjects. All groups received music instruction from the researcher for 30 minutes once a week. Subjects were administered the IMMA prior to the start of the treatment period to identify level of music aptitude and a student questionnaire to identify prior rhythmic activity experience. All students received rhythm pattern training on the neutral syllable bah before the treatment period. During the treatment period, the students learned the same rhythm patterns: instruction differed only in the type of rhythm syllable system used. The time-based treatment group used Kodaly rhythm syllables, the function-based 18

treatment group used Gordon syllables, and the control group continued to use a neutral syllable. A metronome was used in the pattern teaching process to provide a steady pulse for the subjects. The rhythm patterns were taught during the first 10 minutes of each music class, the remainder of the class was used for regular classroom music activities. At the conclusion of the 14 week treatment period, all subjects were administered the MIT. The researcher designed the MIT which included both unfamiliar patterns and familiar patterns learned during the treatment process. The subjects were required to circle 'duple', 'triple' or '?' on the MIT answer sheet. After data was compiled from the subjects' test scores, a 3 x 3 two-way ANOVA was used to investigate differences among groups. It also identified interaction effects between treatment and level of subjects' rhythm aptitude. Results The ANOVA revealed that there were no significant differences among groups with regard to treatment. The ANOVA also revealed that there were no interaction effects between treatment and level of rhythm aptitude. Conclusions Based on data acquired from the present study, it may be concluded that choice of rhythm syllable system has no effect on students' abilities to identify meter. The findings in the present study do not support Bluestine's statement that a syllable system is necessary in order to learn rhythm patterns efficiently and effectively; however, it is 19

important to note that the 14-week study may not have provided subjects with enough time to use the syllables to their full potential. Analysis of the mean scores did reveal that subjects in the function-based treatment group to have scored higher on the Meter Identification Test with a mean score of 16.11. Both of the other groups scored lower: the time-based group mean was 13.62 and the control group mean was 14.13. Although not statistically significant, the higher mean scores of the function-based treatment group provides some evidence that use of a function-based rhythm syllable system may contribute to the development of students' abilities to aurally identify meter to a greater extent than a time-based syllable system or no syllable system. The results of the study may have been affected by the inequity of students with high, medium and low rhythm aptitude in each of the three groups. It was assumed that subjects in this study would represent a normal distribution of rhythm aptitude; however, the function-based group had 13 high, 19 medium and 4 low rhythm aptitude subjects. The time-based group had 2 high, 25 medium and 8 low rhythm aptitude subjects, whereas the control group consisted of 3 high, 16 medium and 17 low aptitude subjects. In the Palmer study, data was subjected to an ANCOVA with rhythm aptitude serving as a covariate. The use of an ANCOVA in the present study might have been useful in controlling the inequity of rhythm aptitude levels which comprised treatment groups. In conclusion, the researcher recommends that future designs of this study include a longer treatment period. The full extent rhythm syllable system training may be apparent if students are given more time to use the syllables in rhythm pattern training than this study allowed. 20

Appendix A 21

Duple Patterns 1 1.4 -I- li- I-rnl Tl Ti De Ta Dnil Ta nni.2 i n,n -. I - J.4 Ti Ti T i nl nde Dn ne n.2 R -.4 5. - 6.1 Tn Di Ti T i Ta Dlu e rn,, Ti Dii Ta ndii Ti De Ti fl Ti ne Ta Di Ti Al Ti Ti Ta Dii De fill i fle Ti Ti Ti Ti Dil 1 e nflu De T! DNu mmm Ti T Tl Tl Ti Ti Dlil e Du ne Dnl ne j I A 2r7 J- im 7 I s. I w * * * 1- w. - - i 1 - - I- - Ka Ti Tl Dni Ti ne Dii i KaTi TI Ti ndii Ti ne Dli De j 77 n7,, 2 n n,,,t j I Ti nil - I- - -I,, -..- 4 3 1 I J- Ti Ti Ti Ti Dii ne nii ne Ti nil Ti ne ---------- 0 1 Ta Dnii Tl Dii Tl Ti Ti Ti Ta Dn ne Dni De Di1 I,.2 7 F- n r-,j i ~1 n'q,, Ij Ti K i Ti Ti Ti K Ti Ki T Du Ta De Di D e Du Tl De Ta Du Ti K TIi K T Du Ti De Tl Du TI Ni m77 -I Ti nil Ti Oe 111. if w w w w if w f f. i n7 J In7] j, IJf J If ni Ti Kl Ti Ki Ti Ti Du Tl Ne Ti DI De,,,n H~] n Ti Ti Ti Ti Ku Ti K Ti Ti Ti Du De Du De T Du Tl De Di De

1. 8 7 m i.,1 i - V p s - *- UI r Tl Ti Ti Ta Ti Ti Tl Ta Di Dia Di iiil Dna Di Dn Triple Patterns 7.m m i ^ Ti Ti Ti Ti Ti Ti Ta Ti 1Ti Ti TDi Di Ni Di DTi Ti Ti Ti Di Ta Ti Ti Ti Ti Ti Ti Ta D ndi D i Di Dii Da Di Dii Dnn Din D i Dii DIa Di Dii Di ni 3.8 2i 11 2 1 Ti Ti Ti i T Ti Ti i Ti Ti Ti Ti Ti Ta Dii Da Di ndil Da i ndi D Di Di 1 Ka 21 2 2 Kas 2t i 1 2 Ti 2 21 DiAi TR DI Di Di T D DiA D Ta Di Di Ii Dia Di Ti Ti Ti Ta T i Ti Ti T Ti Dil na Dii i Di D i Dii Da Di T2 Ti Tl Ta Ti Ti 21 Dii Di DR Di Dii DII Da Di 10, Ti 1 KA1 Ti Ti Ti i Ka 1i Ti T KaTi Ti Ti Dii Da ni TR ndi lh ni TR ndi Da ni Ta ni nla ni 1Lu njnj 21 K Ti Ka.T Ti Ka Ta ndii Ta nd ni T ndii i n J Ti Ka Ti Ti Ks Ti T1 Ti Dnl Ti Di Di Ta Dii Di a Ti Ti 11 Ta Ta Ti Ti Ti nl Da i N Dn DI Dil Da Di 12.1 _i j I, -... Ti Ka Ti Ks Ti K Ta Dii Ta Da Ta Di Ta Dii I fm -I Tl Ks Ti Kl T KaTi Ti Ti Di Ta DI Ta Di Ta DiT Da ni

Appendix B 24

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550 rbeckman@chclc.org Mrs. Betsy Holschuh Principal Johnson Elementary School Kresson Road Cherry Hill, NJ 08034 January 6, 2003 Dear Mrs. Holschuh, I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Master's Thesis in your school. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the Second and Third grade classes I presently have in my schedule. The research for the thesis will take place during a sixteen week period starting the week of January 6 th and ending the week of April 28th. The research will involve providing instruction with two different types of rhythm solfege both of which are congruent to the Cherry Hill Music Standards. All students in the second and third grade classes will take a written pre-assessment prior to the study and a post-assessment at the end of the study. Instruction for both the control and treatment groups will be the same accept for the rhythm syllables. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation, please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance for your consideration to my request. Sincerely, Richard Beckman

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550 rbeckman(chclc. org Mr. Robert Homer Principal Sharp Elementary School Old Orchard Road Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 January 6, 2003 Dear Mr. Homer, I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Master's Thesis in your school. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the Second grade class I presently have in my schedule. The research for the thesis will take place during a sixteen week period starting the week of January 6th. The research will involve providing instruction with two different types of rhythm solfege both of which are congruent to the Cherry Hill Music Standards. All students in the second grade class will take a written pre-assessment prior to the study and a post-assessment at the end of the study. Instruction for both the control and treatment groups will be the same accept for the rhythm syllables. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation, please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance for your consideration to my request. Sincerely, Richard Beckman

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550 rbeckman@chclc.org Mr. Joseph Akinskas Assistant Principal & Supervisor of the Arts Cherry Hill High School West 2101 Chapel Avenue Cherry Hill, New Jersey 08034 January 6, 2003 Dear Mr. Akinskas, I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Master's Thesis in Johnson and Sharp Elementary schools. I am currently a music teacher in both buildings and will be the only one participating in the administration of the research study. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the Second and Third grade classes I presently have in my schedule. The research for the thesis will take place during a sixteen-week period starting in the spring semester of 2003. The research will involve providing instruction with two different types of rhythm solfege, both of which are congruent to the Cherry Hill and State Standards for music. All students involved will take a written pre-assessment prior to the study and a post-assessment at the end of the study. Instruction for both the control and treatment groups will be the same except for the use of different rhythm syllables. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation, please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance for your consideration of my request. Sincerely, Richard Beckman

Richard Beckman Sharp Elementary School Cherry Hill, NJ 08003 (856) 424-1550 rbeckman@chclc.org Ms. Tammy Murphy Assistant Superintendent Instruction and Professional Development Cherry Hill Public Schools 45 Ranoldo Terrace Cherry Hill, New Jersey 08034 Dear Ms. Murphy, January 6, 2003 I am writing to request permission to conduct the research for my Master's Thesis in Johnson and Sharp Elementary schools. I am currently a music teacher in both buildings and will be the only one participating in the administration of the research study. Specifically, I would like permission to work with the Second and Third grade classes I presently have in my schedule. The research for the thesis will take place during a sixteen-week period starting in the spring semester of 2003. The research will involve providing instruction with two different types of rhythm solfege, both of which are congruent to the Cherry Hill and State Standards for music. All students involved will take a written pre-assessment prior to the study and a post-assessment at the end of the study. Instruction for both the control and treatment groups will be the same except for the use of different rhythm syllables. If you have any questions, concerns or require further explanation, please do not hesitate to contact me at the above address. I would like to thank you in advance for your consideration of my request. Sincerely, Richard Beckman

Appendix C 29

Student Questionnaire Please read each question and statement carefully. Circle YES or NO to indicate if the statement does or does not describe you. Name Teacher A.ge GCrade 1. I have taken piano lessons for at least one year. YES NO 2. I am taking piano lessons this year. YES NO 3. I have played a musical instrument for at least one year. (not including the recorder) YES NO 4. I am playing an instrument this year. (not including the recorder) YES NO 5. I have taken dance lessons for at least one year. YES NO 6. I am taking dance lessons this year. YES NO

Appendix D 31

Name: Meter Identification Test Teacher: Date: Practice Question 1 METER IN 2 Practice Question 2 METER IN 2 1. METER IN 2 2. METER IN 2 3. METER IN 2 4. METER IN 2 5. METER IN 2 6. METER IN 2 7. METER IN 2 8. METER IN 2 9. METER IN 2 10. METER IN 2

11. METER IN 2 12. METER IN 2 13. METER IN 2 14. METER IN 2 15. METER IN 2 16. METER IN 2 17. METER IN 2 18. METER IN 2 19. METER IN 2 20. METER IN 2 21. METER IN 2 22. METER IN 2 23. METER IN 2 24. METER IN 2

Patterns included in the Meter Identification Test Practice 1 Pracctice 2 1 fi -m -7mi. -11- S ''' -- ' in 1 -J m J- 4 - I- J- 11 13 S I i - II - - Ii - il I- _. II -j m i J. j. II 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 gj J. 7] J. -77 J-T i-j 4 I.. 16 4 II 16 J J-3 J J 17 8'n -J J 11 1 g8 I.. 11 19. I 77111. J. 20-1 21 4 In 22 4 23 24 J - -n- I -. nj n 13rn

Bibliography Bacon, T. (1998). A comparison of rhythm syllable systems used in beginning instrumental instruction. (Master's thesis, Michigan State University, 1998). Digital Dissertations, 36. Bebeau, Muriel J. (1982). Effects of traditional and simplified methods of rhythmreading instruction. Journal of Research in Music Education, 30(2), 107-119. Best, J. & Kahn, J. (2003). Research in Education. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Beethoven, J., Davidson, J. & Nadon-Gabrion, C. (1988). World of Music. Morristown, NJ: Silver Burdett & Ginn Inc. Blustine, E. (2000). The Ways Children Learn Music: An introduction and practical guide to music learning theory. Chicago: GIA Publications, Inc. Campbell, P. S. and Scott-Kassner, C. (1995). Music in Childhood: From preschool through the elementary grades. New York: Schirmer Books. Gordon, E. E. (1982). Intermediate Measures of Music Audiation. Chicago: GIA Publications, Inc. Gordon, E. E. (1997). Learning Sequences in Music: Skills, content, and Patterns. Chicago: GIA Publications, Inc. Gordon, E. E. (2000). Rhythm: Contrasting the implications of audiation and notation. Chicago: GIA Publications, Inc. Grunow, F., Gordon, E., and Azzara, C. (2000). Jump Right In: The instrumental series. Chicago: GIA Publications, Inc. 35

Kuhlman, K. (1996). The effects of movement-based instruction, meter, and rhythmic aptitude on beginning instrumental music students' abilities to communicate metric structure in performance. (Doctoral dissertation, The University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 1996). Mark, M. (1996). Contemporary Music Education. New York: Schirmer Books. Music Educators National Conference. (1994). The school music program: A new vision: The K-12 national standards, pre-k standards, and what they mean to music educators. Reston, VA: MENC. Palmer, M. (1976). Relative effectiveness of two approaches to rhythm reading for fourth-grade students. Journal of Research in Music Education, 24(3), 110-118. Schleuter, Stanley L. (1997). A Sound Approach to Teaching Instrumentalists. Ohio: Kent State University Press. 36