GENERAL ARTICLE. The Brain on Music. Nandini Chatterjee Singh and Hymavathy Balasubramanian

Similar documents
What is music as a cognitive ability?

The Healing Power of Music. Scientific American Mind William Forde Thompson and Gottfried Schlaug

Music Training and Neuroplasticity

Dimensions of Music *

BIBB 060: Music and the Brain Tuesday, 1:30-4:30 Room 117 Lynch Lead vocals: Mike Kaplan

Quantifying Tone Deafness in the General Population

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Stewart, Lauren and Walsh, Vincent (2001) Neuropsychology: music of the hemispheres Dispatch, Current Biology Vol.11 No.

Therapeutic Function of Music Plan Worksheet

The relationship between properties of music and elicited emotions

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

August Acoustics and Psychoacoustics Barbara Crowe Music Therapy Director. Notes from BC s copyrighted materials for IHTP

Tuning the Brain: Neuromodulation as a Possible Panacea for treating non-pulsatile tinnitus?

University of Groningen. Tinnitus Bartels, Hilke

Tinnitus: The Neurophysiological Model and Therapeutic Sound. Background

Expressive information

Susanne Langer fight or flight. arousal level valence. parasympathetic nervous. system. roughness

Music Perception & Cognition

Abnormal Electrical Brain Responses to Pitch in Congenital Amusia Isabelle Peretz, PhD, 1 Elvira Brattico, MA, 2 and Mari Tervaniemi, PhD 2

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

DOI: / ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Evaluation protocol for amusia - portuguese sample

Compose yourself: The Emotional Influence of Music

The power of music in children s development

Pitch and Timing Abilities in Inherited Speech and Language Impairment

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

Trauma & Treatment: Neurologic Music Therapy and Functional Brain Changes. Suzanne Oliver, MT-BC, NMT Fellow Ezequiel Bautista, MT-BC, NMT

Music HEAD IN YOUR. By Eckart O. Altenmüller

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

Music and the emotions

MUSICAL EAR TRAINING THROUGH ACTIVE MUSIC MAKING IN ADOLESCENT Cl USERS. The background ~

From "Hopeless" to "Healed"

DEMENTIA CARE CONFERENCE 2014

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

Chapter Five: The Elements of Music

doi: /brain/awp345 Brain 2010: 133; The cognitive organization of music knowledge: a clinical analysis

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

Pitch Perception. Roger Shepard

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

The purpose of this essay is to impart a basic vocabulary that you and your fellow

What Can Experiments Reveal About the Origins of Music? Josh H. McDermott

SYMPOSIA: MUSICAL TRAINING FOR CHILDREN

OVER THE YEARS, PARTICULARLY IN THE PAST

The Effects of Stimulative vs. Sedative Music on Reaction Time

Musical Illusions Diana Deutsch Department of Psychology University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Lutz Jäncke. Minireview

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY

Dr Kelly Jakubowski Music Psychologist October 2017

Population codes representing musical timbre for high-level fmri categorization of music genres

Brain.fm Theory & Process

Creative Computing II

WORKING MEMORY AND MUSIC PERCEPTION AND PRODUCTION IN AN ADULT SAMPLE. Keara Gillis. Department of Psychology. Submitted in Partial Fulfilment

Psychology PSY 312 BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR. (3)

The laughing brain - Do only humans laugh?

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

Perception of emotion in music in adults with cochlear implants

Analysis on the Value of Inner Music Hearing for Cultivation of Piano Learning

Making Connections Through Music

12/7/2018 E-1 1

The Context Quarterly e journal of language, literary and cultural studies

Modeling Melodic Perception as Relational Learning Using a Symbolic- Connectionist Architecture (DORA)

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Affective Priming. Music 451A Final Project

Structural and functional neuroplasticity of tinnitus-related distress and duration

PRESCOTT UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT District Instructional Guide January 2016

Perceptual Considerations in Designing and Fitting Hearing Aids for Music Published on Friday, 14 March :01

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Improving Piano Sight-Reading Skills of College Student. Chian yi Ang. Penn State University

This Is Your Brain On Music. BIA-MA Brain Injury Conference March 30, 2017 Eve D. Montague, MSM, MT-BC

PHI 3240: Philosophy of Art

Perceiving Differences and Similarities in Music: Melodic Categorization During the First Years of Life

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Physicians Hearing Services Welcomes You!

Music Curriculum. Rationale. Grades 1 8

COPYRIGHT 2004 SCIENTIFIC AMERICAN, INC.

Curriculum Mapping Subject-VOCAL JAZZ (L)4184

Composing and Interpreting Music

MUSIC COURSE OF STUDY GRADES K-5 GRADE

The Neural Mechanisms of Tinnitus and Tinnitus Distress

Melody: sequences of pitches unfolding in time. HST 725 Lecture 12 Music Perception & Cognition

The Beat Alignment Test (BAT): Surveying beat processing abilities in the general population

K-12 Performing Arts - Music Standards Lincoln Community School Sources: ArtsEdge - National Standards for Arts Education

Involved brain areas in processing of Persian classical music: an fmri study

The Power of Listening

聲音有高度嗎? 音高之聽覺生理基礎. Do Sounds Have a Height? Physiological Basis for the Pitch Percept

Automatic Music Clustering using Audio Attributes

Impaired perception of harmonic complexity in congenital amusia: A case study

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Our Perceptions of Music: Why Does the Theme from Jaws Sound Like a Big Scary Shark?

Affective response to a set of new musical stimuli W. Trey Hill & Jack A. Palmer Psychological Reports, 106,

A NON LINEAR APPROACH TOWARDS AUTOMATED EMOTION ANALYSIS IN HINDUSTANI MUSIC

Music and the brain: disorders of musical listening

EMPLOYMENT SERVICE. Professional Service Editorial Board Journal of Audiology & Otology. Journal of Music and Human Behavior

MHSIB.5 Composing and arranging music within specified guidelines a. Creates music incorporating expressive elements.

The Neural Code of Pitch and Harmony

Transcription:

The Brain on Music Nandini Chatterjee Singh and Hymavathy Balasubramanian Permeating across societies and cultures, music is a companion to millions across the globe. Despite being an abstract art form, music affects all of us deeply, encouraging us in periods of difficulty, comforting us in moments of sadness and celebrating with us in times of joy. In recent years, neuroscientists have recognized that listening to and producing music involves a tantalizing mix of practically every human cognitive function. Thus, music has emerged as an invaluable tool to study varying aspects of the human brain such as auditory and motor perception and learning, attention, memory, and emotion. In this paper, we will discuss the different attributes of music and the neural structures associated with them. We will also highlight some recent work in our laboratory at the National Brain Research Centre in India on emotions associated with Hindustani rāga music. Dr. Nandini Chatterjee Singh is a cognitive neuroscientist at thenationalbrainresearch Centre, Manesar. Research in her laboratory is directed at studying brain circuits involved in language processing, reading and music. Perception of Music and Amusia In a paper written in 1951, philosopher Susanne Langer noted, The most highly developed type of purely connotational semantic is music," [1] thereby suggesting that meaning or communication through music came to us long before meaning given by words. This led to the proposition that along the evolutionary road, the articulation of emotions possibly began through other sensory modalities and included music and gestures. In the past two decades, a number of studies have been undertaken to understand the perception of music by the brain. Thus, research has revealed that music provides a tool to study not only emotion processing but also many other processes like sound processing, motor-skill learning, attention, memory storage and retrieval and sensory-motor integration. Hymavathy is a project assistant under Dr. Singh at NBRC, Manesar. Her research interest lies in understanding neural processing underlying music perception. Her work focus on studying the statistical features of Hindustani music rāgas and understanding the effects of music training on auditory perception Keywords Music, brain, neuroscience, amusia, fmri, rāga, emotion. RESONANCE March 2018 299

Figure 1. The pathway from the ear to the cortex during music perception depicted on the coronal section of the brain. Sound signals are converted to neural impulses at the cochlear level and move up to the primary auditory cortex for music information decoding. From the auditory cortex, we see strong connections with associated regions of motor, premotor regions. (Coronal section image credits: Patrick J Lynch, Medical Illustrator; C Carl Jaffe, MD, Cardiologist) Left Auditory cortex (Spectral processing : Pitch) Thalamus Primary motor cortex (Rhythm perception) Right Auditory cortex (Temporal aspect of music) Midbrain Cochlear nuclues The initial perception of music, similar to other sound stimuli, starts in the cochlea, in the inner ear, where acoustic information is translated into neural activity. This neural activity is then translated into distinct music features (attributes) such as pitch, timbre, roughness, and intensity in the midbrain. Moving up to the thalamus, the relay center for sensory stimuli, this information is then directed to the auditory cortex of the brain. Following this, music processes are divided, with the left auditory cortex dominating spectral processes like pitch and the right auditory cortex governing temporal aspects like beat, rhythm and tempo. The temporal aspects of music have been shown to involve a strong interaction between the auditory and motor cortices. Additionally the processing of temporal information in music is very similar across perception and production. This means that the brain responds very similarly when the rhythm is being perceived (auditory) and when it is being produced (motor). Thus, an interesting feature of music processing is its intercortical nature since most processes involve a strong interaction between two or more cortices. Figure 1 shows the different structures and pathways in the brain known to be involved in music processing. How has music processing in the brain been studied? The ear- 300 RESONANCE March 2018

liest report of the neural correlates of music came from deficits in music processing. In 1878, Grant-Allen reported the case of a 30-year-old educated man without any brain lesions suffering from a severe musical handicap. The man was unable to discriminate the pitch of two successive tones, failed to recognize familiar melodies and could not carry a tune. This condition went on to be termed as amusia. Amusia is impaired perception, understanding, or production of music not attributable to disease of the peripheral (external to the brain, in this case the ear) auditory system or motor system. Individuals with amusia are unable to recognize wrong notes or even familiar melodies. Amusia is impaired perception, understanding, or production of music not attributable to disease of the peripheral (external to the brain, in this case the ear) auditory system or motor system. Individuals with amusia are unable to recognize wrong notes or even familiar melodies. The disorder has further been categorized into two types congenital and acquired amusia. Congenital amusia, also known as tone-deafness, is a musical disorder that is inherited whereas acquired amusia occurs as a consequence of brain damage. People suffering from congenital amusia lack basic musical abilities that include melodic discrimination and recognition. This disorder cannot be explained by prior brain lesion, hearing loss, cognitive deficits, socio-affective disturbance, or lack of environmental stimulation. Individuals suffering from congenital amusia often only have impaired musical abilities but are able to process speech, common environmental sounds and human voices similar to typical individuals. This suggested that music is biological i.e., it is innately present in humans. Although there are no cures for congenital amusia, some treatments have been found to be effective in improving the musical abilities of those suffering from congenital amusia. In one such study, singing intervention was shown to improve the perception of music in amusic individuals, and it is hoped that more methods will be discovered that may help people overcome congenital amusia. Acquired amusia is a cognitive disorder of perception and/or processing of music and is found in patients with brain damage. Acquired amusia has been observed in two distinct forms, namely anhedonia and auditoryagnosia, basedonthedifferent deficits in music processing. Anhedonia is a disorder that leads to inability of experiencing pleasure while listening to music. Thus, Acquired amusia is a cognitive disorder of perception and/or processing of music and is found in patients with brain damage. RESONANCE March 2018 301

individuals with this disorder have difficulty experiencing or recognizing emotion in music. On the other hand, individuals with associative agnosia generally fail to recognize the source of the sound. Despite being taught, they might fail to identify the instrument producing the sound or may have trouble naming the tune played. 1 A brain area that is part of the limbic system. Recognition of emotion in music is not restricted to a single region but is distributed over a network of regions that also include the fronto-temporal lobes. Brain damage resulting in acquired amusia may arise from strokes, diseases, tumors etc. Consequently, there are a large number of cases of acquired amusia, all implicating different brain regions. Here, it is important to note that a considerable understanding of how the brain processes music has emerged because of loss of function. For example, a rare subject with complete bilateral damage in the amygdala 1 was found to be significantly impaired only in musical emotion processing. This finding indicates that the role of amygdala in music processing is crucial for recognizing emotion in music. Further research on the amygdala then went on to establish its role in emotion processing in general. Similarly, in another study, musical emotion was affected in 26 patients with fronto-temporal lobar degeneration. As the name indicates, fronto-temporal lobar degeneration is associated with a progressive decay in the nerves of the frontal and temporal regions of the brain. This illustrated that recognition of emotion in music is not restricted to a single region namely the amygdala but is distributed over a network of regions that also include the fronto-temporal lobes. Over the years, research has shown that musical emotion is processed by a large distributed network of regions that includes insula, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex, anterior temporal, posterior temporal and parietal cortices, amygdala, and the subcortical mesolimbic system (see Figure 2 for regions involved in emotion network). Therefore, it is fair to say that music processing is distributed all over the brain. Music and Emotion Despite the knowledge that the primary function of music is to communicate emotion, scientific studies investigating the impact 302 RESONANCE March 2018

Anterior cingulate cortex (Emotion regulation) Medial prefrontal cortex (Recognition) Orbitofrontal cortex (Recognition) Amygdala (Emotion recognition) Primary motor cortex (Rhythm perception) Auditory cortex (Spectral and temporal processing) Medial geniculate nucleus (Relay center) Inferior colliculus (Sound compression) of music on emotion are relatively recent. In order to understand and systematically assess the emotional response of participants to music, psychological studies used emotion rating. In order to quantify the extent of emotion experienced, emotion studies used self-reports, where participants are required to rate the emotion experienced from a given set of emotion cues while listening to that music sample. To understand or experience this, visit http://emotion-in-music.nbrc.ac.in/p1/. In order to establish the neurobiological evidence for emotions experienced by the brain while listening to music, research studies have used techniques like functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fmri), which measures brain activity by detecting changes associated with blood flow. It is based on the understanding that when an area of the brain is in use, blood flow to that region also increases. In fmri studies, brain activity is measured while individuals listen to music in the MRI scanner, and make responses on how much they liked or disliked the music. Such studies fall under the realm of psychophysiology as they link psychological processes like emotion perception with physiological brain responses. Figure 2. Sagittal section of the brain depicting the lobes and regions involved during music processing. The following lobes are depicted in different colors: frontal lobe (blue), temporal lobe (yellow), parietal lobe (orange), and limbic lobe (green). Sound processed at the cochlear level reaches the thalamic level (medial geniculate nucleus), which projects it to the primary auditory cortex. From the auditory cortex, we see interactions with the primary motor cortex for rhythm perception, the orbitofrontal cortex for recognition, amygdala, cingulate, and limbic system for emotion recognition. (Sagittal section image credits: Patrick J Lynch, Medical Illustrator; C Carl Jaffe, MD, Cardiologist.) In addition to the emotional impact of music on the brain, these studies have established that listening to music can be linked to changes in the hormonal system of the body mediated by the brain. Music has been shown to have a positive effect on the mood of an individual and listening to music has been linked to RESONANCE March 2018 303

The expression of happiness is associated with faster tempo, a high-pitch range, and a major rather than minor interval, and these cues are reversed in musical expressions of sadness. the release of the pleasure hormone dopamine. Furthermore, performing musical activities has been suggested to strengthen social bonds such as coordination, cooperation, communication, social cognition, contact, copathy, and social cohesion in a group. Since music is a spectrally rich, time varying auditory stimulus, a large body of research has focused on establishing the association between the musical structure and emotions perceived by listeners. Research from the field of music psychology has indicated that emotions are elicited as a consequence of the musical structure. Musical structure is characterized by acoustic features that can be broadly classified into tonal (intensity, pitch height, timbre, dissonance, harmony) and rhythmic (rhythmic complexity, tempo) cues, which blend together to create a seamless perception of pitch in the musical composition. Some of the earliest research conducted in the field of music psychology manipulated these cues of musical structure to identify the change in participants response along the happy-sad continuum axis. These research studies used musical pieces that systematically varied in several cues and the results of these studies indicated that mode, tempo, and rhythm are the three cues that are the strongest determinants of the perceived emotion in listeners. The expression of happiness is associated with faster tempo, a high-pitch range, and a major rather than minor interval (see Box 1 for definition), and these cues are reversed in musical expressions of sadness. A Case for Hindustani Music Most of our current understanding of musical emotions and its association with musical structure has come from studies conducted using western music. Although past studies using western music have established the association of musical mode (minor/major) with mood (negative/positive), and tempo with intensity of the emotion response, most findings lack generalizability across multicultural representations of music. Consequently, while music and emotion studies have standardized the use of Western classical music as a source of stimuli, only a few studies have incor- 304 RESONANCE March 2018

porated genres of music native to other cultures. This not only precludes any interpretations of universality in musical emotions from their findings, it also overlooks an entire canon of musical stimuli, which might be uniquely suited to disentangling different acoustic effects on musical emotion. Classical music from the subcontinent of India, exists in two forms Carnatic and Hindustani. Hindustani music, popularly known as North Indian Classical Music (NICM), born out of cultural synthesis of Vedic chant tradition and traditional Persian music, has been historically associated with emotional (rasa) communication. Thus, by its innate design of tonal structure, Indian Classical Music lends itself rather appropriately to investigate some existing questions and raise new ones for cross-cultural studies of musical emotion. Rāga Rasa Theory in the Context of Music Emotion Research As stated earlier, Indian classical music is principally based on melody and rhythm, rather than harmony, counterpoint, chords, modulation (see Box 1 for definitions) and the other basics of Western classical music. Rāgas form the central notion in both Hindustani and Carnatic music. The word rāga originates in Sanskrit and is described as the act of coloring or dyeing (the mind and mood/emotions in this context) and refers metaphorically to any feeling or passion especially love, affection, sympathy, desire, interest, motivation, joy, or delight. The notion of a rasa (mood) is thus inherent in both Hindustani and Carnatic music. A rāga uses a set of five or more notes from the fixed scale of seven notes to construct a melody. However, it is not enough to define a rāga in terms of mode or scale alone, as a number of rāgas have the same notes, yet each maintains its own musical identity. For instance, both rāgas Miyan ki Malhar and Bahar contain the same notes (Sa, Re, ga, Ma, Pa, Dha, ni, Ni) and yet sound quite different because of the way the notes in the scale are approached and combined. The word rāga originates in Sanskrit and is described as the act of coloring or dyeing (the mind and mood/emotions in this context) and refers metaphorically to any feeling or passion especially love, affection, sympathy, desire, interest, motivation, joy, or delight. A rāga composition is typically presented as a specific sequence RESONANCE March 2018 305

Box 1. Definitions Tonal (tonic) Intervals: A tonal interval is determined by two tones, one of which is conventionally the tonic. The tonic is the root note around which a musical piece is organized, providing a reference for each tone that is sounded during the performance. In Hindustani classical music, it is always Sa. Major and Minor Intervals: Based on the 12 tone equal temperament scale used in Western classical music, the major intervals are the natural notes namely, second, third, sixth, and seventh interval. A minor interval is one semitone less than each of the major intervals of the scale (flat positions). In terms of Hindustani music, the major intervals are the shuddh swaras while the minor intervals are the komal swaras. Melody: A melody is a linear succession of musical tones that the listener perceives as a single entity. In Hindustani music, rāga uses a set of five or six notes to construct the melody. Rhythm: Rhythm is described as the systematic patterning of sound in terms of timing and grouping leading to multiple levels of periodicity. Harmony: In Western classical music, harmony is the simultaneous occurrence of multiple frequencies and pitches (tones, notes or chords). Counterpoint: Counterpoint is the art of combining different melodic lines in a musical composition and is a characteristic element of Western classical musical practice. Chords: Harmony with three or more notes are called chords and they provide the harmonic structure or background mood of a piece of music. Intervals are the building blocks of chords. Modulation: Modulation, is the change from one key to another; also, the process by which this change is brought about. of events, namely the alaap followedbythegat. Alaap is the note-by-note delineation of a rāga bound by a slow tempo, but not bound by any rhythmic cycle. Gat is the composition rendered at a faster tempo with accompaniment of a percussion instrument that provides a rhythmic cycle. The rhythmic cycle is measured in terms of time units or beats. Recently Mathur et al., [2] empirically showed the association between emotions and rāga. Three minute instrumental renditions of 12 ragas were played by 2 A stringed instrument. a professional musician on a sarod 2 and digitally recorded in both 306 RESONANCE March 2018

alaap and gat. An online survey was created (http://emotion -in-music.nbrc.ac.in/p1/) where participants listened to these rāga samples (randomized in order for each participant) and rated each sample with the emotion labels on a Likert scale 3 from 0 4(0 "notatallfelt"to4 "feltthemost"). The emotion labels used in the study were; happy, romantic, devotional, calm/soothed, angry, longing/yearning, tensed/restless, and sad. Data collected from 122 participants provided the experimental verification of the hypothesis that distinct emotional responses are associated with the alaap and gat of a rāga. Specifically, this study showed that rāgas evoke a wide variety of emotional responses that range from happy and calm to tensed and sad. It also demonstrated that rhythmic regularity was distinctly related to the arousal of emotion. For instance, the emotional response to rāgas (like Desh and Tilak Kamod ) shifted from calm/soothing in the slower arrhythmic alaap to happy in the faster rhythmic gat. A major finding of the study by Mathur et al., was that major and minor intervals are also indicators of positive and negative emotions respectively. Thealaap and gat use the sameset ofnotesas defined in therāga and are thus controlled in their use of tonal intervals and vary only in their rhythmic structure, with the gat having an explicit rhythm. This provided a unique opportunity to study the effect of rhythm on emotions in music. Research in our laboratory at the National Brain Research Centre is exploring the role of different musical structures in predicting the emotions in music. 3 TheLikertScaleisafive (or seven) point scale which is used to allow the individual to express how much they agree or disagree with a particular statement. In recent years, with the advancement in the field of perception of Data collected from 122 music in the brain, its application as an intervention in addressing participants provided the experimental verification mood disorders and emotion processing, especially in disorders of the hypothesis that like autism spectrum disorder has become widely popular. While distinct emotional brain imaging has provided support for this, it is crucial that any responses are associated new claim made in this area be approached with caution, be well with the alaap and gat of investigated and supported by robust research evidence before it a rāga. is implemented. We hope this brief review will provide a glimpse into the fascinating world of music and the brain and will motivate research towards understanding perceptual aspects of mu- RESONANCE March 2018 307

sic through approaches using psychology, neurobiology, and neuroimaging techniques to tap into the intricacies involved in its processing and production. Acknowledgments The authors would like to acknowledge Vitul Agarwal, Avantika Mathur, and Sakshee Singh for useful contributions to the manuscript. Suggested Reading Address for Correspondence Nandini Chatterjee Singh Hymavathy Balasubramanian National Brain Research Centre NH-8, Manesar 122 051 India. Email: nandini@nbrc.ac.in hyma.2194@gmail.com [1] S K Langer, Philosophy in a New Key, Harvard University Press, p.93, 1951. [2] A Mathur, S H Vijayakumar, B Chakrabarti, and N C Singh, Emotional Responses to Hindustani Rāga Music: The Role of Structure, Front. Psychol., Vol.6, p.513, 2015. [3] R J Zatorre, Music, the Food of Neuroscience?, Nature, Vol.434, 7031, pp.312 315, 2005. [4] G Allen, Note-deafness, Mind, Vol.10, pp.157 167, 1878. [5] J Ayotte, I Peretz, and K Hyde, Congenital Amusia: A Group Study of Adults Afflicted With a Music-specific Disorder,Brain, Vol.125, pp.238 251, 2005. [6] P Juslin and J Sloboda, Music and Emotion: Theory and Research, Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press, 2001. [7] Von Helmholtz, On the Sensations of Tone as a Physiological Basis for the Theory of Music, London: Longmans; Green, 1912. [8] S Koelsch, Brain Correlates of Music-evoked Emotions, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol.15, No.3, pp.170 180, 2014. [9] W Kaufmann, Rasa, Rāga-maalaa and Performance Times in North Indian Rāgas, Ethnomusicology, Vol.9, pp.272 291, 1965. [10] N A Jairazbhoy, The Rāgas of North Indian Music: Their Structure and Evolution, 2nd Edn. Mumbai: Popular Press Pvt. Ltd, 1995. [11] J M Valla, J A Alappatt, A Mathur, and N C Singh, Music and Emotion A Case for North Indian Classical Music, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol.8, 2115, 2017. 308 RESONANCE March 2018