Construction of a harmonic phrase

Similar documents
Analysis of local and global timing and pitch change in ordinary

The effect of exposure and expertise on timing judgments in music: Preliminary results*

Judgments of distance between trichords

Subjective evaluation of common singing skills using the rank ordering method

Harmonic Factors in the Perception of Tonal Melodies

The role of texture and musicians interpretation in understanding atonal music: Two behavioral studies

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

The information dynamics of melodic boundary detection

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

Expectancy Effects in Memory for Melodies

Sensory Versus Cognitive Components in Harmonic Priming

CHILDREN S CONCEPTUALISATION OF MUSIC

On human capability and acoustic cues for discriminating singing and speaking voices

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN MELODIC PITCH CONTENT AND RHYTHMIC PERCEPTION. Gideon Broshy, Leah Latterner and Kevin Sherwin

Modeling perceived relationships between melody, harmony, and key

CHORDAL-TONE DOUBLING AND THE ENHANCEMENT OF KEY PERCEPTION

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Chords not required: Incorporating horizontal and vertical aspects independently in a computer improvisation algorithm

University of Westminster Eprints

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

DYNAMIC MELODIC EXPECTANCY DISSERTATION. Bret J. Aarden, M.A. The Ohio State University 2003

Peak experience in music: A case study between listeners and performers

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

EXPECTATION IN MELODY: THE INFLUENCE OF CONTEXT AND LEARNING

Children s recognition of their musical performance

RHYTHM PATTERN PERCEPTION IN MUSIC

Notes on David Temperley s What s Key for Key? The Krumhansl-Schmuckler Key-Finding Algorithm Reconsidered By Carley Tanoue

On the Role of Semitone Intervals in Melodic Organization: Yearning vs. Baby Steps

Copyright 2006 The Authors. Deposited on: 09 October 2013

Children s implicit knowledge of harmony in Western music

A GTTM Analysis of Manolis Kalomiris Chant du Soir

Perception of Intrinsic Formal Functionality: An Empirical Investigation of Mozart s Materials

Perceptual Tests of an Algorithm for Musical Key-Finding

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic

Musical Forces and Melodic Expectations: Comparing Computer Models and Experimental Results

PRESCOTT UNIFIED SCHOOL DISTRICT District Instructional Guide January 2016

EXPLAINING AND PREDICTING THE PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL STRUCTURE

Modal pitch space COSTAS TSOUGRAS. Affiliation: Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Faculty of Fine Arts, School of Music

The Effects of Study Condition Preference on Memory and Free Recall LIANA, MARISSA, JESSI AND BROOKE

THE OFT-PURPORTED NOTION THAT MUSIC IS A MEMORY AND MUSICAL EXPECTATION FOR TONES IN CULTURAL CONTEXT

Effects of Musical Training on Key and Harmony Perception

IN THE HISTORY OF MUSIC THEORY, THE CONCEPT PERCEIVING THE CLASSICAL CADENCE

Online detection of tonal pop-out in modulating contexts.

An Integrated Music Chromaticism Model

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

Expectancy in Melody: Tests of Children and Adults

An Experimental Analysis of the Role of Harmony in Musical Memory and the Categorization of Genre

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Perception of the "Macroform" in the Quartetto per Archi in due Tempi (1955) by Bruno Maderna

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

Pitch Perception in Changing Harmony

Perceptual Considerations in Designing and Fitting Hearing Aids for Music Published on Friday, 14 March :01

Robert Rowe MACHINE MUSICIANSHIP

Perception: A Perspective from Musical Theory

Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

Measuring the Facets of Musicality: The Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index. Daniel Müllensiefen Goldsmiths, University of London

Timbral description of musical instruments

AP Music Theory Syllabus

Perceptual Evaluation of Automatically Extracted Musical Motives

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

Modeling memory for melodies

Music Curriculum Kindergarten

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Melody: sequences of pitches unfolding in time. HST 725 Lecture 12 Music Perception & Cognition

A-LEVEL Music. MUS2A Mark scheme June Version 1.0: Final Mark Scheme

THE TONAL-METRIC HIERARCHY: ACORPUS ANALYSIS

Music Annual Assessment Report AY17-18

An Interactive Case-Based Reasoning Approach for Generating Expressive Music

A new tool for measuring musical sophistication: The Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index

A CAPPELLA EAR TRAINING

Visual and Performing Arts Standards. Dance Music Theatre Visual Arts

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Toward an analysis of polyphonic music in the textual symbolic segmentation

The influence of performers stage entrance behavior on the audience s performance elaboration

Learning and Liking of Melody and Harmony: Further Studies in Artificial Grammar Learning

Facilitation and Coherence Between the Dynamic and Retrospective Perception of Segmentation in Computer-Generated Music

Pitch Spelling Algorithms

Commentary on David Huron s On the Role of Embellishment Tones in the Perceptual Segregation of Concurrent Musical Parts

SAMPLE. Music Studies 2019 sample paper. Question booklet. Examination information

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

STEVENS HANDBOOK OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

High School String Players Perception of Violin, Trumpet, and Voice Intonation

Improving music composition through peer feedback: experiment and preliminary results

TONAL HIERARCHIES, IN WHICH SETS OF PITCH

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

Klee or Kid? The subjective experience of drawings from children and Paul Klee Pronk, T.

The Relative Importance of Local and Global Structures in Music Perception

Quantifying Tone Deafness in the General Population

Arts Education Essential Standards Crosswalk: MUSIC A Document to Assist With the Transition From the 2005 Standard Course of Study

MPATC-GE 2042: Psychology of Music. Citation and Reference Style Rhythm and Meter

Calculating Dissonance in Chopin s Étude Op. 10 No. 1

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using vocabulary and language of music.

Transcription:

Alma Mater Studiorum of Bologna, August 22-26 2006 Construction of a harmonic phrase Ziv, N. Behavioral Sciences Max Stern Academic College Emek Yizre'el, Israel naomiziv@013.net Storino, M. Dept. of Music Bonfiglioli, L. Incasa, I. Caterina, R. Baroni, M. Dept. of Music ABSTRACT Background. Previous studies (Krumhansl, 2001, 2005; Tillmann, Bharucha, & Bigand, 2000) have established that the average listener possesses a certain representation of tonal grammar. This acquired knowledge, stored in long term memory, guides listening and helps create a schematic representation of musical pieces. Most studies (Povel & Jansen, 2002) either require participants to judge the appropriateness of short harmonic or melodic sequences, or ask participants to reconstruct, through the puzzle paradigm (Deliège, 1987, 1989, 2001), given musical pieces. Aim. The aim of the present study was to examine whether the grammatical knowledge of the tonal idiom is strong and stable enough to direct the active construction of a sequence of chords. The subjects were divided into three groups corresponding to different levels of presumable competence: from a minimum to a maximum. Their competence was valued according to different criteria, in order to avoid a too simple distinction between In: M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, M. Costa (2006) Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition (ICMPC9), Bologna/Italy, August 22-26 2006. 2006 The Society for Music Perception & Cognition (SMPC) and European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM). Copyright of the content of an individual paper is held by the primary (first-named) author of that paper. All rights reserved. No paper from this proceedings may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval systems, without permission in writing from the paper's primary author. No other part of this proceedings may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval system, without permission in writing from SMPC and ESCOM. and non-. Method. 66 participants took part in the study. Participants were presented with 8 piano chords (I I II III IV V V 7 VI) in random order. The participants were asked to listen to each chord, and then to combine them in order to create a satisfying musical sequence. Participants were allowed to try out as many combinations as they liked. Results. Two main criteria were chosen to evaluate the results: a positive criterion (a perfect cadence subdominant, dominant and tonic in the final position) and a negative one (no tonic in the first and final position). According to these criteria and to their possible mixtures (analyses focused on the positioning of the various chords in the sequence) the results were organized form a maximum to a minimum of apparent tonal competence. Preliminary results seem to indicate that, although a certain theoretical musical competence linked to musical studies seems to be necessary in order to get accuracy and precision in the construction task, different and not clear relationship can be found between the levels of musical competence and the correctness of the results. Conclusions. The research shows the necessity to establish more accurate distinctions among the participants of tonal test, beyond the two traditional categories of and non-. INTRODUCTION Previous studies have established that the average listener possesses a stable representation of tonal grammar. This knowledge is acquired even without formal musical training, and is stored in long-term memory (Krumhansl, 2001, 2005; Tillmann, Bharucha & Bigand, 2000). When listening to musical pieces, it guides the listener both in connecting between successive notes (Cuddy & Lunney, ISBN 88-7395-155-4 2006 ICMPC 1872

1995; Narmour, 1990) and in creating a schematic representation of the piece as a whole (Deliège & Mélen, 1997). Most studies require participants to judge given short harmonic or melodic sequences on a variety of parameters (Janata, Birk, Tillmann & Bharucha, 2003; Jansen & Povel, 2004). Participants in these studies are thus passive listeners to artificial musical stimuli. Other studies (e.g.: Deliège, 1996, 2001; Koniari, Predazzer & Mélen, 2001) require participants to actively reconstruct a given whole musical piece, through the puzzle paradigm. Studies comparing performance between and non- vary in the criterion for distinguishing from non-, defining as participants having 6 (Dowling, Tillmann & Ayers, 2002), 8 (Holleran et al., 1995) or 12 years of formal musical training (Bigand & Pineau, 1997). The present study was designed to answer two main questions: 1. Would the mental representation of tonal grammar guide participants in an active task of constructing a tonal harmonic phrase from a set of given chords? Presented with an unorganized set of diatonic chords, would participants juxtapose them to create a balanced harmonic phrase? 2. Would a finer distinction between levels of musical expertise, differentiating between and non- in more specific categories, explain differences in musical compositions? It was hypothesized that the higher the level of musical expertise, the closer the musical phrase composed would be to a balanced, hierarchical harmonic phrase, beginning on the tonic, and ending with a perfect cadence, according the rules of tonal grammar. METHOD Participants Sixty-six participants took part in the study (38 females, mean age= 25.71, s.d.=6.05, 28 males, mean age=31.36, s.d.=8.05). General mean age=28.11, s.d.=7.47. Participants were divided into three groups, according to musical expertise: participants having 8 years or more of formal musical training were considered professional (23 participants), participants having between 4-8 years of musical training were considered nonprofessional (15 participants), and participants having less than 4 years of formal musical training were considered non- (28 participants). Questionnaire A questionnaire regarding musical expertise was created for the study. Participants were asked whether they studied music, and if so, how many years. They were then asked to rate on a scale from 1 to 5 (1=not at all, 5=very much) their degree of liking of various musical styles: classical, rock, pop, jazz, techno, rap, folk, ethno, latin, new-age. They were also asked to indicate the number of hours they listen to music each day: 1= never, 2= less than one hour a day, 3= between two and three hours a day, 4= between four and seven hours a day, 5= all the time. Apparatus A special computer program was designed for the study. On the top part of the screen a row of squares was presented, numbered from 1 to 8. By placing the cursor on one of the squares and pressing the right button on the mouse, participants could hear each numbered segment, which was a chord. On the bottom part of the screen, eight empty squares appear, into which participants may copy the number of the chord in each position. By pressing a button marked "try the sequence" participants could listen to the series of chords they combined in succession. By pressing a button called "cancel the sequence" they could erase the combination they tried. Musical stimuli The chords used in the study were diatonic chords from the C major key. The degrees used were: I, I, II, III, IV, V, V 7, VI. The order of presentation in the initial series is presented in table 1: Table 1. order of presentation of chords Position 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Degree VI II I I III V 7 V IV Procedure Participants were presented with the computer program, and received an explanation about how to use it. They were then asked to listen to the segments separately a first time, and then to try to combine them on the bottom line, in whichever order they liked. They were told they could try as many combinations as they liked, until they arrive at a combination which includes all segments, each segment only once (the program did not enable them to use a segment twice), and which sounds good to them. No further specifications were made, in order to allow participants as much freedom as possible in deciding on final combinations. After finishing the task, participants were asked to fill in the questionnaire described above, and to rate on a scale ranging from 1 to 5 (1=not at all, 5=very much) their degree of satisfaction with the combination they created. RESULTS Listening habits No significant differences between professional, non-professional, and non were found in hours spent listening to music each day. Participants in all groups listen on average between two and three hours a day to music. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 2006 ICMPC 1873

As for preferred musical styles, several significant differences were found between the three groups: Professional prefer classical music significantly more than non-, and less than non-professional and non- to techno. They listen significantly less than non- to rap and new-age music. Means and standard deviations of liking for the most liked styles are presented in table 2. Table 2. Means and standard deviations of preferences for musical styles Professional Non-prof. Non Classical* 4 (1.38) 3.33 (1.17) 2.71 (1.3) Rock 3.17 (2.28) 4.13 (1.12) 3.14 (1.43) Pop 2.96 (1.14) 2.47 (1.3) 3.07 (1.38) Jazz 3.09 (1.31) 3.47 (1.55) 2.82 (1.5) Techno* 1.09 (0.5) 1.73 (0.96) 1.68 (1.09) Rap* 1.3 (0.65) 2.07 (1.16) 2.29 (1.38) Folk 2.09 (1.31) 2.73 (1.43) 1.86 (1.2) Ethno 2 (1.24) 2.73 (1.33) 2.61 (1.39) Latin 2.26 (1.84) 1.8 (0.86) 2.54 (1.2) New Age 1.52 (0.59) 1.73 (1.03) 2.46 (1.2) *p<.05 As can be seen in table 2, the preferred styles of professional were classical music, rock and jazz in that order. The non-professional prefer the same styles, but their ordering is rock, jazz and classical music. The non- prefer rock and pop. A new variable was computed, in which the number of musical styles receiving a rating of 4 or 5 was counted for each participant. Although the differences between the groups were not significant on this variable, the nonprofessional received a higher mean on this variable (3.06, s.d.=1.43) than professional (2.3, s.d.=1.6) and non- (2.82, s.d.=1.46). This tends to suggest that non-professional have a wider range of musical styles which they listen to. Musical combinations In order to compare the final musical combinations achieved by participants, several dichotomic categorical variables were defined, focusing on the conclusion and the beginning of the sequence according to the rules of tonal musical grammar: 1. A sequence ending on V (7) -I or II(IV)-V (7) -I 2. A sequence ending on I 3. A sequence beginning on I 4. A sequence neither ending nor beginning on I The total number and percentage of participants fulfilling the new criteria variables are shown in table 3. Expected values were calculated as the probability of placing each chosen chord in a given position. For example, the probability of placing the I degree in the final position was 2/8, since the I degree appeared twice in the given segments. Table 3. Number and percentage of participants fulfilling combination criteria Yes No Chi square Ending on V (7) -I Or II (IV)-V (7) -I 25 (38%) 41 (62%) 94 Ending on I 44 (66%) 22 (33%) 61 Beginning on I 27 45 8.9 Neither Ending nor beginning on I (40%) 10 (15%) 56 (85%) p<.05 32 As can be seen in table 3, all results were significant, showing that in general, participants tended to place the I degree in the first and final position, and the V or V 7 in the position before last. Participants were relatively satisfied with their compositions (mean=3.77, s.d.=.76). No significant relations were found between fulfilling the different criteria and hours spent listening to music or preference for the various musical styles. Comparison between levels of musical expertise The number and percentage of participants within each group who filled these criteria are presented in table 4. Table 4. Number and percentage of participants fulfilling combination criteria Professional Non-prof. Non Ending V-I or II(IV)-V-I 13 (56.5%) 5 (33%) 7 (25%) Ending with I 20 (87%) 9 15 (53%) Beginning with I 12 (52%) 9 6 (21%) Neither ending nor beginning with I 1 (4%) 1 (6%) 8 (28%) No significant differences between professional, non-professional and non- were found in the first parameter, concluding the combination with a perfect cadence. However, significant differences were found between the groups in the three other parameters: ending the combination with the I degree (chi square= 6.722, p=.035), beginning the sequence with the I degree (chi square= 7.865, p=.020), and neither ending nor beginning on the first degree (chi square= 6.851, p=.033). In general, professional ' final combinations tended to conform more than non-professional ' and non-professional ISBN 88-7395-155-4 2006 ICMPC 1874

' to compositional norms of tonal music. It is interesting to note, however, that the percentage of nonprofessional beginning the sequence with the I degree was higher than that of professional. No significant differences between the groups was found in degree of satisfaction with final combinations. When looking at each group separately, several significant differences were found: in the professional ' group, a significant difference was found in ending the sequence on the I degree (chi square=12.565 ), and in neither ending nor beginning on the I degree (chi square=19.174, ). In the nonprofessional ' group, a significant difference was found in neither ending nor beginning on the I degree (chi square=11.267, p=.001). In non-, a significant difference was found in ending with a cadence (chi square=7, p=.008), in beginning with the I degree (chi square= 9.143, p=.002), and in neither ending nor beginning on the I degree (chi square=5.143, p=0.023). No significant differences were found within each group in the levels of satisfaction between participants who fulfilled each criterion and those who didn't. DISCUSSION The present study required participants to combine a set of diatonic chords to create a harmonic phrase. Results show that beyond levels of musical expertise, participants in general succeeded in recognizing the tonic, and tended to place it at the conclusion of the musical phrase. The use of the puzzle paradigm in the present study enhances results found in previous studies investigating the mental representation of the tonal system (e.g.; Holleran, Riess Jones & Butler, 1995; Jansen & Povel, 2004) by asking participants to actively manipulate the musical stimuli, instead of simply rating given musical segments on various scales. Results may thus give a stronger indication of the influence of previous knowledge on the processing of music. Moreover, the use of a simple series of chords, instead of a segmentation of a real repertoire piece (e.g.; Deliège, Mélen, Stammers & Cross, 1994), allows participants greater freedom in their manipulation, and does not imply strictly correct or incorrect answers. As was expected, level of musical expertise influenced the construction of the harmonic phrase. Participants having a high level of formal musical training tended to combine the given chords in a fashion conforming to tonal norms more than non-professional and non-. However, even within the two levels of lesser musical expertise, participants succeeded above chance in creating such a phrase. Another important remark is the influence of the musical styles preferred by the three groups of listeners. As we have seen, the professional listen to classical music more than non-professional and non. The different results in the construction of the harmonic phrase could depend not only on the musical competence but also on the characteristics of the usually listened musical styles. The rock and pop music are based on different sequences of harmonies respect of tonal music; they have not always the simple harmonic schema of tonal classical music. On the other side long many years the professional play tonal music constructed according the succession of chords used in the present research. This point suggests the need for further studies in order to investigate the real influence of the listened musical styles in task of evaluation and of reconstruction of tonal music. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Thanks due to Fabio Regazzi for the creation of the computer program and for his precious suggestions. REFERENCES Bigand, E. & Pineau, M. (1997). Global context effects on musical expectancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(7), 1098-1107. Cuddy, L.L. & Lunney, C.A. (1995). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: Perceptual judgments of melodic continuity. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(4), 451-462. Deliège, I. (1996). Cue Abstraction as a Component of Categorisation Processes in Music Listening. Psychology of Music, 24/2, 131-156. Deliège, I. (2001). Prototype Effects in Music Listening: An Empirical Approach to the Notion of Imprint. Music Perception, 18/3, 233-244, 371-407. Deliège, I., Mélen, M., Stammers, & Cross, I. (1994). Musical schemata in real time listening. In I. Deliège (Ed.), Proceedings of the 3rd ICMPC (pp. 271-272). Liège: ESCOM Publications. Deliège, I. & Mélen, (1997). Cue abstraction in the representation of musical form. In I. Deliège & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Perception and Cognition of Music (pp. 387-412). Hove: Psychology Press. Dowling, W.J., Tillmann, B. & Ayers, D.F. (2002). Memory and the experience of hearing music. Music Perception, 19(2), 249-276. Holleran, S., Riess Jones, M. & Butler, D. (1995). Perceiving implied harmony: The influence of melodic and harmonic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21(3), 737-753. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 2006 ICMPC 1875

Janata, P., Birk, J., Tillmann, B. & Bharucha, J.J. (2003). Online detection of tonal pop-out in modulating contexts. Music Perception, 20(3), 283-305. Jansen, E. & Povel, D.J. (2004). Perception of arpeggiated chord progressions. Musicae Scientiae, 8(1), 7-52. Krumhansl, C.L. (2001). Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch. New York: Oxford Press. Krumhansl, C.L. (2005). The cognition of tonality as we know it today. Journal of New Music Research, 33, 253-268. Narmour, E. (1990). The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-Realization Model. Chicago: of Chicago Press. Povel, D. & Jansen, E. (2002). Harmonic Factors in the Perception of Tonal Melodies. Music Perception, 20/1, 51-86 Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J.J. & Bigand, E. (2000). Implicit learning of tonality: A self-organizing approach. Psychological Review, 107, 885-913. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 2006 ICMPC 1876