m=search&session_search_id= &hitnum=9&se ction=music.00070

Similar documents
Absolute Pitch. R. Parncutt and D. J. Levitin

Comparing methods of musical pitch processing: How perfect is Perfect Pitch?

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Dial A440 for absolute pitch: Absolute pitch memory by non-absolute pitch possessors

Hearing Research 240 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Hearing Research. journal homepage:

Absolute Memory of Learned Melodies

Absolute Pitch: An Approach for Identification of Genetic and Nongenetic Components

7. GROUNDS OF ABSOLUTE PITCH DEVELOPMENT IN YAMAHA MUSIC SCHOOL Dorina Iușcă 19

Absolute Pitch and Its Frequency Range

Prevalence of absolute pitch: A comparison between Japanese and Polish music students

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

UC San Diego UC San Diego Electronic Theses and Dissertations

Consonance, 2: Psychoacoustic factors: Grove Music Online Article for print

Okólnik 2, Warszawa, Poland

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Pitch Perception. Roger Shepard

How do we perceive vocal pitch accuracy during singing? Pauline Larrouy-Maestri & Peter Q Pfordresher

Absolute pitch correlates with high performance on interval naming tasks

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

ABSOLUTE PITCH W. DIXON WARD. Hearing Research Laboratory University of Minnesota, Minneapolis Minneapolis, Minnesota !.

Perceiving Differences and Similarities in Music: Melodic Categorization During the First Years of Life

Interplay of Tone and Color: Absolute Pitch and Synesthesia

Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch

WORKING MEMORY AND MUSIC PERCEPTION AND PRODUCTION IN AN ADULT SAMPLE. Keara Gillis. Department of Psychology. Submitted in Partial Fulfilment

AUD 6306 Speech Science

Shifting Perceptions: Developmental Changes in Judgments of Melodic Similarity

Stewart, Lauren and Walsh, Vincent (2001) Neuropsychology: music of the hemispheres Dispatch, Current Biology Vol.11 No.

The power of music in children s development

Do Zwicker Tones Evoke a Musical Pitch?

"The mind is a fire to be kindled, not a vessel to be filled." Plutarch

Absolute pitch and Tone Identification

The Semitone Paradox

Construction of a harmonic phrase

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Recognition of notated melodies by possessors and nonpossessors of absolute pitch

What Can Experiments Reveal About the Origins of Music? Josh H. McDermott

Quantifying Tone Deafness in the General Population

MEMORY & TIMBRE MEMT 463

TUNING* Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences University of Washington Seattle, Washington !. INTRODUCTION

Children s recognition of their musical performance

BIBB 060: Music and the Brain Tuesday, 1:30-4:30 Room 117 Lynch Lead vocals: Mike Kaplan

Murrieta Valley Unified School District High School Course Outline February 2006

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

Jessicah R. Saldana. Songs and Chants

Thirty-three Opinionated Ideas About How to Choose Repertoire for Musical Success

Countering*Trade*Opponents *Issues*with*TPP:*Point*and*Counterpoint* * * Opponents *Point* * * * * * * * Counterpoint**

Absolute pitch and Tone Identification

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

Absolute Pitch: Effects of Timbre on Note-Naming Ability

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

Development of the Perception of Musical Relations: Semitone and Diatonic Structure

Topics in Computer Music Instrument Identification. Ioanna Karydi

Modeling Melodic Perception as Relational Learning Using a Symbolic- Connectionist Architecture (DORA)

i 13 xxi 59 xli 107 ii 15 xxii 62 xlii 110 iii 17 xxiii 65 xliii 112 iv 20 xxiv 67 xliv 114 v 22 xxv 69 xlv 117 vi 25 xxvi 72 xlvi 119

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Perceptual Considerations in Designing and Fitting Hearing Aids for Music Published on Friday, 14 March :01

Brain.fm Theory & Process

Cognitive Processes for Infering Tonic

EE391 Special Report (Spring 2005) Automatic Chord Recognition Using A Summary Autocorrelation Function

Music Perception with Combined Stimulation

Quantification of the Hierarchy of Tonal Functions Within a Diatonic Context

Estimating the Time to Reach a Target Frequency in Singing

The Power of Listening

Absolute pitch memory: Its prevalence among musicians and. dependence on the testing context

Music 175: Pitch II. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015

Effects of Auditory and Motor Mental Practice in Memorized Piano Performance

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

Acoustic and musical foundations of the speech/song illusion

Musical Illusions Diana Deutsch Department of Psychology University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093

University of Wollongong. Research Online

What is music as a cognitive ability?

Proceedings of the 7th WSEAS International Conference on Acoustics & Music: Theory & Applications, Cavtat, Croatia, June 13-15, 2006 (pp54-59)

An Integrated Music Chromaticism Model

Analysis, Synthesis, and Perception of Musical Sounds

II. Prerequisites: Ability to play a band instrument, access to a working instrument

Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

PHY 103 Auditory Illusions. Segev BenZvi Department of Physics and Astronomy University of Rochester

Music Theory: A Very Brief Introduction

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

Perception of melodic accuracy in occasional singers: role of pitch fluctuations? Pauline Larrouy-Maestri & Peter Q Pfordresher

A CAPPELLA EAR TRAINING

Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords

Topic 4. Single Pitch Detection

Children's Discrimination of Melodic Intervals

Processing Linguistic and Musical Pitch by English-Speaking Musicians and Non-Musicians

COMPUTER ENGINEERING SERIES

The Healing Power of Music. Scientific American Mind William Forde Thompson and Gottfried Schlaug

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

Memory and Production of Standard Frequencies in College-Level Musicians

How stable is pitch labeling accuracy in absolute pitch possessors?

COGNITIVE INTERFERENCE IN THE PERCEPTION OF PITCH AND LOUDNESS IN A FIVE-NOTE MUSICAL PATTERN DISSERTATION. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

EXPECTANCY AND ATTENTION IN MELODY PERCEPTION

Perceiving patterns of ratios when they are converted from relative durations to melody and from cross rhythms to harmony

Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Sequential Association Rules in Atonal Music

Transcription:

http://www.grovemusic.com/shared/views/article.html?fro m=search&session_search_id=684507428&hitnum=9&se ction=music.00070 Macmillan Publishers Ltd, 2001-2002 Absolute pitch. RICHARD PARNCUTT, DANIEL J. LEVITIN The ability either to identify the chroma (pitch class) of any isolated tone, using labels such as C, 261 Hz or do ( passive absolute pitch), or to reproduce a specified chroma for example, by singing or adjusting the frequency of a tone generator without reference to an external standard ( active absolute pitch (AP): Bachem, 1937; Baggaley, 1974; Ward, 1982). Both skills may be called tone-ap. Absolute pitch may also involve recognizing whether a familiar piece is played in the correct key (passive), or singing a familiar song in the correct key (active); this skill is known as piece-ap. Cognitively, both tone- and piece-ap involve two separate sub-skills: long-term pitch memory and an appropriate form of linguistic coding for attaching labels to stimuli (Levitin, 1994). True tone-ap requires individual internal pitch standards for all 12 chroma. This template can shift with age by as much as two semitones (Vernon, 1977; Wynn, 1992); shifts can also be induced neurochemically (Chaloupka, Mitchell and Muirhead, 1994). A musician with only one absolute pitch reference (e.g. a' = 440 Hz) and good relative pitch has pseudo-ap (Bachem, 1937); so has an experimental participant who internalizes several, but not all, pitches of the chromatic scale (Cuddy, 1970). The labels used in tone-ap are musical note names; in piece-ap they are names of pieces and texts of songs.

The popular term perfect pitch is misleading. Musicians claiming tone-ap are not necessarily better at discriminating tones of almost the same frequency, or at perceiving small deviations in intonation, than other musicians (Bachem, 1954; Burns and Campbell, 1994). AP possessors can typically tune pitches to within 20 60 cents of target frequencies (Rakowski and Morawska- Büngeler, 1987). In passive tasks, they regularly make semitone errors (Lockhead and Byrd, 1981; Miyazaki, 1988), and are not necessarily better than other musicians at identifying octave registers (Rakowski and Morawska- Büngeler, 1987; Miyazaki, 1988). There is nothing perfect about absolute pitch. Nor does absolute pitch appear to correlate with other musical skills. Composers with tone-ap (e.g. Mozart, Skryabin, Messiaen, Boulez) have not written indisputably better or worse music than composers without it (e.g. Wagner, Tchaikovsky, Ravel, Stravinsky: see Slonimsky, 1988). While tone-ap is sometimes an advantage (helping horn players to imagine tones before playing them, singers to perform atonal music and theorists to follow large-scale tonal structures by ear), it can also be a hindrance (e.g. when playing or singing in a key other than written). Regarding relative pitch, musicians with tone-ap can be less skilled than other musicians, calculating intervals and chords from note names rather than hearing them directly (Miyazaki, 1991 2, 1993 4). Moreover, their constant awareness of musical pitch labels can detract from their enjoyment of music. Only about one person in 10,000 claims to have tone-ap (Profita and Bidder, 1988). The distinction between possessors and non-possessors is not clearcut: the former can usually label 70 100% of randomly selected middlerange piano tones (Miyazaki, 1988), while the latter identify up to 40% of the tones well above the chance level of 8 3% (Lockhead and Byrd, 1981; Miyazaki,

1988). This is not surprising given that neurological information on absolute pitch is available at all levels of the auditory system (Moore, 1977). Even songbirds (Hulse, Cynx and Humpal, 1984), wolves (Tooze, Harington and Fentress, 1990) and monkeys (D'Amato, 1987 8) demonstrate absolute pitch memory. Clearly, tone-ap must be learnt from exposure to music containing fixed pitches, coupled with knowledge of pitch labels (Wedell, 1934; Levitin, 1999). Chroma identification rates are higher, and response times lower, for white piano keys than for black (Miyazaki, 1989 90; Takeuchi and Hulse, 1991), presumably because white keys occur more often in piano music and have simpler labels (see Rosch, 1975). Similarly, piece-ap relies on repeated exposure to pieces played in the same key. Tone-AP may also be unlearnt during musical acculturation in which familiar music and pitch relationships are regularly transposed into different keys; this may explain its rarity (Abraham, 1901 2; Watt, 1917). Like language, tone-ap usually develops during a critical period in early life (Ward, 1982). Musicians who start musical training early are more likely to acquire tone-ap than those who start late (Wellek, 1938; Sergent, 1969). Younger children acquire piece-ap more easily than older children (shown by singing a song in its regular key: Sergeant and Roche, 1973). Tone-AP can be acquired in later life, but only with considerable motivation, time and effort (Meyer, 1899; Cuddy, 1968; Brady, 1970). Late acquirers of tone-ap are generally less spontaneous and accurate in their identification of pitches; they tend not to develop a complete internal chroma template, filling the gaps by means of relative pitch. Both infants (Clarkson and Clifton, 1985) and adults (Wedell, 1934) seem able to perceive pitch absolutely within a range of about three semitones. According to the

innateness hypothesis (Révész, 1913; Bachem, 1937) newborns vary in their predisposition to acquire tone-ap, that is, to reduce this range to one semitone and apply chromatic labels. This hypothesis has not been confirmed experimentally. Even if it were, it would not provide unequivocal support for innateness: newborns have at least four months of prenatal auditory experience (Lecanuet, 1995). The search for an absolute-pitch gene (Profita and Bidder, 1988) or brain centre (Schlaug and others, 1995) may be in vain, given that, in a learnt skill, nature and nurture cannot easily be separated (Jeffress, 1962) and that absolute pitch involves several neurally separate sub-processes (pitch perception, classification, labelling, storage in long-term memory, retrieval from memory: Levitin, 1999). Absolute pitch can be enhanced by association or integration with other perceptual or cognitive parameters (Siegel, 1974; Zatorre and Beckett, 1989). For example, tone-ap is enhanced by linking pitches to colours (chromaesthesia: Peacock, 1984 5; Rogers, 1987). Musicians with tone-ap tend to identify the tones of their main instrument more reliably than other timbres (Lockhead and Byrd, 1981), suggesting an intrinsic cognitive link between pitch and timbre. For similar reasons, piece-ap, involving complex, meaningful sound objects, is more widespread than tone-ap: musicians not claiming tone-ap can recognize whether a familiar piece is played in its correct key (Terhardt and Seewann, 1983 4), and non-musicians can sing well-known tunes in the same key on different occasions (Halpern, 1989; Heaton, 1992), or in the keys in which they learnt them (Levitin, 1994), at levels considerably exceeding chance. Piece-AP is further facilitated by the use of everyday linguistic labels rather than abstract note names. Tests for absolute pitch should be designed to prevent other parameters from facilitating tone recognition. It is

impossible to rule out the use of relative pitch (Ward, 1982; Costall, 1985), although slow reactions can be reliable evidence of its use (Bachem, 1954). Use of timbre can be completely eliminated by randomly varying the spectral envelope of presented tones, or by having participants sing their responses. Because the pitch of a pure tone depends on its intensity (Stevens, 1935), results of absolute pitch experiments using pure tones should be interpreted with caution. BIBLIOGRAPHY M. Meyer: Is the Memory of Absolute Pitch Capable of Development by Training?, Psychological Review, vi (1899), 514 16 O. Abraham: Das absolute Tonbewusstsein, SIMG, iii (1901 2), 1 86 G. Révész: Zur Grundlegung der Tonpsychologie (Leipzig, 1913) H.J. Watt: The Psychology of Sound (Cambridge, 1917) C.H. Wedell: The Nature of Absolute Judgement of Pitch, Journal of Experimental Psychology, xvii (1934), 485 503 S.S. Stevens: The Relation of Pitch to Intensity, JASA, vii (1935), 150 54 A. Bachem: Various Types of Absolute Pitch, JASA, ix (1937), 146 51 A. Wellek: Das absolute Gehör und seine Typen, Beihefte zur Zeitschrift für Angewandte Psychologie und Charakterkunde, lxxxiii (1938), 1 368 A. Bachem: Time Factors in Relative and Absolute Pitch Determination, JASA, xxvi (1954), 751 3 L.A. Jeffress: Absolute Pitch, JASA, xxxiv (1962), 987 L.L. Cuddy: Practice Effects in the Absolute Judgement of Pitch, JASA, xliii (1968), 1069 76 D. Sergeant: Experimental Investigation of Absolute Pitch, JRME, xvii (1969), 135 43 P.T. Brady: Fixed Scale Mechanism of Absolute Pitch, JASA, xlviii (1970), 883 7

L.L. Cuddy: Training the Absolute Identification of Pitch, Perception and Psychophysics, viii (1970), 265 9 D.C. Sergeant and S. Roche: Perceptual Shifts in the Auditory Information Processing of Young Children, Psychology of Music, i/1 (1973), 39 48 J. Baggaley: Measurement of Absolute Pitch, Psychology of Music, xxii/1 (1974), 11 17 J.A. Siegel: Sensory and Verbal Coding Strategies in Subjects with Absolute Pitch, Journal of Experimental Psychology, ciii (1974), 37 44 E. Rosch: Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, civ (1975), 192 223 B.C.J. Moore: An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing (Baltimore and London, 1977, 4/1997) E. Vernon: Absolute Pitch: a Case Study, British Journal of Psychology, lxviii (1977), 485 9 G.R. Lockhead and R. Byrd: Practically Perfect Pitch, JASA, lxx (1981), 387 9 W.D. Ward: Absolute Pitch, The Psychology of Music, ed. D. Deutsch (New York, 1982, 2/1999), 265 98 E. Terhardt and M. Seewan: Aural Key Identification and its Relationship to Absolute Pitch, Music Perception, i (1983 4), 63 83 S.H. Hulse, J. Cynx and J. Humpal: Absolute and Relative Pitch Discrimination in Serial Pitch Perception by Birds, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, cxiii (1984), 38 54 K. Peacock: Synaesthetic Perception: Alexander Skriabin's Colour Hearing, Music Perception, ii (1984 5), 483 506 M.G. Clarkson and R.K. Clifton: Infant Pitch Perception: Evidence from Responding to Pitch Categories and the Missing Fundamental, JASA, lxxvii (1985), 1521 8 A. Costall: The Relativity of Absolute Pitch, Musical Structure and Cognition, ed. P. Howell, I. Cross and R. West (London, 1985), 189 208

A. Rakowski and M. Morawska-Büngeler: In Search of the Criteria for Absolute Pitch, Archives of Acoustics, xii (1987), 198 207 G.L. Rogers: Four Cases of Pitch-Specific Chromesthesia in Trained Musicians with Absolute Pitch, Psychology of Music, xv/1 (1987), 198 207 M.R. D'Amato: A Search for Tonal Pattern Perception in Cebus Monkeys: Why Monkeys Can't Hum a Tune, Music Perception, v (1987 8), 453 80 K. Miyazaki: Musical Pitch Identification by Absolute Pitch Possessors, Perception & Psychophysics, xliv (1988), 501 12 J. Profita and T.G. Bidder: Perfect Pitch, American Journal of Medical Genetics, xxix (1988), 763 71 N. Slonimsky: Perfect Pitch: a Life Story (Oxford, 1988) A.R. Halpern: Memory for the Absolute Pitch of Familiar Songs, Memory and Cognition, xvii (1989), 572 81 R.J. Zatorre and C. Beckett: Multiple Coding Strategies in the Retention of Musical Tones by Possessors of Absolute Pitch, Memory and Cognition, xvii (1989), 582 89 K. Miyazaki: Absolute Pitch Identification: Effects of Timbre and Pitch Region, Music Perception, vii (1989 90), 1 14 Z.J. Tooze, F.H. Harrington and J.C. Fentress: Individually Distinct Vocalizations in Timber Wolves canis lupus, Animal Behaviour, xl (1990), 723 30 K. Miyazaki: Perception of Musical Intervals by Absolute Pitch Possessors, Music Perception, ix (1991 2), 413 26 A.H. Takeuchi and S.H. Hulse: Absolute-Pitch Judgements of Black- and White-Key Pitches, Music Perception, ix (1991 2), 27 46 C.P. Heaton: Air Ball: Spontaneous Large-Group Precision Chanting, Popular Music and Society, xvi/1 (1992), 81 4 V.T. Wynn: Absolute Pitch Revisited, British Journal of Psychology, lxxxiii (1992), 129 31

K. Miyazaki: Absolute Pitch as an Inability: Identification of Musical Intervals in a Tonal Context, Music Perception, xi (1993 4), 55 71 E.M. Burns and S.L. Campbell: Frequency and Frequency-Ratio Resolution by Possessors of Absolute and Relative Pitch: Examples of Categorical Perception?, JASA, xcvi (1994), 2704 19 V. Chaloupka, S. Mitchell and R. Muirhead: Observation of a Reversible, Medication-Induced Change in Pitch Perception, JASA, xcvi (1994), 145 9 D.J. Levitin: Absolute Memory for Musical Pitch: Evidence from the Production of Learned Melodies, Perception & Psychophysics, lvi (1994), 414 23 J.-P. Lecanuet: L'expérience auditive prénatale, Naissance et développement du sens musical, ed. I. Deliège and J.A. Sloboda (Paris, 1995), 7 37; Eng. trans. as Musical Beginnings (Oxford, 1996), 3 34 G. Schlaug and others: In Vivo Evidence of Structural Brain Asymmetry in Musicians, Science, cclxvii (1995), 699 701 D.J. Levitin: Absolute Pitch: Self-Reference and Human Memory, International Journal of Computing Anticipatory Systems, iv (1999), 255 266