Beneath the Paint: A Visual Journey through Conceptual Metaphor Violation

Similar documents
How Semantics is Embodied through Visual Representation: Image Schemas in the Art of Chinese Calligraphy *

AN INSIGHT INTO CONTEMPORARY THEORY OF METAPHOR

Metaphors we live by. Structural metaphors. Orientational metaphors. A personal summary

The promises and problems of a semiotic approach to mathematics, the history of mathematics and mathematics education Melle July 2007

On the Subjectivity of Translator During Translation Process From the Viewpoint of Metaphor

Musical Knowledge and Choral Curriculum Development

Gestalt, Perception and Literature

INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON ENGINEERING DESIGN ICED 05 MELBOURNE, AUGUST 15-18, 2005 GENERAL DESIGN THEORY AND GENETIC EPISTEMOLOGY

Cognitive poetics as a literary theory for analyzing Khayyam's poetry

Embodied music cognition and mediation technology

The Cognitive Nature of Metonymy and Its Implications for English Vocabulary Teaching

Ithaque : Revue de philosophie de l'université de Montréal

Understanding the Cognitive Mechanisms Responsible for Interpretation of Idioms in Hindi-Urdu

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Isabel Hernández Gomariz University of Córdoba

How 'Straight' Has Developed Its Meanings - Based on a metaphysical theory

Colloque Écritures: sur les traces de Jack Goody - Lyon, January 2008

Mixing Metaphors. Mark G. Lee and John A. Barnden

Tamar Sovran Scientific work 1. The study of meaning My work focuses on the study of meaning and meaning relations. I am interested in the duality of

Citation Dynamis : ことばと文化 (2000), 4:

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

Interpreting Museums as Cultural Metaphors

Visualizing Euclidean Rhythms Using Tangle Theory

Spatial Formations. Installation Art between Image and Stage.

Review of Illingworth, Shona (2011). The Watch Man / Balnakiel. Belgium, Film and Video Umbrella, 2011, 172 pages,

Is composition a mode of performing? Questioning musical meaning

The Interconnectedness Principle and the Semiotic Analysis of Discourse. Marcel Danesi University of Toronto

Metonymy Research in Cognitive Linguistics. LUO Rui-feng

METAPHOR Lecture Material Master Program in Literature Department of Linguistics, Faculty of Humanities University of Indonesia

Approaching Aesthetics on User Interface and Interaction Design

Review of Steve Larson, Musical Forces: Motion, Metaphor, and Meaning in Music (Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 2012)

Natika Newton, Foundations of Understanding. (John Benjamins, 1996). 210 pages, $34.95.

PARTICIPATION IN ALTERNATIVE REALITIES: RITUAL, CONSCIOUSNESS, AND ONTOLOGICAL TURN

Metaphors in English and Chinese

Barbara Tversky. using space to represent space and meaning

My thesis is that not only the written symbols and spoken sounds are different, but also the affections of the soul (as Aristotle called them).

Culture, Space and Time A Comparative Theory of Culture. Take-Aways

Rusudan Japaridze SYNESTHETIC METAPHORS IN WILLIAM BUTLER YEATS POETRY

REVIEW ARTICLE IDEAL EMBODIMENT: KANT S THEORY OF SENSIBILITY

The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination and Reason by Mark Johnson, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1987

M. Night Shyamalan s Unbreakable

Incommensurability and Partial Reference

Architecture as the Psyche of a Culture

Image and Imagination

SocioBrains THE INTEGRATED APPROACH TO THE STUDY OF ART

Costin Lianu. Bucharest University. Keywords: Aristotle, semantics, images, perception, brands, branding, homo economicus

Editor s Introduction

Chapter 2: The Early Greek Philosophers MULTIPLE CHOICE

Embodiment and body parts metaphors, idioms and figurative expressions

Introduction. 1 See e.g. Lakoff & Turner (1989); Gibbs (1994); Steen (1994); Freeman (1996);

that would join theoretical philosophy (metaphysics) and practical philosophy (ethics)?

Discourse analysis is an umbrella term for a range of methodological approaches that

Perceiving emotional causality in film: a conceptual and formal analysis

Keywords: semiotic; pragmatism; space; embodiment; habit, social practice.

CIDOC CRM A High Level Overview of the Model. George Bruseker ICS-FORTH CIDOC 2017 Tblisi, Georgia 25/09/2017

The TAO theory (function-construction theory)

Blending in action: Diagrams reveal conceptual integration in routine activity

Visual communication and interaction

Reuven Tsur Playing by Ear and the Tip of the Tongue Amsterdam/Philadelphia, Johns Benjamins, 2012

Musical Entrainment Subsumes Bodily Gestures Its Definition Needs a Spatiotemporal Dimension

Philosophical foundations for a zigzag theory structure

Introduction: Why Should Applied Linguists Care about Metaphor and Metonymy in Social Practices?

CUST 100 Week 17: 26 January Stuart Hall: Encoding/Decoding Reading: Stuart Hall, Encoding/Decoding (Coursepack)

Introduction: Metonymy across languages *

Does Comprehension Time Constraint Affect Poetic Appreciation of Metaphors?

Truth and Method in Unification Thought: A Preparatory Analysis

What is music as a cognitive ability?

THE ECOLOGICAL MEANING OF EMBODIMENT

Lecture (04) CHALLENGING THE LITERAL

Notes on Semiotics: Introduction

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

The Study of Motion Event Model and Cognitive Mechanism of English Fictive Motion Expressions of Access Paths

Georg Simmel and Formal Sociology

Studia Metrica et Poetica 1.1, 2014,

J 0 rgen Weber The Judgement of the Eye

CONCEPTUAL METAPHORS IN ENGLISH AND SHONA: A CROSS- LINGUISTIC AND CROSS-CULTURAL STUDY ISAAC MACHAKANJA

Challenging Form. Experimental Film & New Media

Notes on Gadamer, The Relevance of the Beautiful

Space, Time, and Interpretation

days of Saussure. For the most, it seems, Saussure has rightly sunk into

With thanks to Seana Coulson and Katherine De Long!

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

The role of texture and musicians interpretation in understanding atonal music: Two behavioral studies

Introduction It is now widely recognised that metonymy plays a crucial role in language, and may even be more fundamental to human speech and cognitio

GV958: Theory and Explanation in Political Science, Part I: Philosophy of Science (Han Dorussen)

Imagining Negative-Dimensional Space

A Hybrid Theory of Metaphor

What do our appreciation of tonal music and tea roses, our acquisition of the concepts

Fine-tuning our senses with (sound) art for aesthetic experience Nuno Fonseca IFILNOVA/CESEM-FCSH-UNL, Lisbon (PT)

Our interactions with home are intimate, sustained, complex, and even

Introduction to The Handbook of Economic Methodology

Chapter 4. Designing with Blends

Vuzik: Music Visualization and Creation on an Interactive Surface

Semiotics of culture. Some general considerations

Towards an Embodied Poetics of Cinema: The Metaphoric Construction of Abstract Meaning in Film

Metaphors: Concept-Family in Context

Week 25 Deconstruction

Aesthetic Equivalence in the Translation of Rubayyat of Khayyam Using Reception Aesthetics and Conceptual Metaphor Theory

UC Irvine Recent Work

The design value of business

Transcription:

Beneath the Paint: A Visual Journey through Conceptual Metaphor Violation Maria M. HEDBLOM 1 a CORE, Free University of Bozen-Bolzano, Italy Abstract. Metaphors are an undeniable part of many forms of art and they hint at the underlying conceptualisation that takes place in the silent conversation between an art piece and its perceiver. Abstract art, in particular, requires the viewer not only to analyse the colour palette and the shapes of the strokes but to subconsciously react to the underlying structures that often define the metaphors. After introducing a few cognitive theories involved in perception and knowledge transfer the paper introduces the reader to the painting Beneath the Paint. It is an abstract acrylic painting that plays on the two conceptual metaphor structures UP is GOOD and DARK is BAD and by presenting them in contradiction force the viewer to subconsciously choose its primary conceptualisation. Keywords. Art, metaphor, pattern recognition, image schema Human expression in music, literature and the arts often rely on the audience to fill in the blanks. The abstraction of music encourages the listener to allow tones and melodies to speak to the audience. Writers use for instance metaphors, hyperbole and synecdoche to force the reader to absorb more information than what the words and expressions contain themselves. In the arts, in particular during and the time after the impressionistic era, expression took the character of unconventional and liberal use of colour, shape and even the subject itself was suddenly up for interpretation. Art was no longer a method to capture a scene, or to tell a narrative, but to more strongly impose the viewer with an impression. Looking at these phenomena from a scientific point of view it has been made clear that the human brain is a skilled interpreter, that both searches and finds patterns in any situation, even where patterns are non-existent, a phenomena called apophenia. This imaginative hide and seek game of pattern recognition has been suggested not only to manifest in visual interpretations such as that in the arts but also to translate into higher levels of cognition [13]. Pattern recognition connects the perceived external world with internal mental constructs so as to generate order in chaos. Gestalt psychology [7] points out that there exist certain cognitive laws by which visual information are interpreted. For example, objects moving in the same direction are interpreted as a group, likewise, broken objects are interpreted as a whole. In Figure 1 a classic example of the gestalt principles is demon- 1 Corresponding Author: maria.m.hedblom@gmail.com

strated. Despite there being no complete triangle in the picture the mind fills in the gaps and connects the fragmented parts to perceive a triangle. Figure 1. Gestalt laws ensure that the viewer perceives a white triangle, despite no such figure existing. While gestalt laws point out how visual information may be completed in uncertain scenarios, how symbols (such as words, figures, paintings etc.) gain their meaning and can refer to a real-world concept is still a topic for debate. Sticking to the shape of triangles, in linguistics, this problem is often discussed in association to the semiotic triangle [15], illustrated in Figure 2. The corners of the triangle capture each one of the three components of any concept. However, how the connections between these corners manifest remains one of the fundamental problems in cognitive sciences. In fact, while there are theories as to how human cognition forms and allows for conceptualisation, there exists no consensus on the view of cognition. Throughout the last century, the development of (modern) psychology research advanced the investigations on human cognition from the purely speculative in philosophy and theology, to more empirically founded theories. The perception of the human mind went through many paradigm shifts and several different theories as to how the components of concepts relate to each other were introduced [3]. The early view of cognition, that grew alongside the birth of what we today would call modern-day computers, was called Computationalism and built on the idea that thinking is computing. Today the view has few completely devoted followers as it appears that the human mind is far too complex and irrational to satisfy the logical realm defined by computationalism. Instead, as cognitive psychology and neuroscience started to unfold the complexities of not only behaviour but also how the brain functions, the view of cognition shifted towards more embodied views of cognitive development and concept formation [11,16,12]. Neurolinguistic studies started to demonstrate how linguistic concepts and expressions were tied to sensorimotor cortices [18,2]. Figure 2. The semiotic triangle.

While embodied cognition is an umbrella term of different levels of devotion to the hypothesis, the general idea is that all human cognition can be traced, through the brain, back to the sensorimotor experiences. The concept (as denoted in the semiotic triangle) is the collected experiences that an individual has with the real world referent. The representation is the way in which this mental construct is communicated and remembered. For example, I could write the word banana on a piece of paper to capture the meaning of it. However, in my mind, the meaning of a banana is most likely the gathered attributes from my experiences with them, such as its particular visual appearance, its scent or its taste. All of which are increasingly difficult to represent and is therefore translated into symbols such as banana that we can use to communicate with. For increasingly complicated concepts such as finance or marriage, our experiences are less structured based on the concrete physical experiences. Despite this we often use spatial language to express increasingly abstract concepts 2. Indeed, one of the major reasons for the emergence of the modern view of embodied cognition was to provide means to explain why it is that human language to such a large degree is rooted in spatial language [10]. Many metaphors rely on spatial or embodied experiences such as: I am at a crossroad in my life, he is on the rise to power and their marriage is falling apart. Underlying these sentences are conceptual skeletons that can be traced back to the sensorimotor experiences acquired in early infancy. One such theory of conceptual skeletons is the theory of image schemas [9,6]. The image schemas are spatiotemporal relationships that have been suggested to be conceptual building blocks upon which metaphors and novel conceptual domains [5] can be built as well as event conceptualisations [14,1,17]. This means that image schemas play the role of the invisible part in many metaphors. In particular, in more abstract domains, image schemas can be the crucial information transfer that the metaphor makes [8]. While image schemas are relationships such as VERTICALITY, SCALE and SOURCE PATH GOAL, these skeletons can be fleshed out to metaphoric structures such as: UP is GOOD/DOWN is BAD, BIG is STRONG and PROGRESS is MOVE- MENT FORWARD [8]. In natural language, these kinds of statements are not usually directly used. Instead, it is a three level hierarchy in which the embodied experience transforms itself into human language and interpretation, see Figure 3. For instance, in the metaphor She s so high, high above me, she s so lovely made famous by Tal Bachman in the late 90 s, clearly, denotes that she is out of reach and that the singer will never be on her level. All these metaphors point the reader to the metaphor that UP is GOOD, which in turn builds on the image schema VERTICALITY. Another common metaphor is LIGHT is GOOD/DARK is BAD which is highly grounded in the visual domain of embodied experience. As in the words by The Rolling Stones where the world is to be painted black to match the colour of the heart: I see a line of cars and they re all painted black, With flowers and my love, both never to come back. In terms of image schematic conceptual structures, in the literature there currently exists no direct mapping to dark and light, and SCALE is the most appropriate connection to make. It has been argued that human interpretation and conceptualisation of metaphors both in linguistics and in the arts are unconsciously processed on these kinds of conceptual structures and skeletons. What then happens if there is a violation of metaphoric structure? 2 See [4] for a breakdown of abstract financial language into image schematic PATH-following.

Expression Conceptual Metaphor Image Schema Figure 3. The three levels of the conceptual structure in image schematic conceptual metaphors. Figure 4 depicts Beneath the paint, an abstract painting. The painting embodies both the VERTICALITY image schema with long coloured strokes, and therefore allowing for the interpretation of the metaphor UP is GOOD/DOWN is BAD, while simultaneously capturing scaling from dark to light associated with the metaphor LIGHT is GOOD/DARK is BAD. As both metaphors have the structure X is GOOD/Y is BAD a conflict emerges as the painting forces the viewer to merge UP with DARK and DOWN with LIGHT. In consequence, UP becomes BAD and DARK becomes GOOD, and DOWN becomes GOOD and LIGHT becomes BAD. In language, these kinds of conflicts are often unconventional and provocative. In real life scenarios, these metaphors often rely on a temporal change. For instance, a sunrise is a perfect example of LIGHT going UP and a sunset of LIGHT go DOWN. Whether or not one could transfer this analysis to claim that a sunrise holds more conceptual positive tone than a sunset is, of course, a completely different matter. In order to learn more about the extent of how conceptual metaphors such as those present in Beneath the paint, are incorporated into human conceptualisation requires further studies. Regardless, using this kind of conceptual metaphor violation provides an interesting addition to different artistic areas to force the viewer to unconscious and unintuitive interpretations that range out from the conventional sphere.

Figure 4. Beneath the paint

References [1] Tarek R. Besold, Maria M. Hedblom, and Oliver Kutz. A narrative in three acts: Using combinations of image schemas to model events. Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures, 19:10 20, 2017. [2] Vittorio Gallese and George Lakoff. The brain s concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive neuropsychology, 22(3):455 79, 2005. [3] Howard Gardner. The Mind s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution. Basic Books, Inc., New York, NY, USA, 1985. [4] Dagmar Gromann and Maria M. Hedblom. Breaking Down Finance: A method for concept simplification by identifying movement structures from the image schema Path-following. In Proceedings of the Second Joint Ontology Workshops (JOWO), volume 1660, Annecy, France, 2016. CEUR-WS online proceedings. [5] Maria M. Hedblom, Oliver Kutz, and Fabian Neuhaus. Image schemas in computational conceptual blending. Cognitive Systems Research, 39:42 57, 2016. [6] Mark Johnson. The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago and London, 1987. [7] K. Koffka. Principles of gestalt psychology. International library of psychology, philosophy, and scientific method. Harcourt, Brace and Company, 1935. [8] Zoltán Kövecses. Metaphor: A Practical Introduction. Oxford University Press, USA, 2010. [9] George Lakoff. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things. What Categories Reveal about the Mind. The University of Chicago Press, 1987. [10] George Lakoff and Mark Johnson. Metaphors We Live By. University of Chicago Press, 1980. [11] George Lakoff and Mark Johnson. Philosophy In The Flesh. Basic Books, 1999. [12] Jean M. Mandler. The Foundations of Mind : Origins of Conceptual Thought: Origins of Conceptual Though. Oxford University Press, New York, 2004. [13] Mark P. Mattson. Superior pattern processing is the essence of the evolved human brain. Brain and Language, 8:1 17, 2014. Frontiers in Neuroscience. [14] Todd Oakley. Image schema. In Dirk Geeraerts and Hubert Cuyckens, editors, The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics, pages 214 235. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2010. [15] Charles Kay Ogden and Ivor Armstrong Richards. International library of psychology, philosophy and scientific method. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 8 edition, 1989. [16] Lawrence Shapiro. Embodied Cognition. New problems of philosophy. Routledge, London and New York, 2011. [17] Robert St. Amant, Clayton T. Morrison, Yu-Han Chang, Paul R. Cohen, and Carole Beal. An image schema language. In International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (ICCM), pages 292 297, 2006. [18] Marco Tettamanti, Giovanni Buccino, Maria Cristina Saccuman, Vittorio Gallese, Massimo Danna, Paola Scifo, Ferruccio Fazio, Giacomo Rizzolatti, and Daniela Perani. Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, pages 273 281, 2005.