Prenasalisation and melodic complexity *

Similar documents
Vowel sets: a reply to Kaye 1

Sonority as a Primitive: Evidence from Phonological Inventories

Sonority as a Primitive: Evidence from Phonological Inventories Ivy Hauser University of North Carolina

LINGUISTICS 321 Lecture #8. BETWEEN THE SEGMENT AND THE SYLLABLE (Part 2) 4. SYLLABLE-TEMPLATES AND THE SONORITY HIERARCHY

LING 202 Lecture outline W Sept 5. Today s topics: Types of sound change Expressing sound changes Change as misperception

Myanmar (Burmese) Plosives

Week 6 - Consonants Mark Huckvale

SOLE Word stylesheet Guidelines for the proceedings of ConSOLE. SOLE Editorial Board

CHAPTER 1 CLUSTER PHONOTACTICS AND THE SONORITY SEQUENCING PRINCIPLE. organized into well-formed sequences according to universal principles of

Phonology. Submission of papers

Sonority restricts laryngealized plosives in Southern Aymara

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

1.0 Reconstruction or the Proto-Germanic Obstruent Inventory 1.1 Vennemann's Approach to Internal Reconstruction or Proto-Germanic

What do our appreciation of tonal music and tea roses, our acquisition of the concepts

Bas C. van Fraassen, Scientific Representation: Paradoxes of Perspective, Oxford University Press, 2008.

The sonority hierarchy and NO LONG VOWEL: Theoretical implications

Language Documentation and Linguistic Theory STYLE SHEET Department of Linguistics, SOAS

The Style Sheet for Gengo Kenkyu, Journal of the Linguistic Society of Japan

BACHELOR'S DEGREE PROGRAMME Term-End Examination CirD-7E3 June, 2018 ELECTIVE COURSE : ENGLISH BEGE-102 : THE STRUCTURE OF MODERN ENGLISH

Social Semiotic Techniques of Sense Making using Activity Theory

Speaking in Minor and Major Keys

INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON ENGINEERING DESIGN ICED 05 MELBOURNE, AUGUST 15-18, 2005 GENERAL DESIGN THEORY AND GENETIC EPISTEMOLOGY

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

BACHELOR'S DEGREE PROGRAMME Term-End Examination December, 2014

The odds of eternal optimization in OT

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

South American Indians and the Conceptualization of Music

Advanced Phonetics and Phonology

Imperatives are existential modals; Deriving the must-reading as an Implicature. Despina Oikonomou (MIT)

Automated extraction of motivic patterns and application to the analysis of Debussy s Syrinx

Rhythm and Melody Aspects of Language and Music

VERBUM Analecta Neolatina s t y l e s h e e t

Para-Linguistic Mechanisms of Production in Human Beatboxing : a Real-time Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study

Reply to Stalnaker. Timothy Williamson. In Models and Reality, Robert Stalnaker responds to the tensions discerned in Modal Logic

Welcome to Vibrationdata

IMPLEMENTATION OF SIGNAL SPACING STANDARDS

Precise Digital Integration of Fast Analogue Signals using a 12-bit Oscilloscope

Methods of analysis for tonal text-setting. The case study of Fe Fe Bamileke

Permutations of the Octagon: An Aesthetic-Mathematical Dialectic

DIGITAL COMMUNICATION

The Reference Book, by John Hawthorne and David Manley. Oxford: Oxford University Press 2012, 280 pages. ISBN

Valuable Particulars

Note : Answer all questions.

Acoustic Prosodic Features In Sarcastic Utterances

288 ~lu~l~c 1,API, to set forth such questions of theoretical or practical character and the answers given to them.

Tempo and Beat Analysis

Foucault's Archaeological method

AUD 6306 Speech Science

An HPSG Account of Depictive Secondary Predicates and Free Adjuncts: A Problem for the Adjuncts-as-Complements Approach

UNIT III. Combinational Circuit- Block Diagram. Sequential Circuit- Block Diagram

Conclusion. One way of characterizing the project Kant undertakes in the Critique of Pure Reason is by

Crosslinguistic Notions of (In)definiteness *

MONOTONE AMAZEMENT RICK NOUWEN

Guidance For Scrambling Data Signals For EMC Compliance

Analysis of the effects of signal distance on spectrograms

Jokes and the Linguistic Mind. Debra Aarons. New York, New York: Routledge Pp. xi +272.

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Humanities Learning Outcomes

Hear hear. Århus, 11 January An acoustemological manifesto

Commentary on David Huron s On the Role of Embellishment Tones in the Perceptual Segregation of Concurrent Musical Parts

[1]. S" = main stress, S = secondary stress, s = unstressed. Proto-Germanic: S s s s s s S s s s s s s S s s. Pintupi: S s S s S s S s S s S s s S s s

English Consonants - how can we classify them? Phonetics and Phonology. English Consonants - how can we classify them?

The Nature of Time. Humberto R. Maturana. November 27, 1995.

1. Introduction NCMMSC2009

Computer-based sound spectrograph system

Sidestepping the holes of holism

Incommensurability and Partial Reference

General description. The Pilot ACE is a serial machine using mercury delay line storage

Book Review. John Dewey s Philosophy of Spirit, with the 1897 Lecture on Hegel. Jeff Jackson. 130 Education and Culture 29 (1) (2013):

Semester A, LT4223 Experimental Phonetics Written Report. An acoustic analysis of the Korean plosives produced by native speakers

Poznań, July Magdalena Zabielska

Computational Parsing of Melody (CPM): Interface Enhancing the Creative Process during the Production of Music

A MULTI-PARAMETRIC AND REDUNDANCY-FILTERING APPROACH TO PATTERN IDENTIFICATION

Edward Winters. Aesthetics and Architecture. London: Continuum, 2007, 179 pp. ISBN

hprints , version 1-1 Oct 2008

VLSI Technology used in Auto-Scan Delay Testing Design For Bench Mark Circuits

Physical Modelling of Musical Instruments Using Digital Waveguides: History, Theory, Practice

Speech and Speaker Recognition for the Command of an Industrial Robot

DELTA MODULATION AND DPCM CODING OF COLOR SIGNALS

Motion Video Compression

of illustrating ideas or explaining them rather than actually existing as the idea itself. To further their

Monadology and Music 2: Leibniz s Demon

foucault s archaeology science and transformation David Webb

Effective from the Session Department of English University of Kalyani

Image and Imagination

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

Expressive Singing Synthesis based on Unit Selection for the Singing Synthesis Challenge 2016

A repetition-based framework for lyric alignment in popular songs

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

Chapter 4. Logic Design

Quantify. The Subjective. PQM: A New Quantitative Tool for Evaluating Display Design Options

Computer Coordination With Popular Music: A New Research Agenda 1

NON-EXAMPLES AND PROOF BY CONTRADICTION

The Cognitive Nature of Metonymy and Its Implications for English Vocabulary Teaching

Interdepartmental Learning Outcomes

Spatial-frequency masking with briefly pulsed patterns

On the Analogy between Cognitive Representation and Truth

Sight and Sensibility: Evaluating Pictures Mind, Vol April 2008 Mind Association 2008

Musical Sound: A Mathematical Approach to Timbre

English Phonetics and Phonology. 1. Voiced and voiceless plosives. Voiced and voiceless plosives: Word-initial position

Transcription:

Prenasalisation and melodic complexity * KUNIYA NASUKAWA Abstract In the northern Tohoku dialect of Japanese (Kanai 1982), it has been assumed that the melodic structure of prenasalised plosives is more complex than that of voiced plosives. This paper questions that view, however, calling upon the argument that prenasalisation occurs only in truly voiced plosives in an intervocalic environment. The analysis refers to the principle of Licensing Inheritance (Harris 1994, 1997), and examines its influence on the headship status of the nasal prime (Nasukawa 1998). 1 Introduction In the northern Tohoku dialect of Japanese (henceforth NTJ), voiceless plosives in intervocalic position are subject to consonantal weakening or 'lenition' in the form of spirantisation and vocalisation. Within the context of Licensing Inheritance (Harris 1994, 1997), lenition results from a depletion of licensing potential in a prosodically weak position. In the same environment the prenasalisation of truly voiced plosives also occurs, this having been analysed (Kanai 1982) as a nasal insertion process in which the nasal phase of a prenasalised expression is inserted before voiced plosives occurring between vowels. The fact that these two processes lenition and prenasalisation share the same environment suggests the possibility that they can be unitied by a common analysis. Below I reject Kanai's approach, and instead, identify prenasalisation in NTJ as lenition: a particular property of voiced plosives fails to be licensed and the outcome is interpreted as prenasalised. The success of this analysis rests on the assumption that the melodic structure of prenasalised plosives is less complex than that of their voiced oral reflexes. My account is based on the notion of Licensing Inheritance, and refers directly to the influence of this principle on the headship status of the nasal prime. The paper is structured as follows. In 2 I describe prenasalisation in general, then focus on voiced plosive prenasalisation in NTJ ( 3.1) and outline an orthodox account of the process ( 3.2). Following that, 4 explores the neutralising mechanism of voiceless plosive spirantisation and vocalisation in the light of Licensing Inheritance. * This paper has benefited greatly from the comments and suggestions of Phillip Backley and John Harris.

54 Nasukawa Finally, in 5 I propose an intrasegmental representation of prenasalised plosives in the context of Element Theory (Kaye, Lowenstamm and Vergnaud 1985; Harris 1990, 1994, 1997; Harris & Lindsey 1995) and analyse prenasalisation as a case of lenition. This is achieved by adopting the idea of headship switching on the nasality/voicebearing prime. 2 Prenasalisation While the majority of languages do not use prenasalisation as a contrastive property, we find a concentration of language systems in Africa and the Pacific area that contain prenasalised segments. It has been claimed that prenasalised segments arise via one of two prenasalisation processes (Herbert 1986). In the first of these, the nasal phase of a prenasalised sound exists by virtue of nasality being lexically present in either the target of the process or its given environment. This is illustrated in the following examples (taken from Herbert 1986): (1) a. Tonga of Inhambane 1 (Herbert 1986: 74, cf. Lanham 1955): (N = nasal noun class) N + loy + i n doyi witch N + banyis + i m banyisi saviour M N + Fuyu guyu fig tree b. Fe?Fe?-Bamileke 2 (Herbert 1986: 47, cf. Hyman 1972): (N- = non-complete and consecutive) l N- + odm adm to accept M N- + Fdm fdm to go m N- + kdm cdm to say c. Ndali 3 (Piggott 1997: 449): in- + otmn h l atmn nose in- + stmxd h m ctmxd banana in- + jt m c` h M ft m c` dove The prefixes in (1a) and (1b) consist solely of nasality, while the Ndali prefix in (1c) 1 A Chopi language of the Narrow Bantu group (Niger-Kordofanian family). 2 A Nun-Bamileke language of the Bane group (Niger-Kordofanian family). 3 A Nyika-Safwa language of the Narrow Bantu group (Niger-Kordofanian family).

Prenasalisation and melodic complexity 55 contains nasality at its right edge. In each case, the nasality that is lexically specified on the prefix is manifested as prenasalisation after attaching to the stem. In the second type of prenasalisation process, an alternation between truly voiced plosives and prenasalised voiced plosives takes place in the absence of any obvious lexical source for nasality. This is observed in some dialects of Japanese, some Western Indonesian languages and in several Bantu languages. 4 (2) Northern Tohoku Japanese Bh + c`qtl` Bh m c`qtl` fire Dharma be covered with flames srtt + f`jjn srt M f`jjn middle school junior high school n + a``r` n l a`r` (polite prefix) grandmother grandmother (polite form) mhft + cy`mf` mhft m cy`mf`c meat potato (name of a dish) The NTJ examples 5 given in (2) illustrate the process of compounding in this dialect: if the first segment of the second member of a compound is a truly voiced obstruent, then this segment is interpreted as its prenasalised reflex. For prenasalisation to occur, the target of the process must be in an intervocalic environment. It is not necessary, however, for nasality to be lexically present in either the target or the environment. In the type of prenasalisation illustrated in (1), the source of the nasal phase in the output forms is clearly apparent. In the case of the phenomenon exemplified in (2), on the other hand, we fail to identify any obvious trigger for prenasalisation since, on the face of it, nasality is not present lexically. The remainder of this paper discusses this latter type of prenasalisation in TJ, and attempts to identify the source of nasality and the mechanism behind the process. 3 Northern Tohoku Japanese 3.1 The distribution of prenasalised segments The NTJ dialect is spoken in the northern part of the Tohoku area of Mainland Japan, 4 The status of the process varies from language to language: it is optional in Nea (Wurm 1972), some dialects of Malay (Hendon 1966) and many Melanesian languages (Ray 1926); but in Nambakaengo (Wurm 1972) and the northern Tohoku dialect of Japanese, it is obligatory. 5 Some exceptions exist. See Inoue (1967) and Iitoyo (1998) for discussion.

56 Nasukawa and is distinguished by the prenasalisation process shown in (2). This occurs only in an intervocalic environment, and targets only truly voiced oral plosives. The characteristics of this dynamic process are also reflected in the lexical distribution of prenasalised plosives: 6 (3) Tokyo Japanese NTJ a. j`fh key j`mfh r`f` destiny r`mf` hf`hf` thorny h M f`h M f` b. g`c`7 skin g`mc` j c` pipe, tube j M c` hcn well h M cn c. j`a turnip j`la jha` tusks jh l a` r`ah rust r`lah Comparing these Tokyo dialect forms in (3) with the corresponding NTJ forms, it can be seen that all intervocalic plosives may exhibit prenasalisation. In comparison, truly voiced oral plosives in either word-initial or foot-initial position (see Vance 1987), together with the voiceless cognates of plosives in intervocalic 6 It has been argued from both diachronic and synchronic perspectives that prenasalised plosives in NTJ are derived from the prenasalisation of truly voiced oral plosives (Inoue 1967, Ashworth 1976-77, Kanai 1982). 7 This dialect exhibits other alternations involving alveolar: voiced alveolar obstruents are prenasalised and interpreted as prenasalised affricates before high vowels: lhcy êv`sdqë c lv m j cyi lottery j m hc(regional) Not only in cases involving prenasalisation but also in all other cases, alveolar obstruents become affricates when they are followed by high vowels: s lhjh êaricksë c lhjh j`s n êbonitoë c j` o This kind of alternation can be analysed as local place assimilation, but it is beyond the scope of the present discussion to pursue its mechanism here.

position, do not undergo the process. This is illustrated below. 8 (4) Tokyo Japanese NTJ Prenasalisation and melodic complexity 57 a. f`jjnn school f`jjnn M ) f`jjn c`qtl` Dharma c`q l` ccm ) c`q l` aaku tapir aaf cl ) aak b. j`jh persimmon j`fh )cj`mfh )cj`mjh r`j` slope r`f` )cr`mf` )cr`mj` g`s` flag g`œ` )cg`mc` )cg`ms` j`s` shoulder j`œ` )cj`mc` )cj`ms` As shown in (4a), word-initial position fails to support the prenasalisation of truly voiced oral plosives. Furthermore, the process may only target voiced plosives: the alternation in (4b) shows that intervocalic voiceless plosives never become prenasalised. Instead, the voiceless stops are potential targets for vocalisation. 9 Interestingly, the neighbouring dialect of Southern Tohoku Japanese also exhibits voiceless plosive vocalisation in the (4b) environment. In the Southern system, however, it is truly voiced cognates, as well as voiceless plosives, that undergo the process. The examples in (5) show the neutralisation of voiced and voiceless plosives in the Southern dialect. (5) Tokyo Japanese Southern Tohoku Japanese a. j`jh persimmon j`fh )cj`mfhcc )cj`mjh j`fh key j`fh )cj`mfhcc )cj`mjh b. g`s` flag g`œ` )cg`mc`cc )cg`ms` g`c` skin g`œ` )cj`mc`cc )cj`ms` 8 In the consonantal inventory of modern Japanese (including the dialect in question), the distribution of the voiceless bilabial plosive is lexically restricted: it can appear only as a full geminate (e.g. kappa water imp ) or as the second part of a NC sequence (sampo stroll ). Therefore, it is never found in the intervocalic environment in (4b). 9 The intervocalic reflexes of voiceless plosives in the Tohoku dialect in general have been regarded as voiced counterparts f and c (Inoue 1968, Kanai 1982). However, my own research shows that the reflexes are not f and c, but the voiced fricative F and voiced lateral tap Œ respectively.

58 Nasukawa 3.2 Nasal Insertion The literature provides several accounts of the voiced plosive prenasalisation effect illustrated above (Inoue 1968, Muraki 1970, Iitoyo 1998). The generative approach adopted by Kanai (1982) uses the following rewrite-rule in his analysis: (6) NASAL INSERTION (Kanai 1982) i [m/m] / V [c/f]v The structural change in (6) takes place exclusively before intervocalic voiced plosives, where the resulting sequence an inserted nasal and a lexically given truly voiced oral plosive is analysed as a prenasalised plosive. This treatment may be viewed as voiced plosive fortition. However, Kanai's analysis ignores the case of prenasalisation on a voiced bilabial plosive a, which is targeted in the same way as c and f. Moreover, the rule in (6) does not overtly address the two fundamental issues of (i) why the process occurs only in an intervocalic environment, and (ii) why the process affects only truly voiced oral plosives. The NASAL INSERTION rule in (6), as with other types of rewrite rules, provides no formal link between a process (nasal insertion) and the context in which it occurs (the position immediately preceding an intervocalic voiced plosive). The arbitrariness of this analysis stems from the very nature of a rule-oriented approach, where the output of the rule and its environment stand as independent entities. In what follows I shall develop a non-arbitrary account of why prenasalisation affects only intervocalic b, d and g. 4 Intervocalic sites as contexts favouring lenition In developing an alternative analysis of prenasalisation in NTJ, I begin by examining the context where the phenomenon takes place. As already noted in 3, the process is observed only in intervocalic position a site well-known to favour lenition (Lass 1984; Harris 1994, 1997; Harris & Lindsey 1995): Spanish, Ibibio, English, Korean and many other languages exhibit this kind of phonological process. The following examples are taken from Harris (1997). (7) a. Ibibio spirantisation cho hide diad hide oneself euj cover eufn cover oneself b. English t-tapping pi[ ]y In Ibibio, o and j weaken intervocalically to A and F respectively; and in the same

Prenasalisation and melodic complexity 59 context a number of English systems show t-tapping. In addressing the question of why phenomena of this type are particularly prevalent in intervocalic position, Harris (1994, 1997) draws on the notion of licensing a head-dependent relation that controls all aspects of phonological architecture in accordance with (8): (8) PHONOLOGICAL LICENSING PRINCIPLE (Kaye 1990) Within a domain, all phonological units must be licensed save one, the head of that domain. Phonological licensing manifests itself in one of two guises: p[rosodic]-licensing defines a licensing relation established between two positions in prosodic structure, while a[utosegmental]-licensing describes the licensing relation between a melodic unit and a prosodic position. The following structure illustrates these two kinds of licensing relation: 10 (9) 9 p-licensing (Foot-level) 9 9 p-licensing (Inter-constiuent-level) [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 a-licensing C V C V In (9), x 2 is a nucleus and the ultimate head of the p-licensors in the domain. x 1 (its preceding onset) and x 4 (another nucleus in the domain) are directly p-licensed by x 2 at the inter-constituent and the foot level respectively, whereas the onset x 3 is indirectly p-licensed by x 2 through x 4 at an inter-constituent level. 11 Differences in the architecture of the p-licensing paths shown in (9) are mirrored by differences in a- licensing potential. A position located relatively low down a licensing path will enjoy less distributional freedom than one located higher up. A direct implication for consonant distribution is that a greater number of contrasts may be supported by x 1 than by x 3, owing to differing amounts of a-licensing potential inherited by a p-licensee from its p-licensor. The following formulation defines this asymmetric relation: (10) LICENSING INHERITANCE (Harris 1992, 1994, 1997) A licensed position inherits its a-licensing potential from its licensor. 10 Following Harris (1997), Xs are used instead of O(nset)-N(ucleus) for the sake of convenience. 11 Thus an onset is universally licensed by its following nucleus. For details, see the definition of Onset Licensing in Harris (1994: 160).

60 Nasukawa The a-licensing potential of a position may be equated with the capacity of that position to support melodic oppositions. (10) requires that, whenever two positions enter into a p-licensing relation, the p-licensed position must gain a-licensing potential (indirectly) from its p-licensor. The a-licensing potential inherited from the p-licensed position is, however, less than that possessed by its p-licensor, since, each time a position is p-licensed, the stock of a-licensing potential is depleted. As a result, this difference in the amount of a-licensing potential possessed by a p-licensor and its p- licensee is reflected in the unequal capacity of each position to support units of melodic structure. Specifically, the melodic structure of a p-licensee can be no more complex than that of its p-licenser. This means that lenited consonants such as those in (7) may be identified as segments occupying the lowest positions on the licensing path in a given domain. An example from (7a) demonstrates this: (11) 9 9 9 [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 cccccchccccaccccd Following the principles of PROSODIC LICENSING and LICENSING INHERITANCE, the representation in (11) shows why lenition is favoured in a foot-internal environment. A prosodically weak position is able to support a relatively small set of lexical contrasts hence, we find a tendency for neutralisation to occur in this context. As the examples in (4b) have shown, the process of voiceless plosive vocalisation in NTJ operates in a similar environment a foot-internal non-nuclear position. (12) Lenition of voiceless plosives in Nothern Tohoku Japanese 9 9 9 [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 jcccc `ccccf ccch The southern Tohoku (STJ) dialect also exhibits lenition in this environment. In this system, however, it is voiced plosives, as well as their voiced cognates, that are subject to the process: (13) Neutralisation of voiceless and voiced plosives in STJ j`fh (cf. Tokyo Japanese: kaki)

Prenasalisation and melodic complexity 61 9 9 9 [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 jcccc`cccccf ccch To account for lenition of this type we can adopt an approach similar to that taken in the case of (11), the one difference being that lexically voiced and voiceless plosives lose their voicing contrast in this particular context. These facts indicate that the prenasalisation observed in NTJ is similarly a consequence of lenition in foot-internal weak positions. (14) Voiced plosive prenasalisation in NTJ 9 9 9 [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 cjcccc`c N f c H In accordance with Licensing Inheritance, we can expect the melodic structure of x 3 in (14) to be less complex than that of x 1, because a position relatively low down the licensing path inherits a reduced amount of a-licensing potential. This result is inconsistent with Kanai s analysis, in which prenasalised plosives are treated as being more complex than voiced plosives. If we accept the analysis in (14), then two questions must be addressed. First, what is the source of nasality in prenasalisation? Second, is it possible to analyse prenasalised plosives as being less complex than voiced plosives? To answer these questions, we must identify the melodic organisation of the relevant segments. 5 Prenasalisation as lenition 5.1 Melodic primes and prime suppression Phonologists are united in the assumption that segments can be decomposed into smaller, indivisible units or primes, although some degree of variation in the nature and the identity of these units can be observed between different theoretical frameworks. In this paper I adopt the set of privative primes known as elements (Kaye, Lowenstamm & Vergnaud 1985, 1990; Harris 1994, Harris & Lindsey 1995),

62 Nasukawa which are privative, 12 independently interpretable and redundancy-free. Those elements pertinent to the present discussion are given below with specifications of their acoustic signal mapping: (15) ELEMENTS ACOUSTIC PATTERN [U] Rump: low spectral peak [@] Neutral: no salient spectral peak [R] Rise: high spectral peak [?] Edge: abrupt and sustained drop in overall amplitude [h] Noise: aperiodic energy [N] Murmur: low broad resonance peak [L] Slack : F0 down Employing the elements given above, foot-internal lenition is illustrated in the following schemata (Harris 1997): (16) a. Labial plosive vocalisation b A [U,?, h] [U, <?>, <h>] b. Velar plosive vocalisation k F [@,?, h] [@, <?>, <h>] c. t-tapping t 3 [R,?, h] [R, <?>, <h>] The vocalisation process is represented in (16) as the suppression of [?] and [h]; the remaining elements [U], [@] and [R] then define a labial approximant, a velar approximant and a tap respectively. Each instance of element suppression is a reflection of the reduced a-licensing potential available in the prosodically weak footinternal position. As (17) shows, vocalisation in both NTJ and STJ receives a parallel analysis. Note that the remaining [R] element in (17b) is interpreted in isolation as an alveolar lateral tap. (17) a. Velar plosive vocalisation ka k i ka Fccch [@,?, h] [@] 12 Recent studies have shown how a commitment to privativeness can contribute significantly to a reduction in the generative capacity of a system of phonological contrasts.

b. t-lateral tapping ha t ` ha Π` [R,?, h] [R] 5.2 The melodic representation of prenasalised plosives Before proceeding to a discussion of voiced plosive prenasalisation as lenition, let us first consider the intrasegmental structure of prenasalised plosives. Within the framework of Feature Geometry, Sagey (1986) proposes the following representation for prenasalised plosives: (18) The two [nasal]s (each with a different value) are assumed to be ordered sequentially under the Soft-palate node: [+nasal] precedes [-nasal]. As Lombardi (1990) and Schafer (1995) claim in their discussion of the structure of affricates, this type of representation predicts the possibility of finding the reverse sequence where [-nasal] precedes [+nasal]. However, this prediction is not borne out by phonological evidence (van der Weijer 1994, Rennison 1998). 13 In frameworks employing privative primes (Mester 1986, McCarthy 1988, Kaye, Lowenstamm & Vergnaud 1990, Steriade 1993, Harris 1994, Lombardi 1995), contour segments are also represented by a configuration in which one prime precedes another within the scope of a single position or root node. This is true of Element Theory, where the structure of a prenasalised alveolar plosive can be given as (19a). Clearly, an alternative representation where [?], [h] and [R] precede [N] is also a grammatical possibility, although this fails to find any empirical support. 13 Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996) claim the existence, at least in phonetic terms, of postnasalised plosives.

64 Nasukawa (19) a. b. c. Further criticism of (19a) is expressed in Takahashi (1993), where it is noted that this structure is unable to encode any chronological distinction between the phonetic manifestation of the left melodic branch and that of the right branch. This follows from the generalisation that the sequential ordering of melodic units is determined exclusively by the prosodic structure, and manifest at the skeletal level. Any specific grouping of elements at the melodic level is rendered insignificant, under the assumption that each prime behaves autonomously and resides on its own autosegmental plane (Goldsmith 1976, 1990; Kaye, Lowenstamm & Vergnaud 1985). For this reason, the respective interpretations of the three structures in (19) will be identical. If intrasegmental components were to encode timing properties, then a great number of unattested contour expressions would inevitably be generated such as the segments in (19b) and (19c) that carry more than two timing differences. Overgeneration of this sort would have immediate repercussions for generative restrictiveness. Within a cognitive model of speech sounds, it should be expected that phonological representations accommodate only information that is lexically contrastive. Accordingly, the timing difference observed in the phonetic interpretation of prenasalised plosives should be deemed phonologically irrelevant for two reasons: first, the difference can never create a phonological opposition; and second, the two supposedly phonetic phases of a prenasalised stop always behave as a single segment for contrastive purposes. This would appear to provide ample justification for treating timing differences of this kind as a product of the Articulatory-Perceptual system, and I therefore omit these details from the representations of prenasalised plosives to be given below. 14 Let us assume that the voiced alveolar plosive in (20a) comprises the components of a plain alveolar plosive ([R,?, h]) and a voicing-bearing element ([L]) (Brockhaus 1992). The internal structure of an alveolar nasal (20b), on the other hand, is expressed 14 For similar reasons, Rennison (1998) proposes a melodic representation for prenasalised alveolar plosives that introduces a floating or 'lazy' element which contributes to the phonetic manifestation of the second part of the prenasalised segment. With the option of applying 'floating' status to any element, however, we immediately re-introduce overgeneration problems of the sort already noted (cf. the impossibility of expressions such as s t and f p ).

Prenasalisation and melodic complexity 65 by the set [R], [?] and [N] (the nasal element) (Harris 1990, 1994). Turning to the prenasalised stop n d in (20c), I tentatively assume that this consists of all the elements found in (20a) and (20b), a proposal based on the fact that n d is the derived reflex of d in voiced plosive prenasalisation in NTJ. (20) Provisional representations of d, n and n d m a. d b. m c. c [R,?, h, L] [R,?, N] [R,?, h, L, N] The representations in (20) successfully establish the necessary phonological contrasts: the distinction between d and m c refers to [N]; and m c is distinguished from n by the presence of [h] and [L]. However, these melodic structures do not square with the analysis given in 4. Recall that lexically given voiced plosives are neutralised in foot-internal position, this environment receiving only a reduced amount of licensing potential that is insufficient to a-license all the lexically specified elements. As discussed in 4, this leads us to propose that the melodic structure of m c must be less complex than that of c. Yet, by adopting the representation in (20c) for a prenasalised plosive, we fail to uphold the account of neutralisation already given. As already mentioned in 4, another problem is the appearance of [N] during the course of the process. If m c has the representation in (20c), then the appearance of [N] during the derivation from c remains unexplained. The arbitrariness of this account sheds little light on the question of why the prenasalisation process only targets voiced plosives. In order to establish an alternative representation for prenasalised plosives, let us focus on the fact that the prenasalisation process never affects voiceless plosives, but only voiced plosives in intervocalic position. What this indicates is that the correlation between voicing and nasality [L] and [N], in elemental terms must be a key factor in identifying the representation of prenasalised plosives. Evidence for a strong correlation between voicing and nasality is not restricted to the process in question, but is also to be found in postnasal voicing assimilation, Yamato-Japanese Rendaku (Itô et al 1995, Nasukawa 1998) and Dahl s Law (Armstrong 1967, Davy & Nurse 1982). In order to capture the relation between [L] and [N], Nasukawa (1998) proposes that the two primes are in fact the same object (see also Ploch 1995), with the difference between voicing and nasality being captured by recourse to the notion of headship: the headed element [N] contributes voicing, while its headless counterpart manifests itself as nasality (the underscore denotes headedness).

66 Nasukawa (21) MURMUR ELEMENT HEADSHIP STATUS PHONETIC INTERPRETATION [N] non-headed nasality [N] headed true voicing In the earliest models of Dependency Phonology (Anderson & Jones 1974), melodic headedness had been developed chiefly to encode vocalic oppositions. Making use of these initial ideas, Harris & Lindsey (1995) describe languages exhibiting an ATR contrast using the same structural property if [U] is headed, for example, then it is interpreted as tense t; on the other hand, if [U] is headless, then it is interpreted as its lax counterpart T. Following (21), I propose the melodic representations in (22): (22) a. d b. n c. m c [R,?, h, N] [R,?, N] [R,?, h, N] In these representations, m c is distinguished from d by the absence of headedness on [N], and the same sound is differentiated from n by the presence of [h] (noise). 5.3 Headedness and melodic complexity Given the representations in (22), voiceless plosive vocalisation in NTJ consists in the suppression of [?] and [h] see 5.1 above. Position X 3 in (23) is prosodically weak and does not receive enough a-licensing potential to sustain [?] and [h]. (23) a. Voiceless velar plosive vocalisation 9 9 9 [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 k a [@] i <[?]> <[h]> F

Prenasalisation and melodic complexity 67 b. Voiceless alveolar plosive vocalisation (t-lateral tapping) 9 9 9 [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 h a [R] a <[?]> <[h]> ΠVoiced plosive prenasalisation is also triggered by insufficient a-licensing potential in the same prosodically weak position; in this instance, the headed [N] of the voiced plosive d loses its headed status: (24) Voiced plosive prenasalisation 9 9 9 [X 1 ] O [X 2 ] N [X 3 ] O [X 4 ] N 9 9 9 9 g c` [R] ` cccccccccccccccccc[?] [h] [N] n d In the case of NTJ in contrast to the southern dialect discussed in 4 I propose that the headship properties of [N], in addition to the melodic primes [?] and [h], are sensitive to the quantity of a-licensing potential available: headedness becomes a target for suppression in prosodically weak positions. In the literature on Element theory, headedness properties have not generally been assumed to contribute to melodic complexity. However, Backley & Takahashi (1998) have proposed a model of Element Activation in which headship properties are encoded in a structurally dynamic way using element tier complements: a headed version of a melodic prime ["] corresponds to an active tier complement, while a headless element ["] translates into the same element tier configuration with an inactive complement. In this approach, tier complements are treated as melodic units on a par with elements that is, equally susceptible to effects such as melodic suppression. This account of voiced plosive prenasalisation and voiceless plosive vocalisation allows us to establish a unified analysis in stark contrast to the orthodox analyses discussed in 3 and 4, where these phenomena are treated as two unconnected processes, lenition and fortition. Another advantage of the representation in (24) is that

68 Nasukawa it captures the fact that voiceless segments, since they lack [N], are not subject to prenasalisation in a weak context. Voiceless plosive prenasalisation can be observed only if an adjacent morpheme or position possesses [N] in its melodic structure and triggers a fortition process (see 2). This contrasts with Kanai s rule-based analysis, which predicts not only voiceless plosive prenasalisation but also other kinds of prenasalisation, even if no nasal category is lexically specified. As already mentioned in 3.2, this kind of rule-oriented analysis can never establish any formal link between a process and the context where it occurs. 6 Summary In this paper, I have discussed the mechanism of voiced plosive prenasalisation in NTJ, and developed an analysis which treats the process as an instance of lenition. In accordance with Licensing Inheritance (Harris 1994, 1997), lenition is viewed as a reduction in melodic complexity occurring in prosodically weak sites. In my analysis, this reduction takes place via two mechanisms: the suppression of elements and the loss of melodic headedness. In NTJ, element suppression is found in voiceless plosive vocalisation, while the loss of headed status accounts for voiced plosive prenasalisation. References Armstrong, Lilias E. (1967). The phonetic and tonal structure of Kikuyu. London: Dawsons of Pall Mall. Ashworth, David. (1976-77). Historical perspective on voicing and the gerund in Japanese. Papers in Japanese Linguistics 5, 27-39. Backley, Phillip & Toyomi Takahashi (1998). Element activation. In Eugeniusz Cyran (ed.), Structure and interpretation: studies in phonology. 13-40. Lublin: Folium. Brockhaus, Wiebke G. (1992). Final devoicing: principles and parameters. PhD dissertation, University College London. Chomsky, Noam (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Davy, J. I. M. & Derek Nurse (1982). Synchronic versions of Dahl's Law: The multiple application of a phonological dissimilation rule. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 4, 157-195. Goldsmith, John A. (1976). Autosegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT. Published 1979, New York: Garland. Goldsmith, John A. (1990). Autosegmental and metrical phonology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Harris, John (1990). Segmental complexity and phonological government. Phonology 7. 255-300. Harris, John (1994). English sound structure. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Harris, John (1997). Licensing Inheritance: an integrated theory of neutralisation. Phonology 14. 315-370. Harris, John & Geoff Lindsey (1995). The elements of phonological representation. In Jacques Durand & Francis Katamba (eds), New frontiers in phonology. 34-79. Harlow, Essex: Longman.

Prenasalisation and melodic complexity 69 Hendon, Rufus S. (1966). The phonology and morphology of Ulu Muar Malay. Yale University publications in anthropology 70. New Haven. Herbert, Robert K. (1986). Language universals, markedness theory, and natural phonetic processes. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Hyman,Larry M. (1972). A phonological study of Fe?-Fe? Bamileke. SAL Supplement 4. Iitoyo, Kiichi (1998). Nihon hoogen kenkyuu no kadai. Tokyo: Kokushokankookai. Inoue, Humio (1967). Consonant system of the Tohoku-dialect. Gengo Kenkyu 52. 80-98. Itô, Junko, Ralf-Armin Mester & Jaye Padgett (1995). NC: Licensing and underspecification in Optimality Theory. Linguistic Inquiry 26. 571-613. Kanai, Yoshimitsu (1982). Morphophonemic-allophonic principle. Linguistic Inquiry 13. 320-323. Kaye, Jonathan D. (1990). Coda licensing. Phonology 7. 301-330. Kaye, Jonathan D., Jean Lowenstamm & Jean-Roger Vergnaud (1985). The internal structure of phonological elements: a theory of charm and government. Phonology Yearbook 2. 305-328. Kaye, Jonathan D., Jean Lowenstamm & Jean-Roger Vergnaud (1990). Constituent structure and government in phonology. Phonology 7. 193-231. Ladefoged, Peter & Ian Maddieson (1996). The sounds of the world s languages. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Lanham, L.W. (1955). A study of Gitonga of Inhambane. Johannesburg: Witwatersrand University Press. Lass, Roger (1984). Phonology: an introduction to basic concepts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lombardi, Linda (1990). The nonlinear organisation of the affricate. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 8. 375-425. McCarthy, John J. (1988). Feature geometry and dependency: a review. Phonetica 45. 84-108. Mester, Ralf-Armin (1986). Studies in tier structure. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Muraki, M. (1970). A sound change in a Tohoku dialect. Papers in Linguistics 3. 341-347. Nasukawa, Kuniya (1998). An integrated approach to nasality and voicing. In Eugeniusz Cyran (ed), Structure and interpretation: studies in phonology. 205-225. Lublin: Folium. Piggot, Glyne L. (1997). Licensing and alignment: a conspiracy in harmony. Phonology 14. 437-477. Ploch, Stefan (1995). French nasal vowels a first approach. SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics & Phonetics 5. 91-106. Ray, Sidney H. (1926). A comparative study of the Melanesian Islands languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rennison, John R. (1998). Contour segments without subsegmental structures. In Eugeniusz Cyran (ed), Structure and interpretation: studies in phonology. 227-245. Lublin: Folium. Sagey, Elizabeth (1986). The representation of features and relations in non-linear phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT. Schafer, Robin (1995). Headedness in the representation of affricates. The Linguistic Review 12. 61-87. Steriade, Donca (1993). Closure, release, and nasal contours. In Marie K. Huffman & Rena A. Krakow (eds.) Nasals, nasalisation, and the velum. Orlando: Academic Press. 401-470. Takahashi, Toyomi (1993). Contour in melodic structure. Ms. University College London. Vance, Timothy J. (1987). An introduction to Japanese phonology. New York: SUNY Press. Weijer, J. M. van de (1994). Segmental structure and complex segments. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.

70 Nasukawa Wurm, Stephen A. (1972). Remarks on lexical similarities between Reef Islands-Santa Cruz, and New Caledonian languages. In L. Bernot and J. Thomas (eds.) Langues et techniques: nature et société 1, 261-266. Paris: Editions Klincksieck.