Jokes and the Linguistic Mind. Debra Aarons. New York, New York: Routledge Pp. xi +272.

Similar documents
JOKES AND THE LINGUISTIC MIND PDF

PAT GUSTIN HOW NOT TO GET LOST IN TRANSLATION

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. (2002: 18) said that pragmatics concerned with people s ability to use language

Essential Aspects of Academic Practice (EAAP)

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. Jocular register must have its characteristics and differences from other forms

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. humorous condition. Sometimes visual and audio effect can cause people to laugh

Cooperative Principles of Indonesian Stand-up Comedy

Lecture 18: Production & Perception

Adisa Imamović University of Tuzla

STYLISTIC ANALYSIS OF MAYA ANGELOU S EQUALITY

Arab Academy for Science, Technology, & Maritime Transport (AASTMT), Egypt

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

DELIA CHIARO Verbally Expressed Humour on Screen: Reflections on Translation and Reception

Toward Computational Recognition of Humorous Intent

Rhetorical question in political speeches

DEGREE IN ENGLISH STUDIES. SUBJECT CONTENTS.

Communication Mechanism of Ironic Discourse

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

SIGNS, SYMBOLS, AND MEANING DANIEL K. STEWMT*

A critical pragmatic approach to irony

Interdepartmental Learning Outcomes

Today we are going to look at techniques to revise and polish technical manuscripts.

A Brief Introduction to Stylistics. By:Dr.K.T.KHADER

Rhetorical Analysis Terms and Definitions Term Definition Example allegory

Pragmatics - The Contribution of Context to Meaning

Pragmatics: How do we speak appropriately and politely?

for Using School to Home Reading for Preschool, Kindergarten, and Primary Children

Effective from the Session Department of English University of Kalyani

An Introduction to Korean Linguistics

Architecture is epistemologically

Chapter III. Research Methodology. A. Research Design. constructed and holistically as stated by Lincoln & Guba (1985).

An Analysis of Puns in The Big Bang Theory Based on Conceptual Blending Theory

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

Monty Python WRITING

Correlation to Common Core State Standards Books A-F for Grade 5

FRENCH IMMERSION LANGUAGE ARTS (FILA) French-Language Film and Literary Studies 12 (4 credits)

FRENCH IMMERSION LANGUAGE ARTS (FILA) French-Language Film and Literary Studies 11 (4 credits)

Non-Reducibility with Knowledge wh: Experimental Investigations

Humanities Learning Outcomes

PUBLIC SCHOOLS OF EDISON TOWNSHIP DIVISION OF CURRICULUM AND INSTRUCTION. Chamber Choir/A Cappella Choir/Concert Choir

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

CHAPTER II REVIEW RELATED LITERATURE. This chapter consisted of many important aspects in analysis the data. The

Curriculum Map: Academic English 10 Meadville Area Senior High School

ILAR Grade 7. September. Reading

Adam: And lastly we had the fourth tone which was a falling tone.

8 Reportage Reportage is one of the oldest techniques used in drama. In the millenia of the history of drama, epochs can be found where the use of thi

Communication Studies Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

Judicial Writing Manual: A Pocket Guide for Judges

The Cognitive Nature of Metonymy and Its Implications for English Vocabulary Teaching

College and Career Readiness Anchor Standards K-12 Montana Common Core Reading Standards (CCRA.R)

Pragmatically Computationally Difficult Pragmatics to Recognize Humour

Sentence Processing III. LIGN 170, Lecture 8

A Relevance-Theoretic Study of Poetic Metaphor. YANG Ting, LIU Feng-guang. Dalian University of Foreign Languages, Dalian, China

Revising Technical Manuscripts, Celia M. Elliott. 12 May 2014

What is Character? David Braun. University of Rochester. In "Demonstratives", David Kaplan argues that indexicals and other expressions have a

CHAPTER 2 THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK

Conference Interpreting Explained

Introduction to English Linguistics (I) Professor Seongha Rhee

On Meaning. language to establish several definitions. We then examine the theories of meaning

LEARN ANCIENT GREEK (GREEK AND LATIN LANGUAGE) BY PETER JONES DOWNLOAD EBOOK : LEARN ANCIENT GREEK (GREEK AND LATIN LANGUAGE) BY PETER JONES PDF

By Tetsushi Hirano. PHENOMENOLOGY at the University College of Dublin on June 21 st 2013)

Learning and Teaching English through the Bible: A Pictorial Approach BIBLE STUDY WORKBOOK PROSE

The pattern of all patience Adaptations of Shakespeare s King Lear from Nahum Tate to Howard Barker

Grade K Book Reviews Mini-Lessons at a Glance

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. language such as in a play or a film. Meanwhile the written dialogue is a dialogue

CASAS Content Standards for Reading by Instructional Level

Sight and Sensibility: Evaluating Pictures Mind, Vol April 2008 Mind Association 2008

A Discourse Analysis Study of Comic Words in the American and British Sitcoms

MONOTONE AMAZEMENT RICK NOUWEN

Formalizing Irony with Doxastic Logic

FIGURATIVE LANGUAGE USED IN OWL CITY S ALBUMS: A PRAGMATICS PERSPECTIVE

А. A BRIEF OVERVIEW ON TRANSLATION THEORY

Gestalt, Perception and Literature

Poetry 1 - Ning Nang Nong

Acoustic Prosodic Features In Sarcastic Utterances

FACOLTÀ DI STUDI UMANISTICI Lingue e culture per la mediazione linguistica. Traduzione LESSON 4. Prof.ssa Olga Denti a.a.

APA Style Guidelines

The Reference Book, by John Hawthorne and David Manley. Oxford: Oxford University Press 2012, 280 pages. ISBN

Metaphors: Concept-Family in Context

Peircean concept of sign. How many concepts of normative sign are needed. How to clarify the meaning of the Peircean concept of sign?

Confessions Of A Public Speaker PDF

Spring Term 2009; Teaching Arapaho Through ASLA Facilitator Comments on Class Instruction

07/03/2015. Jakobson s model of verbal communication. Michela Giordano

The Polish Peasant in Europe and America. W. I. Thomas and Florian Znaniecki

THE CO-OPERATIVE PRINCIPLE AND IMPLICATURE

Unit of Work: ROFL Year: 6 Term: 4

Abstracts workshops RaAM 2015 seminar, June, Leiden

History Admissions Assessment Specimen Paper Section 1: explained answers

ISTINYE UNIVERSITY FACULTY OF ARTS AND SCIENCES. DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH LANGUAGE and LITERATURE COURSE DESCRIPTIONS

Second Grade Art Curriculum

Vagueness & Pragmatics

Agreed key principles, observation questions and Ofsted grade descriptors for formal learning

Irony as Cognitive Deviation

WINONA STATE UNIVERSITY PROPOSAL FOR GENERAL EDUCATION PROGRAM COURSES

GCSE MUSIC. The GCSE is 60% Coursework and 40% Examination.

HOW TO DEFINE AND READ POETRY. Professor Caroline S. Brooks English 1102

The unit focuses on features of personal record writing. Pupils read a range of biographical and autobiographical texts and write a short biography.

Story Title: Miss Alaineus A Vocabulary Disaster Unit: 1 Pages: 20 39

With prompting and support, ask and answer questions about key details in a text. Grade 1 Ask and answer questions about key details in a text.

SocioBrains THE INTEGRATED APPROACH TO THE STUDY OF ART

Transcription:

Jokes and the Linguistic Mind. Debra Aarons. New York, New York: Routledge. 2012. Pp. xi +272. It is often said that understanding humor in a language is the highest sign of fluency. Comprehending de dicto jokes, specifically, demonstrates our tacit knowledge of and competence in that language. In Aarons s Jokes and the Linguistic Mind, multitudinous examples of humor are examined in regard to the wide range of linguistic concepts they address. The text is based on Chomsky s (1965) definition of competence as our implicit knowledge of the rules of language; it aims to show that when, for example, a joke about the ambiguity of a certain article is funny, it is precisely because it brings a rule governing article use to our consciousness. Aarons detailed analyses of jokes play on semantic, phonologic, pragmatic, and syntactic rules, and are therefore of interest to the linguist. At the same time, her analyses are of equal importance to the language teacher, considering that many of these rules and explanations are of pedagogical use. In her new book, Aarons analyzes linguistic jokes jokes that play on some aspect of language with a view to showing that they present evidence for the idea that there is knowledge we have about language that is not directly accessible to consciousness (p. 1). The humor in these jokes comes from principles of language that readers might not have examined before. Although this text centers on humor in English and is about the English language, the claims made can be extrapolated to any language. As Aarons states, Any person who knows his or her language can, in principle, get a joke (p. 8). The fact that many different features of English are examined makes this text relevant to students across a number of fields, including theoretical linguistics, psycholinguistics, cognitive science, as well as applied linguistics. In addition, the text provides much information that can help fluent speakers of English reflect on the complexities of the language; it also offers educators a rich source of material for lessons. The reasons why certain jokes are funny are presented in such a way that those familiar with linguistics terminology will find it easy to understand. In particular, it sheds light on a number of important issues, ranging from aspects of semantic knowledge that are easy to use but not explain (p. 91), to the rules of phonology and morphology in English. The author s analyses are thorough and very detailed. Considering the fact that the investigation itself may well overshadow the humorousness of each joke, Aarons carefully ends the first chapter with this comment, Humor can be dissected as a frog can, but the thing dies in the process and the innards are discouraging to any but the pure scientific mind (p. 18). The book is organized into nine chapters. Chapter 1 sets the stage with relevant research on the linguistic aspects of humor and the basic cognitive science theories necessary for readers to understand the analysis in the successive chapters. This opening chapter discusses the topics of language play, grammar, (un)translatability, as well as issues related to the processing of humor. Chapters 2 through 5 look at the aspects of language that different jokes can play to: pragmatics, semantics, morphology and phonology, and syntax, respectively. Chapter 6, a case study on the way these different aspects of linguistics can intersect in one piece, is centered on word association in a Monty Python skit. Chapters 7 and 8 focus on jokes that use more than one language and 35

those that are about language. The final chapter expounds on the mechanics of cryptic crosswords and the convergence of written and spoken English found in their clues. Chapter 1 presents background research and foundational theories on which Aarons bases her main arguments. A large section is devoted to the theories purported by Chomsky (1965) including universal grammar, as well as the distinction between language competence and performance. According to Aarons, the former is the more important element in understanding jokes. She also notes the inability of jokes to be translated despite the retention of linguistic meaning. This is a topic of importance to those interested in language learning; after all, learners cannot simply transfer their humor into the target language. The discussion of how non-native speakers relate to linguistic jokes is also of interest to those studying and researching in applied linguistics, even though the author only speculates on the relationship but does not mention research done on this topic. For instance, Aarons presents a short joke: What s brown and sticky? A stick. She shares that many users of English as a second language do not find it particularly funny, possibly because their knowledge of English does not draw on the same source as children s does (p. 13). She speculates that as part of their development in language, children create utterances that are based on their intuition of language and its rules; non-native speakers, however, do not do this with the same ease. The chapter ends with resources related to humor studies and a reiteration of the overall goal of the book, which is not to delineate what language tells us about jokes, but rather, to find what jokes tell us about language (p. 17). Of the four chapters about the different aspects of language that can be manipulated for humor, Chapter 2 on pragmatics is especially thought-provoking, for it contains many examples with punch lines that are difficult to explain at first glance. Aarons begins by differentiating between semantics and pragmatics: the former is concerned with the linguistic meaning of sentences, whereas the latter is concerned with utterances in their context of use (p. 20). By introducing. Grice s (1975) four conversational maxims and the idea of conversational implicature, Aarons presents a very well articulated discussion concerning why certain jokes are funny. Although 30 examples may seem ostensibly an inordinate number, several of them do touch on more than one of the maxims flouted to humorous effect, and are overall well chosen in terms of familiarity and accessibility. Issues such as referential assignment, the ambiguity of certain pronouns, and the misunderstandings of illocutionary force all serve as rich sources of data pertaining to the English language. The dissection of jokes can become very tedious, and indeed it does later in Chapter 5, with numerous sentence diagrams. This same chapter, on the other hand, contains material that clearly and succinctly supports the overall point that these jokes would not be comprehensible without a tacit knowledge of the rules of linguistic appropriateness, especially as they pertain to the interaction of rules of sentence structure with rules of use (p. 54). Chapter 3, Playing with Semantics, complements the discussion on pragmatics with a look at aspects of semantics that are easy to use but not explain (p. 91). The chapter begins by examining issues of sameness through synonymy, homonymy, and polysemy in 23 examples. Because there are numerous issues involved in establishing semantic meaning, it does not seem excessive to include so many instances. The chapter 36

continues with a look at homophony, shown through an assortment of puns, literal and non-literal use of language, and ambiguity. Jokes exploiting the ambiguity in article usage are especially relevant, for they remind us of the complexity of the rules regarding their use. Aarons illustrates this point well with the example: Every 15 minutes in New York City a man gets mugged. / If I were him, I d leave (p. 84). The analysis of jokes that exploit the morphology and phonology of English in Chapter 4 is also very enlightening, given that learners and teachers of English often consciously reflect on sounds and parts of word. These two aspects of language are presented in the same chapter probably because they are exploited in jokes in similar ways. After all, both morphemes and phonemes are meaningless units when found in the wrong combination or context, and the cruces of many of the jokes presented in the book are based on the ordering of these units in words. The overgeneralization of word formation rules and the messy cranberry-morphemes (i.e., morphemes that do not have their own meaning, as is the cran in cranberry) will be very familiar to many in the field of linguistics. Example 10, which is about a person who appears gruntled, again illustrates the difficulties that may arise when we generalize rules about language. Syntax, the focus of Chapter 5, is presented through myriad sentence diagrams. These are quite interesting to those with the appropriate training and familiarity. However, if, as the author states, that her goal is to show that the audience for linguistic jokes is not restricted to linguists (p. 126), then diagramming may not be the most accessible method of examination for the wider intended audience. Rewriting the jokes as contrasting sentences might have been easier to follow than the different forms of trees, at least for some of her readers. Although the comedy team Monty Python s Flying Circus is known and admired for comic genius, Chapter 6 becomes tedious and repetitive by the fifth page with the extensive analysis of their word association skit. A shorter explanation of one segment of this skit would suffice, especially when the author is not exploring several different types of jokes but rather over 50 examples of word association jokes. Chapter 7, titled Jokes in More than one Language, begins with a look at bilingual humor, and later, at macaronic language language that bases its humor on the orthographic-sound system of two languages. The exhaustive example is of several rhymes written in nonsense French which, when read aloud, sound as they would in heavily French-accented English. This chapter furthers Aarons point from Chapter 4 that a string of phonemes outside a context of use may mean nothing in one language but may have one or many meanings, depending on the languages available to joke tellers and joke listeners, (p. 170). Again, she gives several examples that are generally well-chosen. However, the second joke is not really a bilingual joke, but more a joke about bilingualism, and the tendency of Americans to underestimate the value of knowing more than one language, (p. 171) which seems to have detracted from the argument in this section. Aarons begins Chapter 8 with the point that many of these are not jokes as much as they are simply pithy little remarks pointing out the absurdities of English (p. 192). As such, the chapter discusses not only recursion but also rebellion against linguistic prescriptivism, with many examples about the use of prepositions. This chapter is 37

relevant to those in the field of applied linguistics as it allows us to think about language as an object, and our use of language as a phenomenon (p. 192). Cryptic crosswords, the focus of Chapter 9, are given the same in-depth analysis as that of linguistic jokes in the preceding chapters. A university lecturer in Australia herself, Aarons is much more familiar with this style of puzzle than the crossword puzzles more recognizable to speakers of American English. Her goal, which would be applicable to other forms of crosswords as well, is to show that these can be useful tools to activate our tacit knowledge of the rules of our language and the necessary conscious knowledge of the rules of written English and its relation to spoken English (p. 223). By addressing topics such as literal meaning versus non-literal meaning of words, as well as ambiguity of meaning, she very helpfully demonstrates the great amount of linguistic knowledge that must be accessed in order to successfully complete a puzzle of this kind. Surprisingly though, a discussion of culture is absent from this discussion. Aarons comments: Cryptic crossword clues are largely based on eliciting what users know about language, primarily in the written form, but also about the relationships between oral and written form (p. 230). This seems only partly true, as a large part of the ability to understand the clues is based on cultural awareness. This text, on the other hand, is meant to deal with the linguistic aspect of jokes. As noted by Aarons: The consciousness of the rules of the particular code is not quite enough; the setter/solver of the cryptic clue must have (near-) native knowledge of the language as well (p. 229). However, even native speakers of one variety of English could have difficulty completing a cryptic crossword written in another dialect, not to say encounter challenges when attempting to comprehend cultural references specific to one English-speaking region. For example, the answer to one of the crossword clues examined in Chapter 9 is Saul Bellow, the Canadian-American author. This clue may not be apparent to those unfamiliar with North American English. As such, it would not be out of place to briefly discuss the cultural component of the puzzles and how they can be addressed. As a linguistics scholar, Aarons stresses that her work is crucially about linguistics and not about humor (p. 3). Yet, while humor is the secondary focus, several of the examples of jokes are not optimally chosen even though well-analyzed. On page 2, she notes: Readers will no doubt be able to think of even better, funnier jokes to illustrate the points I have raised. If that is the case, I shall consider that my aim is well on its way to being met. It may be that readers would prefer their own favorite jokes used as the examples. Nevertheless, the actual issue with the text is that the majority of the jokes used are not current enough. Some of the more oft-quoted comedians throughout the text, such as Spike Milligan and Groucho Marx, were the most prolific in the 1950s and 1960s. Their jokes, though funny, do not necessarily reflect the current tacit knowledge. Undoubtedly, English speakers all over the world are more familiar than ever with English language pop culture, but their exposure to humor is now more likely to be based on The Simpsons (mentioned in the examples on p. 97) than the midcentury offerings. It may be that the older jokes are simply more familiar to the audience and will allow for easier analysis, but this issue is not discussed. According to Aarons, jokes activate something that normal interactions don t; they cause us to reflect on language (p. 8). Fluent speakers of English have aspects of 38

competence that can be accessed through jokes. The discussion of such aspects of competence in Jokes and the Linguistic Mind helps place it as a companion piece to the main text in a linguistics course. The strength of the chapters on semantics and pragmatics would also render this book a supplementary text in applied linguistics courses. Linguistic concepts such as semantic ambiguity would become more accessible and easier to examine when presented in the context of a common joke. It may, of course, ruin the joke as well. ELAINE ROBERTS Immigrant Support Program Manager, International Center REFERENCES Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Grice, H. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 3). New York: Academic Press. 39