Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

Similar documents
Notes on David Temperley s What s Key for Key? The Krumhansl-Schmuckler Key-Finding Algorithm Reconsidered By Carley Tanoue

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

Construction of a harmonic phrase

HST 725 Music Perception & Cognition Assignment #1 =================================================================

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

Harmonic Factors in the Perception of Tonal Melodies

Harmony and tonality The vertical dimension. HST 725 Lecture 11 Music Perception & Cognition

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

Pitch Spelling Algorithms

An Integrated Music Chromaticism Model

Unity and process in Roberto Gerhard s Symphony no. 3, 'Collages'

MUSIC THEORY CURRICULUM STANDARDS GRADES Students will sing, alone and with others, a varied repertoire of music.

Observations and Thoughts on the Opening Phrase of Webern's Symphony Op.21. Mvt. I. by Glen Charles Halls. (for teaching purposes)

INTERACTIVE GTTM ANALYZER

Chapter Five: The Elements of Music

Connecticut State Department of Education Music Standards Middle School Grades 6-8

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using vocabulary and language of music.

Similarity matrix for musical themes identification considering sound s pitch and duration

Toward an analysis of polyphonic music in the textual symbolic segmentation

Curriculum Development In the Fairfield Public Schools FAIRFIELD PUBLIC SCHOOLS FAIRFIELD, CONNECTICUT MUSIC THEORY I

Boulez. Aspects of Pli Selon Pli. Glen Halls All Rights Reserved.

K-12 Performing Arts - Music Standards Lincoln Community School Sources: ArtsEdge - National Standards for Arts Education

Student Performance Q&A: 2001 AP Music Theory Free-Response Questions

Melody: sequences of pitches unfolding in time. HST 725 Lecture 12 Music Perception & Cognition

Music Theory. Fine Arts Curriculum Framework. Revised 2008

Analysis of Webern s Pieces for Cello and Piano Op. 11, No. 1. Like much of Anton Webern s music, the short duration of Pieces for Cello and Piano

Effects of a Change in Instrumentation on the Recognition of Musical Materials

A GTTM Analysis of Manolis Kalomiris Chant du Soir

Perceptual Tests of an Algorithm for Musical Key-Finding

Perceptual Evaluation of Automatically Extracted Musical Motives

On Interpreting Bach. Purpose. Assumptions. Results

Similarity, Invariance, and Musical Variation

A FUNCTIONAL CLASSIFICATION OF ONE INSTRUMENT S TIMBRES

Perceptual Considerations in Designing and Fitting Hearing Aids for Music Published on Friday, 14 March :01

29 Music CO-SG-FLD Program for Licensing Assessments for Colorado Educators

Student Performance Q&A:

COURSE OUTLINE. Corequisites: None

Kansas State Music Standards Ensembles

Music Theory: A Very Brief Introduction

ILLINOIS LICENSURE TESTING SYSTEM

MELODIC AND RHYTHMIC CONTRASTS IN EMOTIONAL SPEECH AND MUSIC

Analysis and Discussion of Schoenberg Op. 25 #1. ( Preludium from the piano suite ) Part 1. How to find a row? by Glen Halls.

CSC475 Music Information Retrieval

Varieties of Tone Presence: Process, Gesture, and the Excessive Polyvalence of Pitch in Post-Tonal Music

Credo Theory of Music training programme GRADE 4 By S. J. Cloete

Commentary on David Huron s On the Role of Embellishment Tones in the Perceptual Segregation of Concurrent Musical Parts

2 The Tonal Properties of Pitch-Class Sets: Tonal Implication, Tonal Ambiguity, and Tonalness

Piano Teacher Program

An Interactive Case-Based Reasoning Approach for Generating Expressive Music

Timing variations in music performance: Musical communication, perceptual compensation, and/or motor control?

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. Perception of just noticeable time displacement of a tone presented in a metrical sequence at different tempos

Composing and Interpreting Music

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Elements of Music David Scoggin OLLI Understanding Jazz Fall 2016

& Ψ. study guide. Music Psychology ... A guide for preparing to take the qualifying examination in music psychology.

Judgments of distance between trichords

TONAL HIERARCHIES, IN WHICH SETS OF PITCH

Introduction. Figure 1: A training example and a new problem.

Copyright 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. or its affiliate(s). All rights reserved. NES, the NES logo, Pearson, the Pearson logo, and National

LESSON 1 PITCH NOTATION AND INTERVALS

Stylistic features Antonio Vivaldi: Concerto in D minor, Op. 3 No. 11

A PRELIMINARY COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF IMMANENT ACCENT SALIENCE IN TONAL MUSIC

Music Performance Ensemble

ILLINOIS LICENSURE TESTING SYSTEM

Perception: A Perspective from Musical Theory

THE EFFECT OF EXPERTISE IN EVALUATING EMOTIONS IN MUSIC

Assessment Schedule 2017 Music: Demonstrate knowledge of conventions in a range of music scores (91276)

Oskaloosa Community School District. Music. Grade Level Benchmarks

Course Overview. Assessments What are the essential elements and. aptitude and aural acuity? meaning and expression in music?

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

EXPLAINING AND PREDICTING THE PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL STRUCTURE

Descending- and ascending- 5 6 sequences (sequences based on thirds and seconds):

Learners will practise and learn to perform one or more piece(s) for their instrument of an appropriate level of difficulty.

T Y H G E D I. Music Informatics. Alan Smaill. Jan 21st Alan Smaill Music Informatics Jan 21st /1

Modeling perceived relationships between melody, harmony, and key

Connecticut Common Arts Assessment Initiative

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

West Windsor-Plainsboro Regional School District String Orchestra Grade 9

BASIC CONCEPTS AND PRINCIPLES IN MODERN MUSICAL ANALYSIS. A SCHENKERIAN APPROACH

MMS 8th Grade General Music Curriculum

MUSIC COURSE OF STUDY GRADES K-5 GRADE

Timbre as Vertical Process: Attempting a Perceptually Informed Functionality of Timbre. Anthony Tan

Texas State Solo & Ensemble Contest. May 25 & May 27, Theory Test Cover Sheet

OKLAHOMA SUBJECT AREA TESTS (OSAT )

Music Curriculum Glossary

Audio Feature Extraction for Corpus Analysis

AoS1 set works Bernstein: Something s Coming Reich: Electric Counterpoint Schoenberg: Peripetie

2011 Music Performance GA 3: Aural and written examination

Curriculum Standard One: The student will listen to and analyze music critically, using the vocabulary and language of music.

USING HARMONIC AND MELODIC ANALYSES TO AUTOMATE THE INITIAL STAGES OF SCHENKERIAN ANALYSIS

Music Complexity Descriptors. Matt Stabile June 6 th, 2008

Music Performance Solo

MEMORY & TIMBRE MEMT 463

Comprehensive Course Syllabus-Music Theory

School of Church Music Southwestern Baptist Theological Seminary

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

The Generation of Metric Hierarchies using Inner Metric Analysis

PERFORMING ARTS. Head of Music: Cinzia Cursaro. Year 7 MUSIC Core Component 1 Term

BLUE VALLEY DISTRICT CURRICULUM & INSTRUCTION Music 9-12/Honors Music Theory

Grade 3 General Music

Transcription:

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical and schemas Stella Paraskeva (,) Stephen McAdams (,) () Institut de Recherche et de Coordination Acoustique/Musique (IRCAM) () Laboratoire de Psychologie Expérimentale (CNRS), René Descartes, EPHE Stella.Paraskeva@ircam.fr smc@ircam.fr Abstract The aim of this study is to investigate the effects of timbre, the presence or absence of tonal hierarchy, and the influence of musical training on the perception of "- schemas" in musical phrases. Two musical excerpts taken from the Baroque 'tonal/metric' and contemporary 'nontonal/nonmetric' repertoires constituted the stimuli of the experiment. Two versions of each excerpt were recorded: one 'piano version', in which the piece was recorded with only one timbre, and one 'orchestral version' in which the piece was played by several instruments. In order to avoid the intervention of the parameters of dynamics, interpretation, and conducting and performance of the piece, the same MIDI score was used for both versions. The experiment consisted of a presentation of successive segments of the musical pieces with different lengths to two different populations: 0 musicians and 0 nonmusicians. Listeners were asked to rate the degree of completeness perceived at the end of each segment, giving a completeness or profile. Our results showed that the timbre and the presence or absence of tonal hierarchy directly influenced the perception of musical - schemas, while musical training played only a minor role and only in the tonal/metric context. Introduction In a real music listening situation, we observe different degrees of musical stability and structural importance: chords which have the function of preparing and others which have a function of musical stability []. From a hierarchical point of view the unimportant notes, the transition notes, the ornamental notes, create the intuition of expectation, what has been called "musical ". On the contrary, the hierarchically important notes create a sense of rest or completion, what has been called "". So, the chords which contain the hierarchically less important notes create strong s and wait for the appearance of hierarchically more important chords in order to resolve to a state of. Depending on their degree of musical stability, the notes create a schema which is called a "tonal hierarchy" []. For all listeners, the tonic and then the dominant, have the highest degree of concordance. The dominant is followed hierarchically by the third and then the other notes of the key, and finally by the notes not belonging to the key. The Western European adult listener who can call upon an explicit/implicit knowledge, is potentially capable of understanding the existing hierarchies between notes of a key, between chords of a key, and between different keys. The intuition of musical and is partly created on the basis on these "distance" relations []. The absence of tonality can result from producing a contrast to the tonal feeling at any instance and whenever possible. Melodically or harmonically the notes, in their succesion, do not allow further the research of a tonal center.. Experimental hypothesis The experimental approach consists in the presentation of two musical pieces in two different versions: one 'piano version' and one 'orchestral version'. We do not have at our disposal theoretical data specifically concerning timbre. However, the experiment produced results showing that the change of timbre causes a modification in the hierarchical coding of the existing relations between musical events. From the listener's point of view, the organization of musical and is constructed in a hierarchical way. In a nontonal and nonmetric piece of music, a hierarchical description cannot be observed. In our experiment we thus sought to compare judgements of in both tonal and atonal contexts. Proc. 997 ICMC, Thessaloniki

The third question refers to the differences in the way musical pieces are processed by two different populations: musicians and nonmusicians. One can, therefore, suppose that the difference between explicit knowledge based on specialized musical education and knowledge acquired implicitly through listening experience may result in different perceptions of musical and schemas. Method. Subjects Eighty (80) subjects participated in this experiment: 0 professional musicians and 0 nonmusicians. Their average age was 0 years. The musicians were selected on the basis of their theoretical knowledge, evaluated with regard to their education in harmony and ear training, and independently of the number of years they practiced an instrument. The nonmusicians had not received any musical training aside from organized musical activities at the primary school level.. Stimuli Two musical excerpts taken from the Baroque and contemporary repertoire constituted the stimuli of the experiment: the Ricercare in six voices by J. S. Bach (measures: -79) and the first movement of Six Pieces for Orchestra, op. by A. Webern (9 version). In order to avoid the intervention of parameters other than pitch, rhythm and timbre that create and schemas (such as dynamics and timing differences introduced in the course of musical interpretation), all the notes were programmed using a computer and synthesizers, securing in this way the same rhythmical structure, the same intensity, the same tempo and the exact duration notated in the score. Two versions of each excerpt were presented: one piano version, in which the piece was recorded with only one timbre, and one orchestral version in which the piece was played by several instruments. The piano version of the Webern was simply a piano reduction of the orchestral score and that of th'e Bach was a piano reduction of the Webern orchestration. In both cases the piano version preserved the register of the notes in the orchestral version. The segmentation criteria of the two pieces were based on timbral changes in the score and more, for the Bach excerpt, on the passage from one key to another one (modulation) as well as on other criteria such as the interruption of an ascending or descending series of notes. In this way, the two excerpts were divided into segments of different lengths and each segment included all the previous ones. Both pieces were presented to each subject in either the piano or the orchestral version. The same tempo was used in both versions. In order to reduce the length of the experiment, an interruption was introduced in the middle of each piece at which point the succeeding segments restarted from the middle and not from the beginning.. Apparatus Different sampler configurations were used depending on the orchestration needs of each piece: piano version of Bach and Webern: KURZWEIL 00 orchestral version of Bach: ROLAND U 0 RS- DCM sound volume, PROTEUS / BIT Multi- Timbral digital sound module, ROLAND SP-700 BIT Sample player. orchestral version of Webern: AKAI S00 Sample player, ROLAND JV-880, PROTEUS / EMU, KORG Mr EX, ROLAND SC- Sound Canvas.. Procedure The experiment consisted of the presentation of units of successive stops of different duration. Each group of subjects was divided into two subgroups of 0 persons and each one of those listened first to one of the two versions of each piece. The concepts of and were addressed through the notion of the degree of completeness of a phrase of language. The subjects were asked to evaluate the degree of completeness of the musical phrase using a 7-point scale corresponding to impressions of completeness varying from not at all complete (=strongest ) to very complete (7=strongest ). Listeners rated the perceived degree of completeness felt at the end of each segment. The series of successive judgments thus gave rise to a kind of completeness (or, inversely, ) profile for each piece. We were primarily interested in differences in these profiles between single timbre and multi-timbre versions of each piece, although effects of musical training and degree of variation of in tonal/metric and nontonal/nonmetric pieces were also examined. Results Analyses of variance were performed to study the effects of the factors timbral version (piano vs. orchestra), musical training (professional musicians vs. nonmusicians), and segment on completeness judgments. The results of the analysis of variance are presented separately for the two pieces. The main effects are first examined, followed by the effects of their interactions. Proc. 997 ICMC, Thessaloniki

. Timbral version It was found that the effect of timbre was very significant for both pieces. In total, all the values of the degree of completeness for the orchestral version were greated than those for the piano version [Bach (F(.7)=.0, p<.000); Webern (F(.7)=8.9, p<.000)].. Using multivariate analysis of variance, it was found that the effect of segment was highly significant [Bach (F(.7)=8.9, p<.000); Webern (F(.7)=.09, p<.000)], indicating that judged completeness varied significantly across the piece in both works.. Musical training There was no global effect of musical training for either piece [Bach (F(.7)<); Webern (F(.7)<)].. Interaction between segment and timbral version This interaction was highly significant for both pieces [Bach (F(.7)=.97, p<.000); Webern (F(.7)=.80, p<.000)] indicating that the profiles were globally different for the piano and orchestral versions (see Figure ). Bach Ricercar, mm. -79 7 most complete Tonal 0 0 7 Nontonal Webern Pieces, op., st mvt. 0 0 Piano Orchestra significant difference between versions most complete Figure : The effect of orchestration of profiles.. Interaction between segment and musical training This interaction was significant only for the Bach piece [Bach (F(.7)=., p<.000); Webern (F(.7)=.09, n.s.)] indicating that musicians and nonmusicians completeness profiles differed (slightly) for tonal/metric music, but were nearly identical for nontonal/nonmetric music (see Figure ). However, no differences between musicians and nonmusicians were found for the completeness profiles across the different timbral versions. Bach Ricercar 7 most complete Tonal 7 Nontonal 0 0 Webern Pieces most complete 0 0 Musicians Nonmusicians Proc. 997 ICMC, Thessaloniki

Figure : The effect of musical training on profiles. Discussion This study confirms the experimental hypothesis that the timbre plays an important role in the perception of relationships of musical and. As for the way that the musical timbre was evaluated by the listeners, it was found on the one hand that the perception of the relation between musical and changes considerably with the existence of a timbre modification and on the other hand that the judgements of completeness showed a reduction in the orchestral version compared to the piano version. During the execution of the same score from the timbre of the piano, the same notes which have been interpreted from different instruments start at the same time. This is the result of an unison because in our case they are played by just one timbre with a sharp attack. In other words, in the orchestral version we perceive different instrumental sources whereas in the piano version we hear only one complex chordal event, probably due to perceptual fusion based attack synchrony. Our results concerning either the hierarchical representation or the presence or absence of tonal and metric hierarchies confirm the hypothesis according to which the listener proceeds with a hierarchization of the musical information irrespective of its structure (tonal/atonal). The profile analysis of the Bach piece implies the existence of a highly hierarchical structure. The analysis of the non-hierarchical Webern piece suggests that, in spite of the absence of key and meter, there are points of varying musical stability: these can also be stable, creating the impression of rest (points of great stability where the musical s seem to be resolved), of middle stability (when the notes or the chords to which they belong have less structural significance) of less stability (when the notes create to the listener a strong expectation impression ) and finally of a strong instability (when the notes or the chords are not non-significant, establishing the intuition of a strong. To each note of the musical discourse a structural importance can be attributed, because it belongs to a hierarchically important chord or because it creates the sense of expectation of the arrival of an essential chord or of a more important note. It is, therefore, verified that music is certainly not the result of a simple succession of sonorous events (notes), but the product of a complex hierarchical organization. Listeners possess implicit knowledge which allows them to perceive this complexity. As far as musical training is concerned, our results show that the relations between musical and observed by musicians and non-musicians do not differ significantly or differ only slightly. A simple observation of the profiles of responses given by musicians on the one hand and non-musicians on the other clearly suggests the development of implicit knowledge of the tonal hierarchy for the Bach excerpt. As for the Webern piece, which is characterized by the absence of key and meter, the schemas of and observed by the nonmusicians do not differ from those observed by musicians. The correlation between the two groups of listeners underlines the fact that the perception of musical and seems to depend very little on specific musical training. Conclusions and Perspectives On the basis of these results, we are led to conclude that: ) Changes of orchestration can influence the hierarchical coding of the existing relations between musical events. ) Western European adult listeners have similar reactions to tonal and atonal music, whether they are musicians or non-musicians. They possess explicit and/or implicit knowledge of the tonal hierarchies, which allows them to perceive and quantify the relation of musical and developed in the flow of the musical phrase by recognizing specific weights of pitches. ) Within the context of tonal/metric and nontonal/nonmetric music, the contrast between the judgments on and schemas indicates that the listeners hierarchize the musical information giving rise to musical and as heard in the context of a musical piece; in this way, they succeed in apprehending a certain coherence and unity in complex musical pieces. Thus, it can be concluded by our results that there is a direct implication of the timbre on the musical perception of and schemas. It could be consequently supposed that a similar research on a population of children could produce different results and an interest in the genetic evolution of the implicit knowledge about music in the human being could be developed. It could be equally interesting to evaluate the effects of other factors, such as the social origin and the cultural differences (oriental/occidental). In addition, the phenomenon of unison for other timbres producing a longer attack (violin, flute) could be studied. In the absence of key, the harmony does not appreciate the succession of and any more. According to [] (p. 7), "an atonal surface is a sequence of events and its pitch-space is flat". Our results on the hierarchical representations of segments, concerning the non-hierarchical piece of Webern, allow us to maintain that the atonal surface of the piece does not constitute a flat space and leads us, therefore, to Proc. 997 ICMC, Thessaloniki

modify somewhat Lerdahl's conclusions. A continuation of the research in this same direction will be necessay to examine this subject in depth. References [] Bigand, E. 99. Contributions of music to research on human auditory cognition", in S. McAdams & E. Bigand (Eds.), Thinking in Sound: The Cognitive Psychology of Human Audition, pp. -77, Oxford University Press, Oxford. [] Krumhansl, C. L. 990. Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch, Oxford University Press, Oxford. [] Lerdahl, F. 989. "Atonal prolongational structure", Contemporary Music Review, Volume, pp. -87. [] Lerdahl, F. & Jackendoff, R. 98. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Proc. 997 ICMC, Thessaloniki