AD-A ýqj m7n IN , 111:

Similar documents
Pitch Perception and Grouping. HST.723 Neural Coding and Perception of Sound

Auditory Illusions. Diana Deutsch. The sounds we perceive do not always correspond to those that are

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

AFRL-RY-WP-TR

2018 Fall CTP431: Music and Audio Computing Fundamentals of Musical Acoustics

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

CTP431- Music and Audio Computing Musical Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology KAIST Juhan Nam

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

2 Autocorrelation verses Strobed Temporal Integration

RATE-ADAPTIVE VIDEO CODING (RAVC)

Getting Started with the LabVIEW Sound and Vibration Toolkit

Do Zwicker Tones Evoke a Musical Pitch?

POST-PROCESSING FIDDLE : A REAL-TIME MULTI-PITCH TRACKING TECHNIQUE USING HARMONIC PARTIAL SUBTRACTION FOR USE WITHIN LIVE PERFORMANCE SYSTEMS

MIE 402: WORKSHOP ON DATA ACQUISITION AND SIGNAL PROCESSING Spring 2003

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

EE513 Audio Signals and Systems. Introduction Kevin D. Donohue Electrical and Computer Engineering University of Kentucky

CTP 431 Music and Audio Computing. Basic Acoustics. Graduate School of Culture Technology (GSCT) Juhan Nam

AN ARTISTIC TECHNIQUE FOR AUDIO-TO-VIDEO TRANSLATION ON A MUSIC PERCEPTION STUDY

Lab P-6: Synthesis of Sinusoidal Signals A Music Illusion. A k cos.! k t C k / (1)

A Comparison of the Temporal Characteristics of LCS, LCoS, Laser, And CRT Projectors

Robert Alexandru Dobre, Cristian Negrescu

Psychoacoustics. lecturer:

9.35 Sensation And Perception Spring 2009

Pitch: The Perceptual Ends of the Periodicity; but Of What Periodicity?

CSC475 Music Information Retrieval

The Measurement Tools and What They Do

PHYSICS OF MUSIC. 1.) Charles Taylor, Exploring Music (Music Library ML3805 T )

Syllabus: PHYS 1300 Introduction to Musical Acoustics Fall 20XX

Analysis, Synthesis, and Perception of Musical Sounds

Inhibition of Oscillation in a Plastic Neural Network Model of Tinnitus Therapy Using Noise Stimulus

ECE438 - Laboratory 4: Sampling and Reconstruction of Continuous-Time Signals

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Dither Explained. An explanation and proof of the benefit of dither. for the audio engineer. By Nika Aldrich. April 25, 2002

Consonance perception of complex-tone dyads and chords

The Semitone Paradox

EE391 Special Report (Spring 2005) Automatic Chord Recognition Using A Summary Autocorrelation Function

Music Representations

I. INTRODUCTION. 1 place Stravinsky, Paris, France; electronic mail:

REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE

Effects of Remaining Hair Cells on Cochlear Implant Function

BER MEASUREMENT IN THE NOISY CHANNEL

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

ONLINE ACTIVITIES FOR MUSIC INFORMATION AND ACOUSTICS EDUCATION AND PSYCHOACOUSTIC DATA COLLECTION

DELTA MODULATION AND DPCM CODING OF COLOR SIGNALS

Pitch circularity from tones comprising full harmonic series

Loudness and Sharpness Calculation

Laboratory Assignment 3. Digital Music Synthesis: Beethoven s Fifth Symphony Using MATLAB

Digital Delay / Pulse Generator DG535 Digital delay and pulse generator (4-channel)

Computer-based sound spectrograph system

DISTRIBUTION STATEMENT A 7001Ö

Signal processing in the Philips 'VLP' system

2. AN INTROSPECTION OF THE MORPHING PROCESS

Book: Fundamentals of Music Processing. Audio Features. Book: Fundamentals of Music Processing. Book: Fundamentals of Music Processing

International Journal of Computer Architecture and Mobility (ISSN ) Volume 1-Issue 7, May 2013

PERFORMANCE OF 10- AND 20-TARGET MSE CLASSIFIERS 1

Quarterly Progress and Status Report. An attempt to predict the masking effect of vowel spectra

Chapter 1. Introduction to Digital Signal Processing

Part I Of An Exclusive Interview With The Father Of Digital FM Synthesis. By Tom Darter.

TEST WIRE FOR HIGH VOLTAGE POWER SUPPLY CROWBAR SYSTEM

ECE 4220 Real Time Embedded Systems Final Project Spectrum Analyzer

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

Pitch is one of the most common terms used to describe sound.

Experiment 2: Sampling and Quantization

Digital Signal. Continuous. Continuous. amplitude. amplitude. Discrete-time Signal. Analog Signal. Discrete. Continuous. time. time.

Determination of Sound Quality of Refrigerant Compressors

Department of Electrical & Electronic Engineering Imperial College of Science, Technology and Medicine. Project: Real-Time Speech Enhancement

How to Obtain a Good Stereo Sound Stage in Cars

Musical Illusions Diana Deutsch Department of Psychology University of California, San Diego La Jolla, CA 92093

HST Neural Coding and Perception of Sound. Spring Cochlear Nucleus Unit Classification from Spike Trains. M.

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107 (3), March /2000/107(3)/1589/9/$ Acoustical Society of America 1589

RADIOGRAPHIC PERFORMANCE OF CYGNUS 1 AND THE FEBETRON 705

2 MHz Lock-In Amplifier

EMERGENT SOUNDSCAPE COMPOSITION: REFLECTIONS ON VIRTUALITY

Full Disclosure Monitoring

Topic 4. Single Pitch Detection

ESD RECORD COPY STUDIES OF DISPLAY SYMBOL LEGIBILITY. Part V. The Effects of Television Transmission on the Legibility of Common Five-Letter Words

Electrical Stimulation of the Cochlea to Reduce Tinnitus. Richard S. Tyler, Ph.D. Overview

Interface Practices Subcommittee SCTE STANDARD SCTE Composite Distortion Measurements (CSO & CTB)

SERIAL HIGH DENSITY DIGITAL RECORDING USING AN ANALOG MAGNETIC TAPE RECORDER/REPRODUCER

Supplemental Material for Gamma-band Synchronization in the Macaque Hippocampus and Memory Formation

Vocoder Reference Test TELECOMMUNICATIONS INDUSTRY ASSOCIATION

IEEE Santa Clara ComSoc/CAS Weekend Workshop Event-based analog sensing

Model 7330 Signal Source Analyzer Dedicated Phase Noise Test System V1.02

4. TITLE AND SUBTITLE 5a. CONTRACT NUMBER. 6. AUTHOR(S) 5d. PROJECT NUMBER

Music Source Separation

Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant

Experiment 13 Sampling and reconstruction

On the strike note of bells

Creative Computing II

Linrad On-Screen Controls K1JT

MODIFICATIONS TO THE POWER FUNCTION FOR LOUDNESS

A Matlab toolbox for. Characterisation Of Recorded Underwater Sound (CHORUS) USER S GUIDE

CONSONANCE AND DISSONANCE 4.2. Simple integer ratios Why is it that two notes an octave apart sound consonant, while two notes a little more or

Note on Posted Slides. Noise and Music. Noise and Music. Pitch. PHY205H1S Physics of Everyday Life Class 15: Musical Sounds

Sound Creation Tool FWCTRL Operation Manual

Transcription:

m7n AD-A256 7 11111 IN 1111111111111111111111 1, 111: ýqj 9-27201

This report is based on studies performed at Lincoln Laboratory. a center for research operated by Massachusetts Institute of Technology. The work was sponsored by the Department of the Air Force under Contract F19628-90-C-0002. This report may be reproduced to satisfy needs of U.S. Government agencies. The ESC Public Affairs Office has reviewed this report, and it is releasable to the National Technical Information Service. where it %ill be available to the general public. including foreign nationals. This technical report has been reviewed and is approved for publication. FOR THE COMMANDER /94y /1 SadA4' Hugh L. Southall, Lt. Col., USAF Chief. ESC Lincoln Laboratory Project Office Non-Lincoln Recipients PLEASE DO NOT RETURN Permission is given to destroy this document when it is no longer needed.

MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY LINCOLN LABORATORY MODELS OF PITCH PERCEPTION T.C. karset Group 41 B. GOLD Group 21 TECHNICAL REPORT 964 28 AUGUST 1992 Approved for public release: distribution is unlimited. LEXINGTON MASSACHUSETTS

ABSTRACT Two pitch perception modeling algorithms are described. The first algorithm models "periodicity" pitch perception, and the second algorithm models "place" pitch perception. The two models are now applied to various psychoacoustic stimuli. Both periodicity and place models yield results that are in general agreement with psychoacoustic measurements for the missing fundamental and for inharmonic stimuli. The place algorithm proved to be a better approximation than periodicity for processing comb-filtered noise. Periodicity was more successful for periodic pulse train stimuli. Aeoession For DTIC TAB Urnarnounced S Jus3t1flce, to. - By,. Availability Codes Dint t and/or Dit Special

TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract List of Illustrations ii iv 1. REVIEW OF SOME ASPECTS OF PSYCHOACOUSTICS 1.1 The Missing Fundamental I 1.2 Pitch of Inharmonic Signals 1 1.3 Pitch of Repeated Noise 1 1.4 Pitch Perception of Pulse Trains I 1.5 Circularity in Judgments of Relative Pitch 1 2. DESCRIPTION OF PERIODICITY AND PLACE PROGRAMS TO MODEL PITCH PERCEPTION 2 2.1 Periodicity Pitch 2 2.2 Place Pitch 3 3. PRELIMINARY MODEL RESULTS FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC STIMULI 4 3.1 Pitch of Inharmonic Signals (Shift of Virtual Pitch) 4 3.2 Pitch of Comb-Filtered Noise 5 3.3 Transition from Rate to Fundamental Frequency 5 3.4 Circularity in Pitch Perception 5 4. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 8 REFERENCES 9 iii

LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS Figure No. (a) Block diagram for the periodicity algorithm and (b) block diagram of the two-stage place algorithm. 2 Page 2 Shift of Virtual Pitch. (a) Periodicity pitch track for a stimulus consisting of harmonics I and 2, (b) three pitch curves for a stimulus consisting of harmonics 3, 4, and 5, and (c) a stimulus consisting of harmonics 2 through 5 inclusive. 4 3 Pitch of comb-filtered noise, which shows the tracks of the detected periods from both place and periodicity pitch detection algorithms. 5 4 Rate versus frequency plot from the periodicity pitch algorithm for the Flanagan-Guttman pulse stimulus. 6 5 Comparison of periodicity and place algorithms for pitch circularity using a Shepard tone complex. The one-half octave shift (circularity) described in the psychoacoustics literature is contrasted to the arbitrary response from the place algorithm to the same stimuli. 7 6 Periodicity and place model responses to Risset's "Endless Glissando." 7 iv

1. REVIEW OF SOME ASPECTS OF PSYCHOACOUSTICS 1.1 THE MISSING FUNDAMENTAL When a listener matches a pure tone (i.e., sine wave) to a complex tone consisting of a set of harmonically related sum of sinusoids (e.g., harmonics 3, 4, and 5 of some fundamental frequency), the match will take place at f, which is the missing fundamental frequency of the complex tone. 1.2 PITCH OF INHARMONIC SIGNALS When a listener is asked to match the sum of sinusoids at frequencies 3f, 4f, and 5f to a pure tone, that match will occur atf. What happens if each of the above three sinusoids is shifted in frequency by Af? DeBoer [I] performed such an experiment, as did van den Brink [2] and Smoorenburg [3]. This research showed that subjects were still able to perceive pitch despite the inharmonicity of the signal. 1.3 PITCH OF REPEATED NOISE Miller and Taylor [4] discovered that pitch could be perceived when the listener was presented with repeated bursts of noise. More current research found if white noise was comb-filtered (i.e., the output is the sum of the input and a delayed version of the input), a pitch of lit was perceived [5-81. The stimulus "sounds like" noise; however, when T is systematically varied such that lit steps through the frequencies corresponding to the seven notes of the major scale, the pitch of these notes is heard [91. 1.4 PITCH PERCEPTION OF PULSE TRAINS Flanagan and Guttman [10] discovered two distinct modes of pitch perception for periodic pulse train stimuli. To quote their paper, "In the first mode, for pulse rates less than 100 pps, the pulse trains are ascribed a pitch equal to the number of pulses per second, regardless of the polarity pattern of the pulses. In the second mode, for fundamental frequencies in excess of 200 Hz, the sounds are assigned a pitch equal to the fundamental frequency." 1.5 CIRCULARITY IN JUDGMENTS OF RELATIVE PITCH Shepard [I l I demonstrated that for specialized signals consisting of the sum of tones separated by octaves (e.g., 150, 300, 600, or 1200 Hz, etc.) listeners will often identify an increase in the tone frequencies as a lowering of pitch. On the average, if all tone frequencies are increased by less than onehalf octave, the new stimulus is judged to be higher in pitch than the old one. If, however, all tone frequencies are increased by more than one-half octave (but less than one octave), the new stimulus is judged to be lower in pitch. Pollack [121 viewed this result as a further example of the decoupling of auditory pitch and stimulus frequency. Through a series of experiments he identifies important parameters of this phenomenon as "the number of components in the signal, the number of offsetting frequencies which are weighted against the direction of Shepard pitch and perhaps the spacing between the components." Deutsch [131 has discovered other interesting properties of "Shepard pitch." For example, a tone pattern can be heard as ascending when played in one key and descending when played in another.

2. DESCRIPTION OF PERIODICITY AND PLACE PROGRAMS TO MODEL PITCH PERCEPTION 2.1 PERIODICITY PITCH Figure I shows block diagrams of both periodicity and place algorithms for pitch detection. The periodicity algorithm assumes a correspondence between the basilar membrane and the filter bank and a further correspondence between the hair cell-auditory nerve complex (on the one hand) and elementary pitch detectors EPD 1 through EPDM. The filter designs are based on physiological measurements of Delgutte [14]. At present, 19 filters have been implemented; the frequency covers a 2-kHz range. The ability of these filters to resolve harmonics is a function of the pitch and formant structure of the incoming signal; thus, a given filter, representing a specific place on the basilar membrane, can sometimes perform a place function and, at other times, the same filter can perform a periodicity function. 204655-! 111111E... GLOBAL SIGNAL DETECTION PITCHPITCH "ALGORITHM PIH FILTER BANK ELEMENTARY PITCH DETECTORS (a) PITCH PITCH DETETIONDETETIONDETECTION 4096PT O BASD ONBASED ON INPUT SIGNAL FFT SPECTRAL SEPARATION CORRELATION PEAKS EWITH PEAKS REFERENCE PATTERNS (b) STAGE 1 STAGE 2 Figure 1. (a) Block diagram for the periodicity algorithm and (b) block diagram of the two-stage place algorithm. 2

Neural spiking tends to follow the peaks of the signal. Given an auditory nerve spike, that same nerve cannot respond to further stimulation for a time immediately following the spike; this is called the refractory period. Following this period, the potential differences inside the neuron gradually return to normal, thus steadily increasing the probability of subsequent firings. The global algorithm shown in Figure 1(a) further processes the succession of time intervals between spike occurrences. The algorithm is based on the hypothesis that the higher auditory centers can interpret intervals separated by several spikes to produce five monotonically increasing intervals. Thus, at any instant, each elementary pitch detector (EPD) presents five numbers to the global detector. Each filter output excites two EPDs; the positive set of EPDs spikes on positive excursions of the signals, while the negative set spikes on negative excursions. Thus, there are 2 x 19 = 38 EPDs, each producing five intervals so that there are 5 x 38 = 190 intervals available. The final global periodicity decision is obtained by developing a histogram of these 190 intervals and choosing the mode or maximum of the resultant probability density function. 2.2 PLACE PITCH The underlying hypothesis of place detection is the ability of the auditory system to resolve enough harmonic peaks of the stimulus. This resolution can take place at the periphery or at higher levels. In fact, Houtsma and Goldstein [ 15] have shown that centrally located auditory processes can indeed perceive pitch even if each ear is subjected to a single harmonic of the fundamental frequency. Figure I(b) shows an implementation of a high-resolution spectral analysis via a 4096-point fast Fourier transform. A reliable algorithm that leads to the Goldstein algorithm [161 can be implemented as a two-stage process. Stage I is a version of the Seneff algorithm [17] that performs a statistical analysis of the frequency separation between spectral peaks. Stage 2 is related to the "harmonic sieve" algorithm of Goldstein's pitch perception model, as implemented by Duifhuis [18]. The spectral peaks are correlated with sets of harmonically spaced narrow windows. The chosen sets are based on the "winning pitch" of stage 1. If the winning pitch of stage I is f, the chosen sets include f ±25 Hz. Thus, the results of stage I greatly restrict the range of measurements of stage 2. The combination avoids many ambiguous pitch results. 3

3. PRELIMINARY MODEL RESULTS FOR PSYCHOACOUSTIC STIMULI 3.1 PITCH OF INHARMONIC SIGNALS (SHIFT OF VIRTUAL PITCH) Figure 2 shows several cases for both place and periodicity model responses to inharmonic stimuli. Since people perceive pitch despite the absence of harmonic structure at the pitch frequency, the term "virtual pitch" is used to describe the results. Figures 2(a) and (c) show the model results for different harmonic structures. Figure 2(b) shows a comparison between the human response (dashed line) and the two models. It appears that both place and periodicity models respond similarly. Alst, both models respond qualitatively in the same manner as the human. Because of our 10-kHz sampling rate, the model results are quantized. 32 HARMONICS 1, 2 204855-2 016 / PERIODICITY S/ -.-.,/ - PLACE N -------- PERCEIVED PITCH (Smoorenburg) o0 I III (a) 32 HARMONICS 3, 4, 5 '16 0* o -o.. "- I I I 32 HARMONICS 2, 3, 4, 5 (b) S16 --. o 00 T -~ I I! 5 is 15 25 35 45 (c) Af (In) Hz Figure 2. Shift of Virtual Pitch. (a) Periodicity pitch track for a stimulus consisting of harmonics I and 2, (b) three pitch curves for a stimulus consisting of harmonics 3, 4, and 5, and (c) a stimulus consisting of harmonics 2 through 5 inclusive. 4

3.2 PITCH OF COMB-FILTERED NOISE Figure 3 shows how the models respond to comb-filtered white noise. Ten delays were imposed; they ranged from 12.0 to 2.1 ms. Each of the comb-filtered signals is processed by both models for 160 ms and then followed by a pause of 55 ms. Figure 3 shows the pitch period; the dips in period are due to the "off' parts of the signal. The place algorithm follows the results obtained from psychoacoustics for the initial six of the ten cases. Periodicity also tends to follow this pattern but much less reliably. Interestingly, however, the higher pitches are better represented by the periodicity models. A speculative hypothesis could attribute lower pitch results to a place model and higher pitch results to a periodicity model. 0 12.0 1 E 8.3 W 20 2.0 PLACE I,- 8.0 o Lu I-3.3 5 4.4 2.0 PERIODICITY Figure 3. Pitch of comb-filtered noise, which shows the tracks of the detected periods from both place and periodicity pitch detection algorithms. 3.3 TRANSITION FROM RATE TO FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCY Figure 4 shows the results of the periodicity pitch model. A single period of the repetitive stimulus is shown in the box at the upper left. Every 0.18 s, the pulse rate r is increased in steps. The fundamental frequency f is always r14. Periodicity pitch follows the rate until r = 300 pulses/s and then abruptly switches to follow the fundamental. This is quite analogous to the behavior of human listeners to the same stimulus. For this stimulus, the periodicity model appears to work properly, but the place model does not. 3.4 CIRCULARITY IN PITCH PERCEPTION Figure 5 shows the pitch periods generated by the periodicity model for various fundamental frequencies of the Shepard stimuli. Figure 5 shows that this result is consistent (for the periodicity model) over an octave range of lowest tones. The place model, on the other hand, appears to yield completely ambiguous results. 5

Jean-Claude Risset experimented with a complex Shepard pitch signal consisting of ten components, each of which descends ten octaves but are perceived together as an endless glissando or pitch slide that remains within a single octave register Risset [19-21]. Figure 6 shows the responses of both periodicity and place models to Risset's "Endless Glissando." Both models remain within the single octave during the cycle with the exception of an ambiguous region for the periodicity model near the half octave point. The place algorithm is not ambiguous and seems to track the physical stimulus well. This is in contrast to its response to Shepard tones. A possible reason for this apparent discrepancy is that in our version of the Shepard tones all harmonics were equal, while our version of the Risset stimulus included his amplitude window. Future experiments may shed further light on this issue..10455-4 r AND f 0 500 SINGLE PERIOD OF STIMULUS [ r = RATE RATE OF PULSE TRAIN 400 t- = FUNDAMENTAL 3oo~~ I t7--0 I i, I FREQUENCY DETECTED PITCH FUNDAMENTAL FREQUENCY OF PULSE TRAIN 80 125 166 200 250 300 333 400 500 RATES Figure 4. Rate versus frequency plot from the periodicity pitch algorithm for the Flanagan-Guttman pulse stimulus. 6

DETECTED PITCH 200 217 240 129 138 149 158 169 178 188 196 I I I I i i I I I I I I I 100 110 120 130 140 150 16G 170 180 190 200 210 LOWEST TONE (Hz) 204855-5 PERIODICITY S1.0 I I I I I EI " 0.2 1.4 0 TIME (s) U1 12.0 PLACE 0.2 1.4 TIME (s) Figure 5. Comparison of periodicity and place algorithms for pitch circularit, using a Shepard tone complex. The one-half octave shift (circularity) described in the psychoacoustics literature is contrasted to the arbitrary response from the place algorithm to the same stimuli. 204855.6 12.0 II I 2.0 PERIODICITY 2.0 18.0 TIME (s) 12. PLACE 2.0 18.0 TIME (s) Figure 6. Periodicity and place model responses to Risset's "Endless Glissando." 7

4. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Two models of pitch perception have been implemented, and the response of these models to various psychoacoustic stimuli have undergone preliminary study. Both models successfully track the pitch of a harmonic signal with missing fundamentals. The periodicity model corresponds to psychoacoustic results from human listeners for inharmonic stimuli, periodic pulse train stimuli, and Shepard stimuli. On the other hand, the place model corresponds to psychoacoustic results for inharmonic stimuli, combfiltered noise, and nonsimultaneous harmonics. These results can help psychophysicists speculate on auditory nerve functions above the periphery, including a possible mechanism that might combine the optimal performance of both models. 8

REFERENCES 1. E. DeBoer, "On the 'residue' and auditory pitch perception," in W.D. Keidel and W.D. Neff (eds.), Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. V, Auditory System, Part 3: Clinical and Special Topics, Berlin, Germany: Springer (1976), 479-583. 2. G. van den Brink, "Two experiments on pitch perception: diplacusis of harmonic AM signals and pitch of inharmonic AM signals," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 48, 1355-1365 (1970). 3. G.F. Smoorenburg, "Pitch perception of two-frequency stimuli," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 48, 924-942 (1970). 4. G.A. Miller and W.G. Taylor "The perception of repeated bursts of noise," J. Acousi. Soc. Am. 20, 171-180 (1948). 5. F.A. Bilsen and R.J. Ritsma, "Repetition pitch and its implication for hearing theory," Acustica 22, 63-73 (1969). 6. R.M. Warren, J.A. Bashford, Jr., and J.M. Wrightson, "Infrapitch echo," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 1301-1305 (1980). 7. W.A. Yost, "The dominance region and ripple noise pitch: A test of the peripheral weighting model," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 72, 416--425 (1982). 8. R.M. Warren and J.A. Bashford, Jr., "Broadband repetition pitch: Spectral dominance or pitch averaging?" J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 84, 2058-2062 (1988). 9. A.J.M. Houtsma, T.D. Rossing, and W.H. Wagenaars, Audio Demonstrations, compact disk brochure, Institute for Perception Research (1987). 10. J.L. Flanagan and N. Guttman, "On the pitch of periodic pulses," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 32, 1308-1319 (1960). 11. R.N. Shepard, "Circularity in judgments of relative pitch," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 36, 2346-2353 (1964). 12. 1. Pollack "Decoupling of auditory pitch and stimulus frequency: The Shepard demonstration revisited," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 63, 202-206 (1978). 13. D. Deutsch, "A musical paradox," Music Perception 3, 275-280, (1986). 14. B. Delgutte (private communication, 1990). 15. A.J.M. Houtsma and J.L. Goldstein, "The central origin of the pitch of complex tones: Evidence from musical interval recognition," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 51, 520-529 (1972). 16. J.L. Goldstein, "An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 54, 1496-1516 (1973). 17. S. Seneff, "Real-time harmonic pitch detector," IEEE Trans. Acoust. Speech Signal Process. ASSP- 26, 358-365 (1978). 9

18. H. Duifhuis, L.F. Willems, and R.J. Sluyter, "Measurement of pitch in speech: An implementation of Goldstein's theory of pitch perception," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 71, 1568-1580 (1982). 19. J.C. Risset (Bell Labs, private communication, 1969). 20. C. Dodge and T.A. Jerse, "Synthesis fundamentals," Computer Music: Synthesis, Composition, and Performance, New York: Schirmer Books (1985), 95-97. 21. M.V. Mathews and J.R. Pierce (eds.), "Paradoxical sounds," Current Directions in Computer Music Research, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press (1989), 149-158. 10

REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE Form Approved OUB No. 0704-OI "ao P n rni"l rq bi~fiof t of,. coa;4oo.4 ot,ltormwtcon LS~yW to 0PD8 verjeg.1 o r r oos,oorfln thoo Orm offfor~lqvr,oafj, lcfljs sflrctbn 0.,stl.g Oat..Oour, glaorflg *00r rnt,ul~n flnq 0r OUt.J flqo* '2040v ano reewwifg OW 004.0 Of,flf tln SaOn, fomrwfts Mtganng m,s sb mafo o- way o0.w a"oic- of mts oo nco Of, ".nn "00o f S.oQO, fgfs nor Oucrng n. ortx to WS fof fdalt OW Sennoet 0Do e I f ntonanon ofmo O os no RaotOs t215.efn.ror Oa- O' ey S.4 1204 Afnfglo VA 222024J02 MnO to 0"" 0o M.O aeganb ano SO4f Paovrrs R.a-o.ar1 P-o1-10704-MO&I Was-grosor, DC 20503 1. AGENCY USE ONLY (Leave blank) 2. REPORT DATE 3. REPORT TYPE AND DATES COVERED 28 August 1992 Technical Report 4. TITLE AND SUBTITLE 5. FUNDING NUMBERS Models of Pitch Perception 6. AUTHOR(S) (" -- FI928-9fl-(-rn0)2 Tim Aarset and Ben Gold 7. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES) 8. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION REPORT NUMBER Lincoln Laboratory, MIT P.O. Box -3 TR-964 Lexington, MA 02173-9108 9. SPONSORING/MONITORING AGENCY NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES) 10. SPONSORING/MONITORING AGENCY REPORT NUMBER HQ Air Force Material Command AFMC/STSC ESC-TR-92-119 Wright-Patterson AFB, OH 45433-5001 11. SUPPLEMENTARY NOTES None 12a. DISTRIBUTION/AVAILABILITY STATEMENT 12b. DISTRIBUTION CODE Approved for public release: distribution is unlimited. 13. ABSTRACT (Maximum 200 words) Two pitch perception modeling algorithms are described. The first algorithm models "periodicity- pitch perception. and the second algorithm models place" pitch perception. The two models are now applied to various psychoacoustic stimuli. Both periodicity and place models yield results that are in general agreement with psychoacoustic measurements for the missing fundamental and for inharmonic stimuli. The place algorithm proved to be a better approximation than periodicity for processing comb-filtered noise. Periodicity was more successful for periodic pulse train stimuli. 14. SUBJECT TERMS 15. NUMBER OF PAGES pitch auditory physiology computer modeling and simulation 14 pitch detection acoustics, hearing signal processing algorithms 16. PRICE CODE pitch perception psychoacoustics acoustic/digital signal processing 17. SECURITY CLASSIFICATION 18. SECURITY CLASSIFICATION 19. SECURITY CLASSIFICATION 20. LIMITATION OF OF REPORT OF THIS PAGE OF ABSTRACT ABSTRACT Unclassified Unclassified Unclassified SAR NSN 7540-01-280-5500 Standard Form 298 (Rev 2-89) Prescribed by AMSI Sid. 239-18 298-102

DATC: