Pragmatics and Discourse Analysis. Lecture (6) The Cooperative Principle and Politeness

Similar documents
Lecture (6) The Cooperative Principle and Politeness

Notes on Politeness Chapter 3

Discourse as action Politeness theory

Face-threatening Acts: A Dynamic Perspective

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. communication with others. In doing communication, people used language to say

THE CO-OPERATIVE PRINCIPLE AND IMPLICATURE

AN ANALYSIS OF NEGATIVE POLITENESS STRATEGIES AS FOUND IN TITANIC MOVIE Luthfi Gustri Eldy 1, Yusrita Yanti 2, Elfiondri 2

Pragmatics - The Contribution of Context to Meaning

Cooperative Principles of Indonesian Stand-up Comedy

CHAPTER II REVIEW RELATED LITERATURE. This chapter consisted of many important aspects in analysis the data. The

Introduction to English Linguistics (I) Professor Seongha Rhee

Grice s initial motivations: logic. In logic: A & B = B & A. But consider:

Pragmatics: How do we speak appropriately and politely?

Sample Chapter. Unit 5. Refusing in Japanese. 100 Unit 5

Politeness versus Manipulation

Semantics and Generative Grammar. Conversational Implicature: The Basics of the Gricean Theory 1

3 rd CSE Unit 1. mustn t and have to. should and must. 1 Write sentences about the signs. 1. You mustn t smoke

LEVEL B Week 10-Weekend Homework

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. background, statement of problems, research objective, research significance, and

The jar of marmalade

Inglês CHAPTERS 11 and 12

4-1. Gerunds and Infinitives

CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURES, CONCEPTS, AND THEORITICAL FRAMEWORK. The first subchapter is review of literatures. It explains five studies related

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION. language such as in a play or a film. Meanwhile the written dialogue is a dialogue

Pragmatics Pragmatics is the study of language at the discourse level; or, how language is used.

A Cognitive-Pragmatic Study of Irony Response 3

Six. Unit. What does he do? Target Language. What does he do?

ANALYSIS OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE STRATEGIES IN TRUMP S INTERVIEW TO NEW YORK TIMES 1 Zafar Maqbool Khan, 2 Muhammad Nadeem Anwar

ENGLISH MODULE CONDITIONAL AND MIXED CONDITIONAL

Teaching language for communication: an action- oriented approach

Part A Instructions and examples

Lecture (5) Speech Acts

Irony as Cognitive Deviation

BÀI TẬP TRẮC NGHIỆM GIAO TIẾP MÔN TIẾNG ANH

The phatic Internet Networked feelings and emotions across the propositional/non-propositional and the intentional/unintentional board

Pragmatics and Discourse

Units 1 & 2 Pre-exam Practice

1 st Final Term Revision SY Student s Name:

10 Steps To Effective Listening

Direct and Indirect Speech

Cooperantics Communication skills

UPGRADE 8 CONTENTS. It's TEOG Time 2... p. 30. It's TEOG Time 3... p. 44. It's TEOG Time 4... p. 58. It's TEOG Time 5... p. 72

Commonly Misspelled Words

S. 2 English Revision Exercises. Unit 1 Basic English Sentence Patterns

Elementary Podcast 2-5 Transcript

An Experiment in Methods: Speech Act Theory in the Poems of Wallace Stevens

ENGLISH ENGLISH BRITISH. Level 1. Tests

LEARNING ENGLISH WITH LAUGHTER

A Children's Play. By Francis Giordano

A PRAGMATIC ANALYSIS OF POLITENESS STRATEGIES IN ELIZABETH BANKS PITCH PERFECT 2. A Thesis

UNIT 1 What a wonderful world!

POLITENESS STRATEGIES USED IN COMPLAINT BY INDONESIAN EFL LEARNERS IN MUHAMMADIYAH UNIVERSITY OF SURAKARTA

HERE AND THERE. Vocabulary Collocations. Grammar Present continuous: all forms

1 Family and friends. 1 Play the game with a partner. Throw a dice. Say. How to play

Conversation 1. Conversation 2. Conversation 3. Conversation 4. Conversation 5

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

Communication Mechanism of Ironic Discourse

The present perfect: Key (pp.14-18)

Perspective Difference in Bald on Record between Japanese and English Speakers

POLITENESS AND IRONY PRINCIPLE

SESSION 5 PRACTICING LINGUISTIC COMMUNICATIVE SKILL: READING, WRITING, TRANSLATION, VOCABULARY AND PRONUNCIATION

INSTITUTO NACIONAL 8 TH GRADE UNITS UNIT 6 COUNTABLE AND UNCOUNTABLE NOUNS

Lesson 1: Idioms from Food

1. A. television B. competition C. information D. population. 2. A. explain B. standard C. aware D. receive

Interaction of Face and Rapport in an American TV Talk Show* 1)

ENGLISH FILE Pre-intermediate

Introducing Dialogue Games Lecture 5

A eyes B ears C nose. A did B made C took. A you going to B you re going to C are you going to. A older B oldest C most old. A than B from C as

Lingua Inglese 3. Lecture 5. Searle s Classification of Speech Acts. Representatives: the speaker is committed in

LearnEnglish Elementary Podcast Series 02 Episode 08

0 Aristotle: dejinition of irony: the rhetorical Jigure which names an object by using its opposite name 0 purpose of irony: criticism or praise 0

BBC LEARNING ENGLISH 6 Minute Vocabulary Discourse markers: showing attitude

Lesson 12: Infinitive or -ING Game Show (Part 1) Round 1: Verbs about feelings, desires, and plans

CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE. related object of this study and its related study. It involves, politeness strategy,

DIRECT AND REPORTED SPEECH

I Tom. L the film starts does the film start? In past simple questions, we use did: L you. I you live do you Live?

4 Complete the sentences with pronouns from the list. Example: A Did John call me? B Yes. He called you at six.

Developmental Sets. 1. Set I: (Spanish speaker)

ENGLISH FILE Intermediate

UNIT 3 Past simple OJ Circle the right words in each sentence.

Jennifer L. Fackler, M.A.

Introducing your students to spoken grammar

Grammar: Imperatives Adverbs of sequence Usage: Completing a recipe

When Incongruity Exists: An Analytical Framework of Humor

ENGLISH ENGLISH AMERICAN. Level 1. Tests

THE 'ZERO' CONDITIONAL

THE NON-OBSERVANCE OF THE CONVERSATIONAL MAXIMS: AN ANALYSIS OF THE DIALOGUES IN ARTHUR MILLER S THE CRUCIBLE

(INT HIGH INT / VERSION

Graphic Features of Text-based Computer-Mediated Communication

Instant Words Group 1

Value: Truth / Right Conduct Lesson 1.6

Share and share alike

Say Sorry. Tiny Conversations. 1. Say Sorry Read the dialogue, and practice with a partner.

This exercise focuses on people and their jobs using the most common suffixes.

About the Author. Support. Transcript Learn English Article 118

Logic and argumentation techniques. Dialogue types, rules

MODAL VERBS ABILITY. We can t meet them tomorrow. Can you hear that noise?

9 Guests are allowed to wear casual dress. 11 There's a possibility that the show will be cancelled think that Andrew will collect the money.

ii) Are we writing in French?. iii) Is there a book under the chair? iv) Is the house in front of them?

Transcription:

Pragmatics and Discourse Analysis 1302752 Lecture (6) The Cooperative Principle and Politeness

Grice and the Cooperative Principle Grice s theory is about how people use language. Hearers show some regularity in their production of inferences and speakers exploit this to imply something more than what is stated: Grice was the first to systematically account for this by positing the Cooperative Principle. The Cooperative Principle (CP ) is a tacit agreement between speaker and hearer to cooperate in communication.

What the CP is not... An ideal of fair and honest cooperation amongst human beings. The term cooperation is not used in an everyday sense to refer to people s feelings. It does not designate a prescriptive set of rules and regulations for human interaction.

The Cooperative Principle Make your contribution such as is required, at the stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange in which you are engaged. Grice s suggestion: In their everyday interaction people observe regularities which arise from rational considerations.

Maxims of the Cooperative Principle These rules of conversation were first formulated by Paul Grice (1975) as the Cooperative Principle. This states that we interpret the language on the assumption that a speaker is obeying the four maxims (known as Grice s Maxims) of: 1 QUALITY (BEING TRUE) 2 QUANTITY (BEING BRIEF) 3 RELATION (BEING RELEVANT) 4 MANNER (BEING CLEAR) Make your conversational contribution such as is required, at the stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange in which you are engaged (Grice, 1989 [1967]: 26).

The Maxim of Quality This maxim requires that we only give true information for which we have evidence. Make your contribution one that is true, specifically: (i) Do not say what you believe to be false. (ii) Do not say that for which you lack adequate evidence.

The Maxim of Quality example A: Who won the match yesterday? B: Manchester did. Implicature: Assuming that B is cooperative and provides information for which he has evidence, his reply is true and therefore Manchester won the match; otherwise, B would have provided a different answer.

The Maxim of Quantity This requires that the speaker provides all the necessary information s/he has for the present needs of the partner. (i) Make your contribution as informative as required. (ii) Do not make your contribution more informative than required.

The Maxim of Quantity example A: I ve run out of petrol. B: There is a petrol station round the corner. Implicature: As far as the speaker knows, the station is open and also sells petrol. If neither of the two was true B would not have said so. Therefore, I can find petrol there. A: Have you read Dostoevsky s novels? B: I ve read some. Implicature: B hasn t read all his novels, only some.

The Maxim of Relevance The maxim is very simple: Make your contribution relevant.

The Maxim of Relevance example A: Can I borrow 10 pounds? B: My wallet is in the bedroom. Implicature: Provided B is cooperative and provides a relevant answer, I can borrow 10 pounds. A: How are the trumpet lessons going? B: Not great, but I d rather not discuss it now. Implicature: The trumpet lessons are not going so well.

The Maxim of Manner Be perspicuous, and specifically: avoid obscurity avoid ambiguity be brief be orderly

The Maxim of Manner example A. Do you love me? B. Of course I do. Implicature: provided that B is cooperative, s/he is giving me a clear, unambiguous answer; therefore, B loves me. The woman got out of her car and went straight into the office. Implicature: The woman got out of her car and then went to the office.

Points to Remember... The maxims are not rules that people have to follow in an interaction. People do not always follow these maxims in everyday interaction. Even when the maxims are violated, the hearer assumes that this is done on purpose and looks for particular inferences.

Points to remember... What is at work here is: The hearer assumes that the speaker is being cooperative and, unless there is an indication to the contrary, the speaker: will tell the truth, will say as much as the occasion requires by estimating what is already known, will know what is being discussed, will make sure the hearer(s) understands her/him.

Maxims may be observed John got into Harvard and won a scholarship. I went to the supermarket and I bought some sugar. and means that both linked events occurred, but implicates also temporal progression due to the maxim of manner: be orderly.

Maxims may be violated (because of a clash with another maxim) A: Where does Dave live? B: Somewhere in the South of France This response infringes the first maxim of quantity, but does so in order to avoid violating the second maxim of quality.

Maxims may be flouted A: Will you come out on a dinner date with me? B: Hasn t the weather been lovely recently? B flouts the maxims of quantity and relevance.

Flouting the Maxims The hearer draws inferences as to what the speaker implies in two distinct ways: When the speaker is observing the maxims, inferences will be quite straightforward: A: I need to check my email. B: The laptop is in the kitchen. When the speaker deliberately flouts the maxims and it is obvious that s/he is doing so, this prompts the hearer to look for a different meaning from what is explicitly expressed.

Flouting the Maxims Grice argues that although speakers, usually choose to co-operate, they can also refuse to abide by that principle, or, in other words, flout it. If a maxim is deliberately broken, it is normally done so to achieve a very specific effect and communicate a specific meaning, known as a conversational implicature, in other words, the special meaning created when a maxim is flouted.

Flouting the Maxims example A: How are we getting there? B: Well, we re getting there in Dave s car. Flouts the maxim of quantity (gives less information). Implicature: A will not be travelling with them.

Flouting the Maxim of Quality A: You know, many people here are depending on you. B: Great! That really is a relief Implicature: B s answer is sarcastic; she says something which is obviously untrue, thus implying that the opposite is true. The true meaning here is something along the lines of That really stresses me out.

Flouting the Maxim of Quantity A: Where does Mary work? B: Room 43 or Room 34. Implicature: B does not know which of the two places Mary works. A: What can you tell me about my son s school performance? B: Your son is always well-dressed and he is never late for class.

Flouting the Maxim of Relation A: Do you like your new flat? B: The flowers are looking great! Implicature: B does not like her/his new flat. A: Are you coming to my party this weekend? B: It looks like it s going to rain.

Flouting the Maxim of Manner A. Do you love me? B. I ll ponder on that matter soon and when an answer pops up, it will be delivered to you without haste. Implicature: B is being unnecessarily ambiguous and confusing; therefore, B either does not know or s/he does not want to answer the question. (a couple has just had an argument and are having supper with their small children) A: Do you still think I was wrong? B: Oh, just go to H-E-L-L.

Flouting the Principles on Purpose Maxim of quality: Be true My phone never stops ringing She s got nerves of steel I love it when you forget to tell me you won t be in

Flouting the Principles on Purpose Maxim of relation: Be relevant A: Can I borrow your car this morning? B: It s not insured in your name.

Flouting the Principles on Purpose Maxim of manner: Be clear Avoid ambiguity, obscurity. Be brief and orderly. What is the difference between 1 and 2? 1. Jack and Jill got married and had a baby. 2. Jack and Jill had a baby and got married.

Conversational Implicature How do you understand an utterance? 1. The conventional meanings of words 2. The cooperative principle & the 4 maxims 3. The linguistic and non-linguistic context of the utterance 4. Items of background knowledge 5. The fact that all of the above are available to both participants and they both assume this to be the case (interlocutors have a shared cultural knowledge)

Discuss these examples: What s on TV? Nothing What s your hamburger like? A hamburger is a hamburger. Flouts maxim of quality not true Flouts maxim of manner unclear Did you invite Bella and Cathy? I invited Bella Flouts maxims of manner and quantity so hearer infers Cathy is not invited Did you feed the cats already? Do you see them hanging around? Flouts maxim of relation

Summary The co-operative principle: Grice s principle for explaining how conversational implicatures arise. The CP is supported by four maxims: Quality, quantity, relevance and manner. Implicatures: implications following from the maxims. They are additional or different meanings from the expressed meaning.

Limitations of the Cooperative Principle Different cultures, countries, and communities have their own ways of observing and expressing maxims for particular situations. There is often an overlap between the four maxims. It can be difficult to say which one is operating and it would be more precise to say that there are two or more operating at once.

Relevance Sperber and Wilson went on to suggest that it is Relevance that is the key to understanding how we interpret utterances. It is our assumption of relevance that allows us to explain the many ways in which we use language in ways that cannot be explained simply by lexical, semantic and grammatical knowledge (and which might otherwise flout Grice s maxims): A. How do I get to Carrefour mate? B. It s twenty past twelve. B. There s a petrol station just round the corner. B. You re not going anywhere.

Hedges Because these principles are assumed in normal interaction, speakers rarely mention them. However, there are certain expressions used to mark that speakers may be in danger of not fully adhering to the principles. These expressions are called hedges. The following examples are taken from Yule (1996:38-39):

Hedges Examples Quality: As far as I know, they re married Quantity: As you probably know, I am afraid of dogs. Relation: Not to change the subject, but is this related to the budget? Manner: I m not sure if this makes sense, but the car had no lights.

Infringing a Maxim: Fail to observe a maxim because of their imperfect linguistic performance (a child or a foreign learner): nervousness; drunkenness; excitement; cognitive impairment; incapability of speaking clearly.

Violating Maxims Unlike flouting, violating maxims means that the speaker knows that the hearer will not recover the implicature and will only see the surface truth. In other words the hearer will take the words at face value and act accordingly.

Violating Maxim of Quantity When violating the maxim of quantity, the speaker does not provide the hearer sufficient information. Violating the maxim of quantity means deliberately providing insufficient information so that the hearer will not fully understand the situation. A Does your dog bite? B No. A (Bends down to stroke dog and is bitten ) Ow! But you said it doesn t bite?!. B It s not my dog.

Violating Maxim of Quality When violating the maxim of quality (and therefore being insincere or lying), the speaker is not honest and provides wrong information. It is quite permissible and acceptable in some contexts and cultures, especially a lie that protects or a white lie, the kind that are told to children.

Violating other Maxims When violating maxim of manner, he / she may say everything excepting what the hearer desires to cognize. When violating maxim of relation, here one can observe that the speaker endeavors to change the discussion subject or to deflect the hearer. husband (asks his wife ): How much did that new dress cost? wife (A1) I know, why don t we eat out for a change? [deliberately violating the maxim of relation]. wife (A2): A tiny fraction of my salary, though most probably a very high fraction of the salary of the shop assistant who sold it to me [violating the maxim of manner, avoiding clarity and being deliberately obscure].

Violating other Maxims As violation is defined as the unostentatious or "quiet" non-observance of a maxim, a Speaker who violates a maxim "will be liable to mislead" (Grice, 1975: 49). Violating a maxim is the opposite of flouting a maxim. Violating a maxim prevents or discourages the hearer from seeking for implicatures and encourages their taking utterances at face value.

Politeness

POLITENESS Linguistic action which makes communication possible between competitive parties because it neutralize the potencial for aggression in social interaction. (Brown & Levinston)

Face and Facework Goffman Face: The positive social image we seek to maintain during interaction. The term, Face, idiomatically refers to one's own sense of dignity or prestige in social contexts.

FACE NEGATIVE FACE: The basic claim to territories, personal preserves and in general freedom of action. POSITIVE FACE: The desire that one's self image be appreciated and sanctioned by others.

Brown & Levinson s Politeness Theory Brown & Levinson are socio-linguists Extend Goffman s notion of face Positive face Negative face And focus only on preventive facework And face needs (positive and negative)

Face work PREVENTIVE FACEWORK Face Threat Interaction continues Positive Politeness Negative Politeness

Positive Face Positive Face (the dog) is the desire to be valued and included by relevant or significant others

Negative Face Negative Face (the cat) is the desire to be free from imposition and restraint and to have control of our time, property, space, and resources

Threats to Pos./Neg. Face Social connections make threat to positive and negative face inevitable. We need to give orders, ask favors, deny favors, give feedback, etc. When part of a job requirement, threats are not problematic During social interaction, threats are problematic and should be prevented.

Severity of Face Threatening Acts (FTAs) Severity = Power, Distance, Rank (PDR) Power of speaker over hearer Distance between hearer and speaker (close or distant) Rank of imposition Both social/conventional ranking of threat And idiosyncratic/relational ranking of threat

THREATS TO THE NEGATIVE FACE OF THE HEARER Orders, requests Suggestions, advices Remindings Threats, warnings OF THE SPEAKER Expressing thanks Expressing apology Accepting thanks Accepting apology

THREATS TO THE POSITIVE FACE OF THE HEARER Expressions of disapproval, criticism, complaints, insult Disagreement Irriverence, mention of taboo subjects Expression of violent emotions OF THE SPEAKER Self-humiliation, selfcontraddiction Acting stupid Confessing, admitting guilt or responsability Lack of emotional control (e.g. laughter, tears)

STRATEGIES FOR DOING FTAs Very Efficient 1. Without redressive action (bald on record) 2. Positive Politeness (redressive action) 3. Negative Politeness (redressive action) 4. Off record (hinting) 5. Don't do the FTA Very Polite

POSITIVE POLITENESS STRATEGIES Convey X is admirable/interesting Notice, attend to X s interests, needs, etc. Exaggerate interest, approval, etc. Claim in-group membership with X Use in-group markers (we, us) Claim common point of view, attitude, opinions, knowledge, empathy Give agreement and avoid disagreement Joke Indicate you are taking X s interests or needs into account Give reasons Give gifts to X

NEGATIVE POLITENESS STRATEGIES Don t assume X is willing or able to perform the action Question, hedge Don t try to coerce X and give X option not to act Be indirect Assume X is not likely to do the action (Be pessimistic) Minimize threat Minimize imposition Give deference Apologize Go on record as incurring a debt, or as not indebting H

Politeness includes all the strategies to minimize face threatening acts cooperating in order to protect the mutual vulnerability of face.

NEGATIVE POLITENESS: Linguistic behaviors which signal that the Speaker recognize the Hearer's fundamental right to autonomy (protecting negative face). e.g. Apologizing, being indirect, not coercing, impersonalizing etc. POSITIVE POLITENESS: Linguistic behavior signalling that the Speaker wants/ needs/ appreciates the same things as the Hearer (protecting positive face). e.g. Using in-group identity markers, avoiding disagreement, exaggerating interest, joking etc.

The level of politeness which the Speaker will use to the Hearer depends on three factors: DISTANCE (between S&H): How close the speaker is in social terms to the hearer. POWER (of H): how much power the hearer can exert over the speaker. RANK (of the imposition): how imposing the act is considered to be in the given culture.

Complications As evident in the politeness strategies, the interaction is complicated by the fact that Some FTAs threaten both pos. and neg. face so a particular message might contain both types of strategies. Consider a complaint Some politeness strategies threaten the speaker s positive or negative face while addressing the hearer s face Consider an apology

Politeness across Cultures

Observation about Politeness Across Cultures Cultural values determine which parameters (i.e., face, status, rank, role, power, age, sex, social distance, intimacy, kinship, group membership) interact with each other, and which ones are weighted more heavily in comparison with the others.

Politeness across Cultures Being polite may differ from culture to culture. there are linguistic and paralinguistic means of conveying politeness, distance and respect which do not hold true in every language. Take the classic French tu-vous distinction using this plural of respect and distance makes politeness easier to spot. For learners coming from these or similar languages, the absence of this in English is rather unsettling and difficult to replace with other linguistic tools.

Politeness across Cultures Another politeness indicator is the highly frequent use of please in English and many other languages. In Greek, this is not used as often as it is in English, so people tend to say that Greeks are rude! Yet, informal requests incorporate this please function via other means: 1. The use of the noun suffix ακι which acts as a diminutive and makes the request less direct, or friendlier. 2. A softer intonation while making such a request using an imperative will further make it sound less abrupt.

Politeness across Cultures The Greek learner who translates word for word into English will generally sound aggressive, rude or abrupt, as they will most often be oblivious to the need to replace their own politeness indicators with their English equivalent. No wonder Greek businessmen are rumoured to be rather aggressive and abrupt in their business negotiations

Politeness across Cultures Japanese of course has many different levels of politeness that would be unimaginable in English, German, or French. There are more than a dozen words that mean I in Japanese (watashi neutral standard, watakushi much more formal, boku informal but humble, ore masculine, kind of macho, atashi feminine or effeminate, etc., depending on how polite you want to be, or whether you want to be perceived as a man or as a woman). Women have special feminine particles, such as the particle wa, which have no real meaning other than to bring attention to the female gender of the speaker after every 5 words or so.

Politeness across Cultures You can use the honorific prefix o, which vaguely means yours, with words for things like health and tea, with some words you basically have to use them ( お金 o-kane = money), you can use respectful language ( 敬語,keigo), or humble language ( 丁寧語 ) teineigo), etc.

Politeness across Cultures

Politeness across Cultures

Politeness across Cultures

Politeness across Cultures

Other Models of Politeness House And Kasper s Model Of FTA Realisations House and Kasper developed a taxonomy of politeness markers to account for their data divided according to two major classes: downgraders and upgraders. Post-modern Approaches politeness is not a universal given, but is contested across cultures and, importantly, within cultures. What constitutes polite behaviour is negotiated between speakers and hearers and cannot be predicted by a fixed model.

Instruments of Politeness

Instruments of Politeness Pronouns of address Romance languages - tu vs. vous forms Thai use of pronouns for I and you depends on status, rank, age, sex, social distance/intimacy & kinship/group membership I phŏm/dichăîn chăn uáʔ kuu rau kháu phîi nǔu etc. khun you thəə lur mɨŋ kææ nɔɔŋ naay than etc.

Instruments of Politeness Honorifics Japanese Yamada ga musuko to syokuzi o tanosinda. Speaker H, referent & son L Yamada-san ga musuko-san to o-syokuzi o tanosim-areta. Speaker L, referent & soon H Yamada-san ga musuko to o-syokuzi o tanosim-are-ta. Speaker & son L, referent H Yamada enjoyed dinner with (his/my) son. German Herr Doktor Professor Hűbner English Honorable, Respected, Sir, Excellency

Instruments of Politeness Kinship terms In many Asian languages, kinship terms are often used for people unrelated to the speaker: Uncle / aunt Older sibling Younger sibling etc.

Instruments of Politeness Set formulas Arabic Alla maʔak = God be with you Alla yihfazak = God preserve you Hindi Praņaaam Xuš raho Korean Where are you going? Just over there.

Instruments of Politeness Plurals In many languages (e.g., Russian, Czech, Serbo-Croatian, some dialects of Polish), plurals may be used to show politeness when addressing a single person.

Instruments of Politeness Questions In some societies, questions are used to express politeness, e.g. Inner Circle Englishspeaking cultures. Could you tell me the time?

Instruments of Politeness Indirect speech acts It s cold in here. In Bengali, requests are sometimes made through plain statements, e.g., in a clothing shop Aamaar šarţ dorkaar I need a shirt. In some cultures, talk about some unrelated topic is first indulged in before the real subject is mentioned.

Instruments of Politeness Topicalization and focus In English, topicalization and focus can effect the degree of politeness. If you DON T MIND my asking, where did you get that dress? WHERE did you get that dress, if you don t mind my asking? Which is more polite sounding? Why?

Instruments of Politeness Effort The greater the effort expended in face-maintaining linguistic behavior, the greater the politeness, E.g., I wouldn t dream of it since I know you are very busy, but I am simply unable to do it myself, so. Is this a universal tendency?

Instruments of Politeness Use of little Many languages use the phrase a little to convey the meaning carried by English please in imperatives. Japanese chotto Thai nooy Milwaukee-ese once (as in Come here once )

Instruments of Politeness Hedges Linguistic devices by which a speaker avoids statements that are considered too strong. Hedges are used to reduce friction in that they leave the way open for the respondent to disagree with the speaker and the speaker to retreat. I wonder if you have some surplus cash to lend me some 50 dollars?!

Instruments of Politeness Gaze, gesture, & body posture Japanese bow, exchange business cards with two hands Thais wai, avoid touching the head The ok sign can mean Money (Japan) Zero (France) An obscene comment (Greece)

Exercises on Pragmatics

A.1) Think of a context/scenario in which the following exchanges are meaningful. 1) A.: I have a 14 year old son B.: Well that s right A.: I also have a dog B.: Oh, I m sorry

A.1) Think of a context/scenario in which the following exchanges are meaningful. 2) A: excuse me, do you know the time? B: yes, I do. And B walks away.

A.1) Think of a context/scenario in which the following exchanges are meaningful. 3) Sarah: "I can t agree with you more. " Marie: "Hmmm." (Thinking: "She can t agree with me?! I thought she liked my idea!")

A.1) Think of a context/scenario in which the following exchanges are meaningful. 4) From Ocean s Eleven by Steven Soderbergh, 2001. Rusty-Brad Pitt and Danny-George Clooney meet and start joking on the way in which they are dressed. Danny is wearing a tuxedo, while Rusty is wearing a dress with a very showy shirt. In the original version we have: Rusty: I hoped you were the groom. Danny: Ted Nugent called. He wants his shirt back.

A.2) Think of the possible felicity conditions for the following exchanges. 1) Take a holiday soon. 2) It won't end here. 3) You're taking this too seriously. 4) I deny all knowledge of this scandal. 5) Don t tell Mom!

B) Here are some real examples of promotional signs: 1) We do not tear your clothing with machinery; we do it carefully by hand. 2) Heated attendant parking 3) Baby & Toddler Sale What do they mean?

C) Below are some examples of indirect speech acts. For each one try to identify both the direct and the indirect act. 1. [Customer at a railway ticket-office window] I'd like a day return to Galway. 2. [Travel agent to customer] Why not think about Spain for this summer? 3. [Customer to barman] I'll have the usual. 4. [Mother to child coming in from school] I bet you're hungry. 5. [Doorman at a nightclub to aspiring entrant] Don't make me laugh.

D) Specify two possible illocutionary and perlocutionary forces for each of the following and create a suitable situation/context 1. Are you drunk? 2. I can't hear a word. 3. I had a flat tyre 4. Can you hear me? 5. It is seven o'clock 6. It is getting quite late. 7. It is raining outside. 8. Your bed is not done. 9. Enjoy yourself! 10. good luck! 11. I see no dictionaries here.

E) Assume that you are teaching a course. A fellow instructor approaches you after you have graded a test and asks, How did Mr. Jones do? You respond with Well, he wrote something down for every question. a. Which of Grice's maxims does your response appear to flout? b. What is the implicature raised by your response?

F) You ask a friend, Do you know where Barack Obama is? The friend responds with Well, he didn't meet me for lunch like he was supposed to. a. Which of Grice's maxims does your friend's statement appear to flout? b. What is the implicature raised by your friend's statement?

I) Comment on the following utterances and explain the politeness processes that they show: Look, I m terribly sorry to bother you but would there be any chance of your lending me just enough money to get a railway ticket to get home? I must have dropped my purse and I just don t know what to do. Hey, got change for a quarter? Look, sonny, it might not be advisable to just go pushing your little fingers into this little pie. If you don t mind me asking, where did you get that dress? Goodness, aren t your roses beautiful! I was just coming by to borrow a cup of flour. I ll be done in one second

End of class 06