ADAPTATION TO DISPLACED AND DELAYED VISUAL FEEDBACK FROM THE HAND 1

Similar documents
DELTA MODULATION AND DPCM CODING OF COLOR SIGNALS

Electrical and Electronic Laboratory Faculty of Engineering Chulalongkorn University. Cathode-Ray Oscilloscope (CRO)

THE OPERATION OF A CATHODE RAY TUBE

THE OPERATION OF A CATHODE RAY TUBE

E X P E R I M E N T 1

CATHODE-RAY OSCILLOSCOPE (CRO)

Assessing and Measuring VCR Playback Image Quality, Part 1. Leo Backman/DigiOmmel & Co.

CATHODE RAY OSCILLOSCOPE. Basic block diagrams Principle of operation Measurement of voltage, current and frequency

DEPARTMENT OF THE ARMY TECHNICAL BULLETIN CALIBRATION PROCEDURE FOR AUTOMATIC VIDEO CORRECTOR TEKTRONIX, MODEL 1440 (NSN )

Full Disclosure Monitoring

Heart Rate Variability Preparing Data for Analysis Using AcqKnowledge

ECE 402L APPLICATIONS OF ANALOG INTEGRATED CIRCUITS SPRING No labs meet this week. Course introduction & lab safety

Human Hair Studies: II Scale Counts

Overview of All Pixel Circuits for Active Matrix Organic Light Emitting Diode (AMOLED)

Analysis of WFS Measurements from first half of 2004

CHARACTERIZATION OF END-TO-END DELAYS IN HEAD-MOUNTED DISPLAY SYSTEMS

2 MHz Lock-In Amplifier

Elements of a Television System

Amateur TV Receiver By Ian F Bennett G6TVJ

How to Obtain a Good Stereo Sound Stage in Cars

The Distortion Magnifier

Recommended Operations

LED driver architectures determine SSL Flicker,

BER MEASUREMENT IN THE NOISY CHANNEL

CHAPTER 4 OSCILLOSCOPES

Realizing Waveform Characteristics up to a Digitizer s Full Bandwidth Increasing the effective sampling rate when measuring repetitive signals

Simple motion control implementation

Introduction: Overview. EECE 2510 Circuits and Signals: Biomedical Applications. ECG Circuit 2 Analog Filtering and A/D Conversion

CHAPTER 3 OSCILLOSCOPES AND SIGNAL GENERATOR

Specifications. End-Point Linearity - ±5% F.S., when used with HACO SCR-speed control

Application of Measurement Instrumentation (1)

Reducing tilt errors in moiré linear encoders using phase-modulated grating

Machinery Diagnostic Plots Part 1 ORBIT Back-to-Basics: What does the data really tell us?

OPERATING INSTRUCTIONS FOR SYLVANIA. Type I08 Cathode-Ray Oscilloscope. Sylvania Electric Products Inc. Industrial Apparatus. Emporium, Pennsylvania

Application Note AN-708 Vibration Measurements with the Vibration Synchronization Module

SPECIAL REPORT OF THE SUBCOMMITTEE ON POLARITY STANDARDS 1

MIE 402: WORKSHOP ON DATA ACQUISITION AND SIGNAL PROCESSING Spring 2003

DIGITAL COMMUNICATION

Laser Beam Analyser Laser Diagnos c System. If you can measure it, you can control it!

From the above diagram, the relationship between X1, X2, X a and C a is given by the following equations:

Scanning System S-2100

The Measurement Tools and What They Do

N T I. Introduction. II. Proposed Adaptive CTI Algorithm. III. Experimental Results. IV. Conclusion. Seo Jeong-Hoon

Dynamic re-referencing Microvolt-level measurements with the R&S RTO oscilloscopes

STANDARDS CONVERSION OF A VIDEOPHONE SIGNAL WITH 313 LINES INTO A TV SIGNAL WITH.625 LINES

BASE-LINE WANDER & LINE CODING

ZONE PLATE SIGNALS 525 Lines Standard M/NTSC

University of Utah Electrical & Computer Engineering Department ECE1050/1060 Oscilloscope

Preface. The information in this document is subject to change without notice and does not represent a commitment on the part of NT-MDT.

(Skip to step 11 if you are already familiar with connecting to the Tribot)

Digital Correction for Multibit D/A Converters

Experiment 9 Analog/Digital Conversion

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

RICHLAND COLLEGE School of Engineering Business & Technology Rev. 0 W. Slonecker Rev. 1 (8/26/2012) J. Bradbury

Experimental Results of the Active Deflection of a Beam from a Kicker System

Making the tracks on video tape visible with a magnetic fluid

Communication Lab. Assignment On. Bi-Phase Code and Integrate-and-Dump (DC 7) MSc Telecommunications and Computer Networks Engineering

Agilent PN Time-Capture Capabilities of the Agilent Series Vector Signal Analyzers Product Note

Dual-input hybrid acousto-optic set reset flip-flop and its nonlinear dynamics

3 EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATIONS Caroline Robson. 3.1 Aims and Objectives. 3.2 Experimental Method Set Up of the Test Stand

OSCILLOSCOPE AND DIGITAL MULTIMETER

ENGINEERING COMMITTEE

Experiment 7: Bit Error Rate (BER) Measurement in the Noisy Channel

Exploratory Analysis of Operational Parameters of Controls

ECE 5765 Modern Communication Fall 2005, UMD Experiment 10: PRBS Messages, Eye Patterns & Noise Simulation using PRBS

EA63-7D. Generator Automatic Voltage Regulator Operation Manual. Self Excited Automatic Voltage Regulator

DIGITIZING Pulse Duration Modulated

On Figure of Merit in PAM4 Optical Transmitter Evaluation, Particularly TDECQ

Fig. 1. The Front Panel (Graphical User Interface)

Investigation of Digital Signal Processing of High-speed DACs Signals for Settling Time Testing

CRITICAL FREQUENCY OF FLICKER AS A FUNCTION OF INTENSITY OF ILLUMINATION FOR THE EYE OF THE BEE

What is the lowest contrast spatial frequency you can see? High. x x x x. Contrast Sensitivity. x x x. x x. Low. Spatial Frequency (c/deg)

Synchronization circuit with synchronized vertical divider system for 60 Hz TDA2579C

WINTER 15 EXAMINATION Model Answer

Specification of interfaces for 625 line digital PAL signals CONTENTS

Electric Rotary Modules. Rotary Actuators

Overview. Teacher s Manual and reproductions of student worksheets to support the following lesson objective:

GAMBIT DAC1 OPERATING MANUAL

MODE FIELD DIAMETER AND EFFECTIVE AREA MEASUREMENT OF DISPERSION COMPENSATION OPTICAL DEVICES

PS User Guide Series Seismic-Data Display

Modified Sigma-Delta Converter and Flip-Flop Circuits Used for Capacitance Measuring

4. ANALOG TV SIGNALS MEASUREMENT

Agilent DSO5014A Oscilloscope Tutorial

CCD Element Linear Image Sensor CCD Element Line Scan Image Sensor

For applications from 0.25 to 5 HP, the MD60 is a simple AC Microdrive that can be panel mounted as well as wall or machine mounted.

COHERENCE ONE PREAMPLIFIER

EAN-Performance and Latency

Digital Lock-In Amplifiers SR850 DSP lock-in amplifier with graphical display

PulseCounter Neutron & Gamma Spectrometry Software Manual

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

Evaluation of the Effects of Frame Time Variation on VR Task Performance

Monitor QA Management i model

Efficient 500 MHz Digital Phase Locked Loop Implementation sin 180nm CMOS Technology

INSTRUMENT CATHODE-RAY TUBE

J R Sky, Inc. tel: fax:

Module 8 : Numerical Relaying I : Fundamentals

frequencies up to 100 cycles per second. The delay is required to be variable from 0.01 to 10 seconds, +5 milliseconds.

Patchmaster. Elektronik. The Pulse generator. February 2013

disk. Students use the system by referencing a problem in the library and entering parameter values

INSTRUMENT CATHODE-RAY TUBE

Transcription:

Journal ol Experimental Psychology 1966, Vol. 72, No. 6, 887-891 ADAPTATION TO DISPLACED AND DELAYED VISUAL FEEDBACK FROM THE HAND 1 RICHARD HELD, AGLAIA EFSTATHIOU, AND MARTHA GREENE Massachusetts Institute of Technology S viewed an oscilloscope trace of a short bar which appeared at the position of his nonvisible right hand. The motion of the trace was equivalent to the movements of the hand, which were restricted to a frontal plane. The bar could be optically displaced by 20 diopter prisms, either to the left or right, and its motion could be made to lag behind that of the hand by 1 of 6 delay intervals ranging from 0.0 to 3.0 sec. Results show that the adaptation to displacement found with no delay is completely eliminated under all delay intervals, including the minimum of 0.3 sec. In recent years investigators have experimented with devices which introduce a time delay between the occurrence of an event and its registration at the eye. Normally, as 6" moves his hand, motion of its retinal image follows with a negligible lag caused by the transmission time of light. However, the new devices have been used to introduce delays in visual feedback ranging from a fraction of a second up to several seconds. A few authors have reported severe loss of control in self-paced, visually guided movements (Kalmus, Fry, & Denes, 1960; Smith, McCrary, & Smith, 1960) as a consequence of delay. However, extensive experiments of Ferrell and Sheridan (Ferrell, 1965; Sheridan & Ferrell, 1963) have shown that 5s will usually adopt a simple strategy of "move and wait" and suffer no such loss of control. Use of this strategy allows time to be traded for accuracy in the performance of manipulative tasks under delay. Although the appropriate strategy 1 This research has been supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (Grant M-7642), the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (Grant NsG-496), the United States Air Force Office of Scientific Research (AF-AFOSR 3S4-63), and the Rockefeller Foundation. 887 apparently precludes loss of visual control of manipulation under delay, we have reason to suspect that the delay should have an effect on at least one aspect of eye-hand coordination. A series of experimental studies has demonstrated the role of movementproduced visual feedback in adaptation to various types of sensory rearrangement (Held & Freedman, 1963; Pick & Hay, 1965) as well as in the early development of sensorimotor coordination (Held & Hein, 1963). Because of spatial and kinematic relations, these feedback signals are correlated with output signals to the musculature responsible for the movements that produce the change in retinal stimulation. We have advanced the idea that this correlated information is basic for adaptation to optical displacement produced by wedge prisms and to other forms of rearrangement. Confirming this notion are the findings that when S views his hand optically displaced and with the normal relation between innervation of limb muscle and actual movement of the limb upset by varying external forces, there is a marked reduction of adaptation to the displacement (Abplanalp & Held, 1965; Efstathiou, 1963). Delay complicates the normal relation

R. HELD, A. EFSTATHIOU, AND M. GREENE between spatial dispositions of the hand and its retinal image. Under delay the position of the retinal image at any given time is related to the past position of the hand as a function of the delay interval and the path and velocity of movement of the hand. If, as we suspect, the nervous system cannot factor out the effects of these variables the result should be equivalent to decorrelation of output and feedback signals. Consequently, adaptation to a constant visual displacement, such as is produced by prism goggles, should be reduced. Comparison of the magnitudes of adaptation to displacement under normal, no delay conditions with those produced under delay should provide a sensitive test of the effect of delay on the visuomotor control system. METHOD Apparatus. During exposure S, seated on a stool, firmly grasped a knob at the end of a control stick, 17.5 in. long, whose other end was fixed to a girnbal. Movement of 5"s hand was then restricted to the surface of a sphere of 17.5 in. radius tangent to his frontal parallel plane at a distance of 14 in. from his eyes. The movements were further limited to right-left and up-down components of displacements in this plane of 15 and 11 in., respectively, and rotations around an axis along the length of the control stick. Movements of the stick actuated three potentiometers of which one was responsive to rotation and the other two to the two components of translatory displacement of the hand. (See Appendix for a more complete discription). The potentiometers modulated three voltages which could be fed either directly to an oscilloscope or to the recording heads of a tape transport system. In the latter case, delays of required duration were produced by varying the length of tape between recording and playback heads. Speed of tape transport was constant. The outputs of the playback heads were then fed to the oscilloscope. The voltages actuating the oscilloscope produced rotational and translational displacements of the scope trace (a straight line segment, li in. long and i in. wide) across the scope face. These motions were proportional in magnitude and direction to those of the hand which actuated the system. The scope trace was optically projected onto a ground-glass screen so that its magnified displacements were equivalent to those of the hand. The 6" viewed the screen in a fully reflecting mirror in an otherwise dark room. The position and orientation of the mirror was such as to make the virtual image of the trace remain superimposed on the invisible hand under conditions of no-delay and no constant visual displacement. For convenience, wedge prisms were used to displace the image of the hand although mechanical or electronic means might have been used. Before and after exposure, 6" was tested in a separate apparatus (Held & Gottlieb, 1958). He marked, with his right hand, the virtual images of two targets viewed binocularly in a fully reflecting mirror that obscured his hand. The targets appeared to lie on a surface whose location approximated that in which his hand moved during exposure. Throughout the experiment, S's head was held by a bite board. Subjects. Male, college undergraduates served as 5"s. All were naive as to the object of this experiment. Procedure.- Each of 24 6"s was first run in two sessions without time delay. Half of the 6"s were exposed with base left (BL) prisms first followed by base right (BR) prisms on the next day; the other half were given the reversed sequence. The sequence of events within each session was as follows: (a) Preexposure marking: 5" alternately marked the apparent position of each of two visual targets for a total of 20 marks, withdrawing his hand between markings. (&) Exposure: 6" grasped the knob of the apparatus firmly and moved it from left to right and back at 21 c/min to the beat of a metronome. He was instructed to watch the oscilloscope trace as he viewed it through a pair of 20 diopter prisms oriented either BR or BL, depending on the session, (c) Postexposure marking: After 2, 6, 14, and 30 min. of cumulated duration of exposure to the prisms, 6" was tested in a manner identical to that of the preexposure marking. From the 24 5s tested, we selected 6 who showed significant and approximately equal adaptation under both BR and BL exposures. Experiments with each of these 6 5s were then conducted in 12 further sessions. The procedure of each of the next 10 sessions was essentially identical to that of the first 2 except for the fact that in each session 5"s visual feedback from his hand movements

ADAPTATION TO DISPLACED AND DELAYED VISUAL FEEDBACK 889 was subjected to a constant time lag whose magnitude was varied from session to session. The delays used were 0.3, O.S, 0.9, 1.7, and 3.3 sec. The 10 sessions using combinations of each of the five delays with each of the two prism orientations were presented to S's in varied sequences. The final 2 sessions were identical to the first 2 in which no delay was introduced. RESULTS Pre- to postexposure shifts for each exposure duration were computed for all delays. The 5"s mean shifts, averaged across the two target locations, were computed for all six magnitudes of time lag (0.0-3.3 sec.). The shifts for BR and BL conditions were also averaged to yield a single score for each condition of delay. A preliminary analysis of variance had shown that 6"s shifts during the first two no-delay sessions did not differ significantly from those obtained in the last two no-delay sessions; therefore, the averaged shifts of these four sessions were used for further analysis. The 5"'s mean shifts are presented in Fig. 1. With time lag in the feedback loop, none of these mean shifts was significantly different from zero by one-tailed t tests at the.05 level. However, with no delay the mean shifts after 2, 6, and 14 min. of cumulative exposure were significant beyond the.01 level of confidence and after 30 min. beyond the.001 level. An analysis of variance of the mean shifts for all magnitudes of delay indicated that the magnitude of delay and cumulative exposure main effects were highly significant (p <.001). This indicated that, first, the introduction of time delay differentially affected adaptation to displacement, and second, that adaptation to displacement was influenced by the amount of time that 6" was exposed to prisms. A second analysis of variance in which the shifts obtained under no-delay sessions were omitted indicated that» 0.8- FIG. 1. Cumulated shifts in marking visible targets. the amount of delay main effect, failed to reach significance at the.05 level implying that amount of delay, ranging from 0.3 to 3.3 sec., did not differentially affect the amount of shift resulitng from exposure. The cumulative exposure main effect was significant (p <.001). However, as mentioned above none of the mean shifts reached significance under delay conditions. The general trend of the curves for delayed feedback (Fig. 1) was not consistently in the adaptive direction. Since the interaction between magnitude of delay and cumulative exposure was not significant, the trends of the curves for the several delays do not differ significantly. Consequently, although cumulative exposure entails significant change, the variation is not of the type that we identify with adaptation. An analysis of variance of the scatter (standard deviation in right-left dimension) in markings of individual target points showed no significant changes over the period of exposure. CONCLUSIONS The results indicate that the correlating mechanism, believed to account for adaptation, cannot handle a feedback signal

890 R. HELD, A. EFSTATHIOU, AND M. GREENE delayed by as little as 0.3 sec. Such delayed signals, then, produce one of the effects that we have ascribed to decorrelation. Of great interest are the effects of delays of less than 0.3 sec. now under study. Such delays are of the order of magnitude of transmission times in the central nervous system. APPENDIX Electronics of Delayed Feedback Apparatus The electronic portion of the apparatus can be described as follows: 5"s hand held a control stick which operated a gimbal arrangement consisting of two linear voltage divider potentiometers for X and Y directions of translational movement and a sinecosine potentiometer for rotation (Fig. 2). The two linear potentiometers were fed a balanced dc voltage of ± ISO v. Any deviation of the control stick from the center position resulted in a voltage corresponding in magnitude and polarity to the magnitude and direction of the stick movement. The sine-cosine potentiometer was fed a 60 cps ac voltage of about 7 v. RMS which was used to provide a straight-line trace, rotatable on the oscilloscope screen. The outputs of the sine-cosine potentiometer consisted of two voltages, corresponding in magnitude to the sine and cosine of the rotational displacement of the control stick from a zero position. The sin signal was added to X and cos 9 to Y thereby superimposing rotational on translational information. The output of each adder consisted of a dc voltage varying in polarity and magnitude with a 60 cps ac voltage varying in magnitude superimposed upon it. Each composite signal was then fed into an analog data recorder where it modulated a 7,500 cps carrier and was then recorded on magnetic tape. Each signal was picked up on a multichannel playback head placed a short distance from the recording head, thus introducing a delay into the system. This delay was directly proportional to the distance between the record and playback heads and inversely proportional to the speed of the tape. The tape speed remained constant at 7\ in/sec for the 'entire experiment. The played-back signal was demodulated and amplified. DIAGRAM OF DELAY APPARATUS + I50V (SIN 8iX-ERROR)IO - 150V (COSe±Y-ERROR)IO FIG. 2. Block diagram of apparatus used to introduce delay into the hand-eye feedback loop.

ADAPTATION TO DISPLACED AND DELAYED VISUAL FEEDBACK 891 At the same time, another channel of the tape recorder was operated with shorted input to produce a signal corresponding to noise and drift generated by the tape recorder. This error signal was subtracted from the two information channels, thus eliminating any error introduced into the information by the tape recorder. The signals were then amplified 10 times and applied to the vertical and horizontal channels of the oscilloscope. The adders, subtracters, and amplifiers were made using standard analog computer operational amplifiers with external resistor networks to obtain the proper functions. The modulator/tape recorder/demodulator unit was a Mnemotron Model 200 with a special adjustable idler roller to give several different amounts of delay. REFERENCES ABPLANALP, P., & HELD, R. Effects of decorrelated visual feedback on adaptation to wedge prisms. Paper read at Eastern Psychological Association. Atlantic City, N. J., 1965. EFSTATHIOU, A. Correlated and de-correlated visual feedback in modifying eyehand coordination. Paper read at Eastern Psychological Association, New York, 1963. FERRELL, W. Remote manipulation with transmission delay. IEEE Trans, hum. Factors Elect., 1965, HFE-6, 24-32. HELD, R., & FREEDMAN, S. Plasticity in human sensorimotor control. Science, 1963, 142, 455-462. HELD, R., & GOTTLIEB, N. Technique for studying adaptation to disarranged eyehand coordination. Percept, mot. Skills, 1958, 8, 83-S6. HELD, R., & HEIN, A. Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visuallyguided behavior. /. comp. physiol. Psychol, 1963, 56, 872-876. KALMUS, H., FRY, D. B., & DENES, P. Effects of delayed visual control on writing, drawing, and tracing. Lang. Speech, 1960, 3, 96-108. PICK, H. L., & HAY, J. C. A passive test of the Held reafference hypothesis. Percept, mot. Skills, 1965, 20, 1070-1072. SHERIDAN, T. B., & FERRELL, W. R. Remote manipulative control with transmission delay. IEEE Trans, hum. Factors Electron., 1963, HFE-4, 25-29. SMITH, W., MCCRARY, J., & SMITH, K. Delayed visual feedback and behavior. Science, 1960, 132, 1013-1014. (Received October 13, 1965)