Koinophilia and Human Facial Attractiveness

Similar documents
The relationship between shape symmetry and perceived skin condition in male facial attractiveness

Why are average faces attractive? The effect of view and averageness on the attractiveness of female faces

Running head: FACIAL SYMMETRY AND PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS 1

Sexual Selection I. A broad overview

Sexual Selection I. A broad overview

PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS. Elaine Hatfield and Richard L. Rapson. University of Hawai i

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

Koinophilia revisited: the evolutionary link between mate selection and face recognition

Domain Specificity in Human Symmetry Preferences: Symmetry is Most Pleasant When Looking at Human Faces

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

Dissociating Averageness and Attractiveness: Attractive Faces Are Not Always Average

WHY DO PEOPLE CARE ABOUT REPUTATION?

Antiquity, 83(322),

This is an Open Access document downloaded from ORCA, Cardiff University's institutional repository:

Dynamics of aesthetic appreciation

The Bowerbirds and the Bees: Miller on Art, Altruism and Sexual Selection. Catherine Driscoll* Dept. of Philosophy. North Carolina State University

STABILIZING AND DIRECTIONAL SELECTION ON FACIAL PAEDOMORPHOSIS


Toward a New Comparative Musicology. Steven Brown, McMaster University

Course Description: Required Texts:

Mating Intelligence, Moral Virtues, and Methodological Vices

Anthro 1401, University of Utah Evolution of Human Nature Study Guide. Alan Rogers

To what extent can we apply the principles of evolutionary theory to storytelling?

SOCI 421: Social Anthropology

Missouri Show-Me Standards Addressed: Knowledge SC 4 Performance 1.6, 3.5, 3.6, 4.1

SYSTEM-PURPOSE METHOD: THEORETICAL AND PRACTICAL ASPECTS Ramil Dursunov PhD in Law University of Fribourg, Faculty of Law ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION

Master of Arts in Psychology Program The Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences offers the Master of Arts degree in Psychology.

Psychology PSY 312 BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR. (3)

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

Construction of a harmonic phrase

But, if I understood well, Michael Ruse doesn t agree with you. Why?

The Beauty Clock Chapter 1 Ebook excerpt

Facial symmetry and the perception ofbeauty

PROFESSORS: Bonnie B. Bowers (chair), George W. Ledger ASSOCIATE PROFESSORS: Richard L. Michalski (on leave short & spring terms), Tiffany A.

Teacher Edition. AlphaWorld. Using Colour. Written by Hannah Reed

Consumer Choice Bias Due to Number Symmetry: Evidence from Real Estate Prices. AUTHOR(S): John Dobson, Larry Gorman, and Melissa Diane Moore

EXTRACT FROM THESIS BY BC JONES (2004) NB evolutionary advantage view of symmetry. preferences referred to in this manuscript as good

What do We Know about Earedness?: A Review of Studies

To those that have, shall be given

Monday 15 May 2017 Afternoon Time allowed: 1 hour 30 minutes

This excerpt from. The Origins of Music. Nils L. Wallin, Björn Merker and Steven Brown, editors The MIT Press.

Age differences in women s tendency to gossip are mediated by their mate value

Keynote speech evolutionary biology Example of an existing collaboration and highlight of recent research results A Keynote

Research Paper Instructions Ethology and Behavioral Ecology Spring 2010

Are human faces more attractive with glasses?

Semiotics of culture. Some general considerations

Influence of timbre, presence/absence of tonal hierarchy and musical training on the perception of musical tension and relaxation schemas

Region 11 Math & Science Teacher Academy Partnership Life Science 7-12 August 11, 2011

Sudhanshu Gautam *1, Sarita Soni 2. M-Tech Computer Science, BBAU Central University, Lucknow, Uttar Pradesh, India

Psychology. PSY 199 Special Topics in Psychology See All-University 199 course description.

Are Random Drift and Natural Selection Conceptually Distinct?

The Origin of Species The Making of a Theory

Media Contacts PI. Delia Nicholls +61 (0) Rebecca Fitzgibbon +61 (0)

Do women s preferences for symmetry change across the menstrual cycle?

Behavioral and neural identification of birdsong under several masking conditions

Chapter 12: Introduction to Module 2 Evolution

Comparison, Categorization, and Metaphor Comprehension

Darwinian populations and natural selection, by Peter Godfrey-Smith, New York, Oxford University Press, Pp. viii+207.

UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH ALABAMA PSYCHOLOGY

TROUBLING QUALITATIVE INQUIRY: ACCOUNTS AS DATA, AND AS PRODUCTS

DEMOGRAPHIC DIFFERENCES IN WORKPLACE GOSSIPING BEHAVIOUR IN ORGANIZATIONS - AN EMPIRICAL STUDY ON EMPLOYEES IN SMES

Are Random Drift and Natural Selection Conceptually Distinct?

The Effects of Web Site Aesthetics and Shopping Task on Consumer Online Purchasing Behavior

Beatty on Chance and Natural Selection

Sex differences in preferences for humor produced by men or women: Is humor in the sex of the perceiver? [word count = <2500]

ARChive Online ISSN: The International Conference : Cities Identity Through Architecture and Arts (CITAA)

Interdepartmental Learning Outcomes

Aposematic Model vs. Sexual Selection Model of Human Evolution

Boyd, Robert and Richerson, Peter J., The Origin and Evolution of Cultures, Oxford University Press, 2005, 456pp, $35.00 (pbk), ISBN X.

Chapter 2 Christopher Alexander s Nature of Order

December 16, 1997 Outline

Natural Scenes Are Indeed Preferred, but Image Quality Might Have the Last Word

Aristotle on the Mechanism of Inheritance

PSYCHOLOGY (PSY) Psychology (PSY) 1

University of Stirling, Stirling, FK9 4LA, Scotland, UK

High School Photography 1 Curriculum Essentials Document

Brain.fm Theory & Process

How to Obtain a Good Stereo Sound Stage in Cars

Paradox, Metaphor, and Practice: Serious Complaints and the Tourism Industry

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

This essay provides a critical review

Radiating beauty" in Japan also?

Significant Differences An Interview with Elizabeth Grosz

A Musical Species. By Caroline Atkinson

1 Introduction to evolutionary psychology

What do our appreciation of tonal music and tea roses, our acquisition of the concepts

Psychology. Department Location Giles Hall Room 320

Expressive performance in music: Mapping acoustic cues onto facial expressions

Psychology. Psychology 499. Degrees Awarded. A.A. Degree: Psychology. Faculty and Offices. Associate in Arts Degree: Psychology

A Note on: Lumaca & Baggio (2017) Cultural Transmission and Evolution of Melodic Structures in

Bio 1 Scientific Term Paper

GENERAL COURSE SEQUENCE IN PSYCHOLOGY EFFECTIVE WITH SPELMAN CLASS OF 2019 AND BEYOND FRESHMAN YEAR SOPHOMORE YEAR JUNIOR YEAR SENIOR YEAR

Absolute Memory of Learned Melodies

Neuroculture On the implications of brain science

Supplementary Figures Supplementary Figure 1 Comparison of among-replicate variance in invasion dynamics

Coastal Carolina University Faculty Senate Consent Agenda March 4, 2015 COLLEGE OF HUMANITIES AND FINE ARTS

Anthropomorphism. the rationalization of animal or system behavior through superposing aspects of the human observer.

Biology, Self and Culture. From Different Perspectives

9.20 M.I.T Lecture #26 Critique of Cultural determinism

KINDS (NATURAL KINDS VS. HUMAN KINDS)

Transcription:

Koinophilia and Human Facial Attractiveness Aishwariya Iyengar, Rutvij Kulkarni and T N C Vidya When photos of individual faces are combined together to give an averaged face, people find such averaged faces to be more attractive than the original, individual faces. This preference for the average phenotype can be described under koinophilia. In this article, we explore the reasons why averaged faces may be attractive. Koinophilia and Human Facial Attractiveness What makes a face attractive? There have been many studies on this topic, in the form of presenting people with photos of different faces and finding out what is common about the faces that people rate as being attractive. Some studies have found that people from different races and cultures rate the same specific faces as very attractive, suggesting that there could be some universal structural features that are attractive to humans. Preferences for attractive faces are seen even amongst infants, with infants preferring the same faces that adults judged as being attractive [1]. Other studies have found that different populations may have different standards of attractiveness [2]. People may also adjust their standards of beauty based on what they have experienced and, therefore, the media can influence beauty standards [3]. However, one very interesting and consistent finding regarding facial attractiveness has been that people find the average of faces in a population to be attractive (Figure 1). This was discovered in the 19th century by Francis Galton in an attempt to fashion a prototype for the criminal face [4]. He discovered that when multiple photographs of faces were superimposed, the resultant face from the composite photograph looked more appealing than individual faces from any of the original contributing photographs. Much later, with the advent of computers, Langlois and Roggman averaged faces using computers and they also found 1 Aishwariya Iyengar is a firstyearphdstudentat JNCASR. With a background in Zoology and Biochemistry, she studies the behavioural neurogenetics of fruit flies. 2 Rutvij Kulkarni is a first year Integrated PhD student who studied Life Sciences and Biochemistry as an undergraduate. He is interested in evolutionary biology and neuroscience. 3 TNC Vidya is interested in animal behaviour and phylogeography, and teaches these at JNCASR. Her primary research currently is on the social organization and behaviour of Asian elephants. Keywords Koinophilia, attractiveness, averaged faces, recognition, mate choice. 311

GENERAL ARTICLE Figure 1. Nine individual faces (a) and a composite face of these nine faces (b). Courtesy: L DeBruine and B Jones, Face Research Lab (facelab.org). See http:// faceresearch.org/demos/average for an interactive averaging of faces. that people judged the averaged faces to be more attractive than the individual faces [5]. This preference for the average has been termed koinophilia (koinos, Greek = common, average; philos, Greek = love or fondness) [6]. Averaged Faces and Symmetry Why does a composite (averaged) face look more attractive than an individual face? One explanation is symmetry [7,8] since composite faces are more symmetric than their original images. Since individual faces may be asymmetric in different ways, by averaging several individual faces, asymmetries may cancel out one another and result in a composite face that is more symmetric. Symmetry is thought to be preferred because it possibly indicates developmental stability in a changing environment, which would 312 RESONANCE April 2015

be an indicator of genetic quality in an individual. Developmental stability is the ability of an organism to buffer its development against environmental or genetic disturbances and produce a specific phenotype. If individuals are not of high genetic quality, they may not be able to buffer their development against environmental fluctuations and this would result in asymmetries. It has been shown that people make judgments of health based on symmetry and, therefore, symmetry may serve as an honest signal of mate quality. Animals other than humans, such as swordtail fish, zebra finches, sticklebacks, and, possibly, peafowl, also prefer symmetry over asymmetry [9 11]. However, it was also found that while symmetry played a role in the perception of attractiveness in humans, its role was significant only in those faces that were initially rated as very unattractive, and perfect symmetry was not found to be necessarily attractive [12]. Moreover, averaged faces were attractive in the absence of symmetry also (when photographs of faces in profile were used) [13]. There is also another view that symmetry may not really be an indicator of genetic quality, and that preference for symmetry may simply arise from the cognitive processes associated with the mechanism of recognition in organisms [14]. Symmetry is thought to be preferred because it possibly indicates developmental stability in a changing environment, which wouldbeanindicator of genetic quality in an individual. Averaged Faces and Familiarity Another characteristic of averaged faces is that they are familiar looking [12]. Familiarity has been shown to make faces appear more attractive [15]. Just as genetic quality is a possible evolutionary reason for symmetry to be preferred, what would be a possible adaptive reason for familiar faces to be preferred? The averaged face is an average of facial configurations, which would, therefore, be perceived as a prototype face of the population. (This assumes a normal distribution otherwise the modal value should be considered as the prototype face, not the mean.) Facial recognition is an important cognitive ability in animals such as humans, in which social communication via visual cues plays a significant role in day-to-day life. It is likely that the brain recognises faces by remembering an average prototype and then comparing deviations 313

GENERAL ARTICLE Koinophilia is thought to have evolved as a strategy for mate selection because common phenotypes are likely to be safer than mutants. of observed faces against it rather than by remembering exact details of specific faces [16,17]. If only deviations are noticed, average faces would appear familiar. Therefore, koinophilia would facilitate recognition of individuals of the same species and appropriate sex during mate recognition, since identifying conspecifics (individuals of same species) and the correct sex would be easier when a face is average. Average faces would, however be difficult to recognize individually as distinct individuals. Koinophilia and Mate Selection Figure 2. Assortative mating, in which individuals prefer to mate with others having similar trait values (positive assortative mating). The frequency distribution of the trait values is shown, with pairings between insects having the same wing colour. In koinophilic mating, insects of all wing colours shown here would prefer to mate with those having the intermediate wing colour (orange-red). Koinophilia is thought to have evolved as a strategy for mate selection because common phenotypes are likely to be safer than mutants (if the advantages or disadvantages of mutations can be ascertained only retrospectively and not at the time that they arise) [6]. Koinophilic mating (in which individuals mate with conspecifics showing the average phenotype) is different from assortative mating (Figure 2), in which individuals choose mates based on their own phenotype as opposed to choosing the average or common phenotype, irrespective of their own phenotype. Commonness would indicate that the phenotype was well suited to the environment and is, therefore, likely to have high fitness. If it were true that the common phenotype was the best suited to the environment and if this phenotype were heritable, koinophilia as a phenomenon would spread through a population in which it arose (assuming that koinophilia is itself heritable). Interestingly, koinophilia could, therefore, also be a mechanism by which traits show a distribution that mimics stabilizing selection, just as assortative mating can give rise to trait distributions akin to that obtained through disruptive selection, assuming that the traits are heritable (see Figure 3). Perhaps koinophilia is a mechanism that maintains the traits involved in species recognition under stabilizing selection. Another possibility is that there is some other force that maintains speciesspecific traits under stabilizing selection and koinophilia is an adaptation for selecting individuals given that distribution. 314 RESONANCE April 2015

Koinophilia: Further Thoughts It has been suggested that koinophilia could also be a mechanism for speciation [6]. If individuals of a koinophilic species occupied different geographic areas with different distributions of phenotypes, the average phenotype in each area would be different, and individuals from other areas would be avoided as mates, leading to reproductive isolation. If the variability in trait values is reduced because of selection of the average phenotype, this may result in a lack of intermediate morphologies between species, as seen in the fossil record [18]. However, this would require multiple species to show koinophilia, as well as other mechanisms that lead to the initial differences in phenotypic distributions across populations. Figure 3. Types of selection. The frequency distributions of trait values (phenotypes) are shown before selection and after selection. The arrows within the three panels in the middle point towards the trait values that are selected against. The direction in which the distribution will move after selection is shown by arrows below the three panels on the right. The population mean will move towards the right under the directional selection shown above, while it will remain unchanged under stabilizing and disruptive selection. The variance in the trait decreases under stabilizing selection, while it increases under disruptive selection. Courtesy: http://www.wikilectures.eu/ index.php/file:selectiontypes. png While koinophilia has been proposed as a mechanism that can explain diverse processes, from mate choice to speciation [18,19], it cannot satisfactorily explain mate choice based on sexual ornaments (such as deer stag antlers or the peacock s tail) or the rare male advantage (when the rarest males in the population are sometimes preferred by females). However, there seems to be some confusion in the literature about species recognition versus 315

GENERAL ARTICLE When choosing the correct species and sex, one would expect individuals to choose those individuals whose trait values are close to the population average in order to minimize acceptance error. If symmetry is indeed reflective of genetic quality and developmental stability is heritable, symmetrywouldbea trait for which high values would be preferred both in the context of species recognition and individual preference. individual recognition during the process of mate selection, in the context of preference for the average. Mate selection involves choosing individuals of the same species and appropriate sex, and then discriminating between individuals of this set of desirable individuals. When choosing the correct species and sex, one would expect individuals to choose those individuals whose trait values are close to the population average in order to minimize acceptance error (the mistake of wrongly identifying an undesirable individual as a desirable individual). However, individuals should choose mates based ontrait values that areaway from the mean in the correct direction when choosing amongst a set of individuals of the appropriate gender and species. The traits used for species and sex identification, therefore, need not be the same as those used for individual discrimination. For example, secondary sexual characteristics are not expected to be useful in species identification but can be useful in individual discrimination. Therefore, koinophilia would not be expected with respect to such characteristics if it were a mechanism for choosing the correct species. Interestingly, since the averaged face is not only a prototype face of the population, but is also rated as an attractive face, faces may be used for species/sex and individual discrimination simultaneously. One would then expect different characteristics of the face to be used for discrimination at different levels. For instance, face shape at a broader level may indicate human faceness while skin texture or details of face shape may indicate individual quality. Composite faces have blemishes removed and better skin texture, and it is known that skin texture is one of the cues used to rate attractiveness. This could be one reason why composite faces are preferred. If symmetry is indeed reflective of genetic quality and developmental stability is heritable, symmetry would be a trait for which high values would be preferred both in the context of species recognition (because this would represent the population average) and individual preference (because the averaged face would represent a high, not average, degree of symmetry, which would be indicative of quality). Attractiveness also de- 316 RESONANCE April 2015

Box 1. Glossary Developmental stability: This is the ability of an organism to buffer its development against disturbances (environmental or genetic) and produce a predicted phenotype from a specific genotype [25]. Fitness: The ability of individuals of a particular genotype or phenotype to contribute offspring to the next generation. This can be assessed in absolute terms or relative to other genotypes/phenotypes. Trait value: A distinct variant of a phenotypic character, for e.g., black is a trait value of the trait hair colour. Natural selection: This is a natural process by which heritable biological traits change in frequency in a population because of differential reproductive success of individuals that carry variants of these traits. In other words, individuals that are best adapted to their environment survive and reproduce to a greater extent than other individuals because of heritable variation that the former individuals possess. Natural selection is a mechanism by which populations evolve. Stabilizing selection: This is a type of natural selection in which the intermediate phenotype is the most fit and extreme values of the trait are selected against. Disruptive selection: This is a type of natural selection in which extreme phenotypes (in either direction, which are farthest from the mean) are the most fit and intermediate phenotypes are selected against. Directional selection: This is a type of natural selection in which one extreme phenotype is the fittest and the intermediate phenotypes and the other extreme phenotype are selected against. Assortative mating: This is a non-random mating pattern in which individuals preferentially mate with those who have the same phenotype as them (positive assortative mating) or with those individuals who have a different phenotype from them (negative assortative mating). Honest signal: This is a signal (behaviour or phenotype used actively by an individual to transmit information and influence the behaviour of another individual) that correctly represents the state of the signaller. Acceptance error: This is the misidentification of an undesirable individual as a desirable one. In the case of mate selection, an undesirable individual may be one from another species, but during kin recognition, most individuals of the same species would also be undesirable. Rejection error: This is the misidentification of a desirable individual as an undesirable one. pends on various other cues that the face bears, as well as the state of the individual who is doing the rating [20]. It has also been found that while averaged faces are attractive, the most attractive faces are not average and often have some unusual characteristic about them [21, 22]. These unusual characteristics may serve as signals of quality and there could be directional selection on 317

GENERAL ARTICLE Surprisingly, it has been found that the preference for the average is not limited to faces: people also rated averaged heterospecifics, such as birds or fish, and artifacts, such as watches or cars, as more attractive than their non-average counterparts. them, with the preferred individuals showing trait values away from the population mean. Surprisingly, it has been found that the preference for the average is not limited to faces: people also rated averaged heterospecifics, such as birds or fish, and artifacts, such as watches or cars, as more attractive than their non-average counterparts [23]. This raised the question about whether there was any mate-selection advantage to preferring the average or whether it was simply an outcome of how information is processed in the brain. It has subsequently been shown that familiarity (which could also partly stem from selective manufacturing of items with certain attractive traits) seems to fully account for attractiveness of the average amongst artifacts but only partially influences attractiveness of the average amongst humans and other animals [23]. Therefore, there is probably an evolved preference for the average face. While there is some evidence for other species avoiding unusual or unfamiliar looking mates [24], a preference for the average, as seen in humans, has not been tested. In primates, preference for symmetry has been tested for but not averageness. This is partly constrained by the inability of our perceptual abilities to determine the perception of test animals. It will be interesting to see if other species show koinophilic behaviours using visual and nonvisual sensory modalities. Further research in this direction may reveal much about recognition and mate-selection. Suggested Reading [1] J H Langlois, L A Roggman, R J Casey, J M Ritter, L A Rieser Danner, V Y Jenkins, Infant preferences for attractive faces: rudiments of a stereotype?, Developmental Psychology, Vol.23, No.3, pp.363 369, 1987. [2] D Jones and K Hill, Criteria of facial attractiveness in five populations, Human Nature,Vol.4,No.3,pp.271 296, 1993. [3] D TKenrickandS E Gutierres, Contrasteffects andjudgments of physical attractiveness: when beauty becomes asocialproblem, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.38, No.1, pp.131 140, 1980. [4] F Galton, Composite portraits, Nature, Vol.18, pp.97 100, 1878. [5] J H Langlois and L A Roggman, Attractive faces are only average, Psychological Science, Vol.1,No.2,pp.115 121, 1990. 318 RESONANCE April 2015

[6] J H Koeslag,Koinophilia: Groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour, Journal of Theoretical Biology, Vol.144, No.1, pp.15 35, 1990. [7] K Grammer and R Thornhill, Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: The role of symmetry and averageness, Journal of Comparative Psychology, Vol.108, No.3, pp.233 242, 1994. [8] D I Perrett, D M Burt, I S Penton Voak, K J Lee, D A Rowland and R Edwards, Symmetry and human facial attractiveness, Evolution and Human Behavior, Vol.20, No.5, pp.295 307, 1999. [9] M R Morris and K Casey, Female swordtail fish prefer symmetrical sexual signal, Animal Behaviour, Vol.55, No.1, pp.33 39, 1998. [10] J P Swaddle and I C Cuthill, Female zebra finches prefer males with symmetric chest plumage, Proceedings of the Royal Society, Vol.258, No.1353, pp.267 271, 1994. [11] J T Manning and M A Hartley, Symmetry and ornamentation are correlated in the peacock s train, Animal Behavior, Vol.42, No.6, pp.1020 1021, 1991. [12] J H Langlois, L A Roggman and L Musselman, What is average and not average about attractive faces?, Psychological Science, Vol.5, No.4, pp.214 220, 1994. [13] T I M Valentine, S Darling and M Donnelly,Whyare average faces attractive? The effect of viewand averageness on the attractiveness of female faces, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, Vol.11, No.3, pp.482 487, 2004. [14] J P Swaddle, Fluctuating asymmetry, animal behaviour, and evolution, Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol.32, pp.169 205, 2003. [15] L A Zebrowitz, B White and K Wieneke, Mere exposure and racial prejudice: Exposure to other-race faces increases liking for strangers of that race, Social Cognition, Vol.26, No.3, pp.259 275, 2008. [16] D A Leopold, I V Bondar and M A Giese, Norm-based face encoding by single neurons in the monkey inferotemporal cortex, Nature, Vol.442, No.7102, pp.572 575, 2006. [17] M K Unnikrishnan, Howis the individuality ofa face recognized?, Journal of Theoretical Biology, Vol. 261, No.3, pp.469 474, 2009. [18] J H Koeslag, Ontheengine of speciation, Journal of Theoretical Biology, Vol. 177, No.4, pp.401 409, 1995. [19] J H Koeslag and P D Koeslag, Koinophilia, Journal of Theoretical Biology, Vol.167, No.1, pp.55 65, 1994. [20] A C Little, B C Jones, L M DeBruine, Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, Vol.366, No.1571, pp.1638 1659, 2011. [21] D I Perrett, K A May, S Yoshikawa, Facial shapes and judgements of female attractiveness, Nature, Vol. 368, No.6468, pp.239 242, 1994. [22] T R Alley and M R Cunningham, Averaged faces are attractive, but very attractive faces are not average, Psychological Science, Vol.2, No.2, pp.123 125, 1991. [23] J Halberstadt and G Rhodes, It s not just average faces that are attractive: computer-manipulated averageness makes birds, fish, and automobiles attractive, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, Vol.10, No.1, pp.149 156, 2003. [24] P Bateson, Sexual imprinting and optimal outbreeding, Nature, Vol.273, pp.659 660, 1978. [25] C H Waddington, Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters, Nature, Vol.150, No.3811, pp.563 565, 1942. Address for Correspondence TNCVidya Evolutionary and Organismal Biology Unit Jawaharlal Nehru Centre for Advanced Scientific Research Jakkur, Bengaluru 560 064, India. Email: tncvidya@jncasr.ac.in 319