Influence of lexical markers on the production of contextual factors inducing irony

Similar documents
Artefacts as a Cultural and Collaborative Probe in Interaction Design

Embedding Multilevel Image Encryption in the LAR Codec

A New Analysis of Verbal Irony

On viewing distance and visual quality assessment in the age of Ultra High Definition TV

Learning Geometry and Music through Computer-aided Music Analysis and Composition: A Pedagogical Approach

Workshop on Narrative Empathy - When the first person becomes secondary : empathy and embedded narrative

Reply to Romero and Soria

Laurent Romary. To cite this version: HAL Id: hal

QUEUES IN CINEMAS. Mehri Houda, Djemal Taoufik. Mehri Houda, Djemal Taoufik. QUEUES IN CINEMAS. 47 pages <hal >

Compte-rendu : Patrick Dunleavy, Authoring a PhD. How to Plan, Draft, Write and Finish a Doctoral Thesis or Dissertation, 2007

Sound quality in railstation : users perceptions and predictability

An Impact Analysis of Features in a Classification Approach to Irony Detection in Product Reviews

A Pragmatic Study of the Recognition and Interpretation of Verbal Irony by Malaysian ESL Learners

Ironic Expressions: Echo or Relevant Inappropriateness?

Indexical Concepts and Compositionality

No title. Matthieu Arzel, Fabrice Seguin, Cyril Lahuec, Michel Jezequel. HAL Id: hal

The Roles of Politeness and Humor in the Asymmetry of Affect in Verbal Irony

An overview of Bertram Scharf s research in France on loudness adaptation

Translating Cultural Values through the Aesthetics of the Fashion Film

A new conservation treatment for strengthening and deacidification of paper using polysiloxane networks

Adaptation in Audiovisual Translation

Masking effects in vertical whole body vibrations

La convergence des acteurs de l opposition égyptienne autour des notions de société civile et de démocratie

Interactive Collaborative Books

Releasing Heritage through Documentary: Avatars and Issues of the Intangible Cultural Heritage Concept

On the Citation Advantage of linking to data

PaperTonnetz: Supporting Music Composition with Interactive Paper

Open access publishing and peer reviews : new models

REBUILDING OF AN ORCHESTRA REHEARSAL ROOM: COMPARISON BETWEEN OBJECTIVE AND PERCEPTIVE MEASUREMENTS FOR ROOM ACOUSTIC PREDICTIONS

Natural and warm? A critical perspective on a feminine and ecological aesthetics in architecture

A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF IRONY INTERPRETATION

Regularity and irregularity in wind instruments with toneholes or bells

A PRELIMINARY STUDY ON THE INFLUENCE OF ROOM ACOUSTICS ON PIANO PERFORMANCE

Philosophy of sound, Ch. 1 (English translation)

Implicit Display Theory of Verbal Irony: Towards A Computational Model of Irony

Communication Mechanism of Ironic Discourse

Clues for Detecting Irony in User-Generated Contents: Oh...!! It s so easy ;-)

A Cognitive-Pragmatic Study of Irony Response 3

Irony as Cognitive Deviation

IRONY COMPREHENSION IN THE NONNATIVE LANGUAGE COMES AT A COST

Motion blur estimation on LCDs

From SD to HD television: effects of H.264 distortions versus display size on quality of experience

THE ROLE OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND SITUATIONAL FACTORS IN PERCEPTION OF VERBAL IRONY

A study of the influence of room acoustics on piano performance

Primo. Michael Cotta-Schønberg. To cite this version: HAL Id: hprints

Verbal Ironv and Situational Ironv: Why do people use verbal irony?

Translation as an Art

Interlingual Sarcasm: Prosodic Production of Sarcasm by Dutch Learners of English

Opening Remarks, Workshop on Zhangjiashan Tomb 247

A joint source channel coding strategy for video transmission

Who s afraid of banal nationalism?

Stories Animated: A Framework for Personalized Interactive Narratives using Filtering of Story Characteristics

Improvisation Planning and Jam Session Design using concepts of Sequence Variation and Flow Experience

Creating Memory: Reading a Patching Language

Spectral correlates of carrying power in speech and western lyrical singing according to acoustic and phonetic factors

Visual Annoyance and User Acceptance of LCD Motion-Blur

Is Verbal Irony Special?

Synchronization in Music Group Playing

The Brassiness Potential of Chromatic Instruments

Some problems for Lowe s Four-Category Ontology

Musicians on Jamendo: A New Model for the Music Industry?

PRODUCING IRONY IN ADOLESCENCE: A COMPARISON BETWEEN FACE-TO-FACE AND COMPUTER-MEDIATED COMMUNICATION

A critical pragmatic approach to irony

Irony comprehension: A developmental perspective. Deirdre Wilson. UCL Linguistics and CSMN, Oslo

Review of A. Nagy (2017) *Des pronoms au texte. Etudes de linguistique textuelle*

The multimodal dining experience - A case study of space, sound and locality

Recognizing sarcasm without language

A new HD and UHD video eye tracking dataset

Optimal Innovation and Pleasure

0 Aristotle: dejinition of irony: the rhetorical Jigure which names an object by using its opposite name 0 purpose of irony: criticism or praise 0

A Comparative Study of Variability Impact on Static Flip-Flop Timing Characteristics

Decision Problem of Instrumentation in a Company involved in ISO 50001

Formalizing Irony with Doxastic Logic

Hearing Loss and Sarcasm: The Problem is Conceptual NOT Perceptual

An Analytic Study of Ironic Statements in Ahlam Mistaghanmi s Their Hearts with Us While Their Bombs Launching towards Us

Effects of headphone transfer function scattering on sound perception

Sarcasm and emoticons: Comprehension and emotional impact

A fine-grained analysis of the acoustic cues involved in verbal irony recognition in French

Coming in and coming out underground spaces

Editing for man and machine

Corpus-Based Transcription as an Approach to the Compositional Control of Timbre

Irony and relevance: A reply to Seto, Hamamoto and Yamanashi

The Diverse Environments Multi-channel Acoustic Noise Database (DEMAND): A database of multichannel environmental noise recordings

Academic librarians and searchers: A new collaboration sets the path towards research project success

ARTICLE VERBAL IRONY USE IN FACE-TO-FACE AND COMPUTER-MEDIATED CONVERSATIONS

UNIVERSIDAD DE CHILE FACULTAD DE FILOSOFÍA Y HUMANIDADES DEPARTAMENTO DE LINGÜÍSTICA

Understanding Hyperbole

Pseudo-CR Convolutional FEC for MCVideo

The dynamics of situations

Artifactualization: Introducing a new concept.

Musical instrument identification in continuous recordings

Multisensory approach in architecture education: The basic courses of architecture in Iranian universities

8 The Risks and Rewards of Ironic Communication

OMaxist Dialectics. Benjamin Lévy, Georges Bloch, Gérard Assayag

Spatial empathy and urban experience: a case study in a public space from Rio de Janeiro

Sonic Ambiances Bruitage -Recordings of the Swiss International Radio in the Context of Media Practices and Cultural Heritage

Communicating novel and conventional scientific metaphors: a study of the development of the metaphor of genetic code

Salience in Visual Context: Effects on Appreciation of Advertisements

The Zoummeroff Collection on Criminocorpus

interpreting figurative meaning

Transcription:

Influence of lexical markers on the production of contextual factors inducing irony Elora Rivière, Maud Champagne-Lavau To cite this version: Elora Rivière, Maud Champagne-Lavau. Influence of lexical markers on the production of contextual factors inducing irony. 10th International and Interdisciplinary Conference on Modeling and Using Context (CONTEXT-17), Jun 2017, Paris, France. Springer, Brézillon P., Turner R., Penco C. (eds) Modeling and Using Context. CONTEXT 2017. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 10257. Springer, Cham 2017. <hal-01791892> HAL Id: hal-01791892 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01791892 Submitted on 15 May 2018 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of scientific research documents, whether they are published or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers. L archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Influence of lexical markers on the production of contextual factors inducing irony Elora Rivière & Maud Champagne-Lavau Aix Marseille Univ, CNRS, LPL, Aix-en-Provence, France elora.riviere@lpl-aix.fr; maud.champagne-lavau@univ-amu.fr 1 Introduction Previous research has suggested that contextual factors are required to lead to an ironic interpretation of an utterance. The highlighted contextual factors have been : the allusion to a failed expectancy, which is the fact to allude to a difference between previous expectations and the actual reality (Kumon-Nakamura, Glucksberg, & Brown, 1995; Utsumi, 2000) ; view by others as the presence of an echo, the fact to mention a previous speech or events, thoughts, social norms or shared expectancies (Kreuz & Glucksberg, 1989; Sperber & Wilson, 1986). Another highlighted contextual factor is pragmatic insincerity, which is the violation of one or more felicity conditions in order to call the attention on the failed expectancy and express the speaker attitude towards this failure (Kumon-Nakamura et al., 1995; Utsumi, 2000). The two last factors are the expression of negative attitude, which can be described as the indirect expression of negative feelings about the failure of the expectancy (Utsumi, 2000) and specific to sarcasm, the presence of a victim (Utsumi, 2000). The victim of irony is an identifiable person or group of persons targeted by a negative comment. Campbell & Katz (2012) were the first to experimentally test the necessity of these factors in the sarcastic interpretation of an utterance. They worked with the Constraints Satisfaction approach that allows the study of all these factors at the same time and the interaction between them. Indeed this approach suggests that the non-literal interpretation of an utterance would be precociously accessible only if the context (i.e., contextual factors) promoting this interpretation is substantial enough. The Constraints Satisfaction approach supposes that the several factors (i.e., called constraints in this approach) give a probabilistic base for a competitive interpretation in parallel and throughout the time. Some constraints then may be more likely to play a role early in the understanding process while others would play a later role. Within this framework, Campbell & Katz (2012) reported that pragmatic insincerity is neither necessary nor sufficient, confirming the results of Colston (2000) about the non-necessity of the pragmatic insincerity. They also showed that none of these other contextual factors is necessary but each is sufficient in the comprehension of sarcasm. 1 However Campbell & Katz (2012) did not manipulate features of the target utterances such as ironic markers or type of irony. There is a distinction between factors (i.e., constraints) and markers of irony. Indeed, factors have to be present for an utterance to be qualify as ironic, without their presence an utterance is no longer ironic (Attardo, Eisterhold, Hay, & Poggi, 2003). On the other hand, ironic markers (e.g., lexical markers) are defined as meta-communicative clues, helping but non essential in the comprehension of irony (Attardo, 2000). Burgers, van Mulken, & Schellens (2012) showed that ironic markers such as lexical markers increase irony comprehension and reduce perceived complexity. In addition, the Implicit Display Theory (Utsumi, 2000) suggests that for an utterance to be understood as ironic, this utterance has to be embedded in an ironic environment and it implicitly displays this ironic environment. The lexical markers may contribute to implicitly display the ironic environment. Thus the role of lexical marker in the comprehension of an utterance as ironic seems to be sizeable. The aim of the present study was to assess in French whether the presence of lexical markers influences the production of constraints inducing irony. According to this short literature review, the constraints tested were allusion to a failed expectancy, negative attitude and presence of a victim. 1 Sarcasm is a variable of verbal irony (Colston, 2000). Sarcasm is characterized by the factors characterizing verbal irony and added to them by the presence of a negative comment about an identifiable victim or a group of identifiable victims (Kumon-Nakamura, Glucksberg, & Brown, 1995).

Our hypothesis was that if lexical markers influence the production of the factors, then the production of these factors should be more important when the ironic target utterance includes a lexical marker. 2 Material and Method 2.1 Production task Twenty participants, 11 women and 9 men (mean age = 21.2 years ± 2.3; mean level of education = 15.1 years ± 2.0) were asked to complete 20 minimal contexts to induce an ironic interpretation of a target utterance (e.g., What beautiful weather). Participant can write here You and Clara want to have a picnic. Participant can write here You look by the window and say to Clara : What beautiful weather Fig. 1. Example of minimal context and target utterance as presented to participants Target utterances were manipulated according to the type of verbal irony: with a victim, directed against someone (e.g., You are so fast) versus about a situation (e.g., What beautiful weather) and according to the presence (e.g., What beautiful weather) vs. absence (e.g., It is useful) of a lexical marker leading to the 4 following conditions: irony with a victim and presence of a lexical marker; irony with a victim without the presence of a lexical marker; irony about situation with presence of a lexical marker; and irony about a situation without the presence of a lexical marker. There were five target utterances for each of the four conditions. 2.2 Target utterances validation Before the task of context production, the target utterances were validated by another group of participants containing 22 women and 22 men (mean age = 24.9 ± 2.4; mean level of education = 15.9 ± 1.4). The purpose of this validation, done on 35 utterances, was to select 20 utterances that can be equally understood as sincere and as ironic to ensure that if a target utterance was judged as ironic after the step of context completion, it was due to the constraints added by the participants and not due to the target utterances themselves. To achieve this validation, the participants were asked to evaluate, on a seven points Likert scale, the use frequency of each utterance as sarcastic. The utterances that obtained a middle score between 2.5 and 4.5 were selected. 3 Results The 400 contexts were produced and then judged as ironic or not. In a second step, the 300 stimuli recognized as ironic were analyzed for the presence of each contextual factor (i.e., allusion to a failed expectancy, negative attitude and presence of a victim). Two types of irony (with victim or about a situation) x 2 lexical markers (presence or absence) repeated-measure ANOVAs were performed on the percentage of stimuli containing each pragmatic constraint (presence of a victim, negative tension and allusion to a failed expectancy). The 2x2 ANOVA on the percentage of stimuli containing the factor presence of a victim results showed a main effect of the lexical marker on the percentage of stimuli containing the contextual factor presence of a victim (F(1.18)=12.426 ; p = 0.002) meaning that when the lexical marker was present (M = 41.83%), less stimuli

were produced with the contextual factor presence of a victim than when it was absent (M = 53.03%). It also showed a main effect of the type of irony (F (1.18)=253.349; p<0.0001) meaning that the irony with a victim (M = 85.66%) contains a percentage more important of stimuli including this factor than irony about a situation (M = 9.20%). The interaction type of irony x lexical marker was no significant: F (1.18) = 3.906; p=0.064. A main effect of lexical marker was also shown by the 2x2 ANOVA on the percentage of stimuli containing the factor negative attitude (F (1.18) = 44.032; p<0.0001) meaning that when the lexical marker was present (M = 91.36%) in the target sentence, more stimuli were produced with the contextual factor negative attitude than when it was absent (M = 69.39%) (see Figure 2). This ANOVA also showed a main effect of the irony type F (1.18)= 33.312; p<0.0001. Indeed, the percentage of stimuli including the factor negative attitude is more important when the stimuli belong to the type irony about a situation (M = 91.45%) than when they belong to the type irony with a victim (M = 69.30%). The interaction type of irony x lexical marker was not significant (F (1.18) = 2.602; p>0.05). Percentage of stimuli 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Lexical marker present Lexical marker absent Irony with a victim Irony about a situation Fig. 2. Percentage of stimuli containing the factor negative attitude in function of the presence or absence of the lexical marker in the target utterances and of the type of irony. The 2x2 ANOVA on the percentage of stimuli containing the factor allusion to a failed expectancy showed no significant effect of the lexical marker F (1.18) = 3.588; p=0.074. It did not show any significant effect of the type of irony (F (1.18) = 1.613; p>0.05). The interaction type of irony x lexical marker didn t reach significance: F (1.18) = 1.124; p>0.05. 4 Discussion The results showed that the presence of a lexical marker in the target utterance influences the production of factors presence of a victim and negative attitude but does not influence the production of the factor allusion to a failed expectancy. The fact that lexical markers are often hyperbolic and thus invite to an increase of the negative feeling most likely explains the results obtained for the factor negative attitude. Indeed our results showed that the factor negative attitude was more present when the ironic target utterance includes a lexical marker than when the lexical marker is absent of the target utterance. These results are in agreement with the Implicit Display Theory (Utsumi, 2000). The linked made by Utsumi (2000) between negative attitude and the presence of lexical marker in the ironic target utterance can effectively be confirmed by our results.

By contrast, the pragmatic constraint presence of a victim was more present when the ironic target utterance did not include a lexical marker than when it included a lexical marker. There is no literature about the influence of lexical marker on the presence of a victim but maybe these results could be explained by the Constraints Satisfaction approach. Indeed, this approach considers that no constraint is necessary but that they are sufficient to reach a stable state, in this case the comprehension of the utterance as ironic or as sincere. Due to the influence of the presence of a lexical marker on the production of the factor negative attitude, this factor would be stronger and may play a precocious role. In consequence, the role played by the factor presence of a victim would be undermined in the presence of a lexical marker. More research should be necessary to confirm this hypothesis. No effect of the lexical marker has been found on the factor allusion to a failed expectancy. These results could be explained by the fact that the constraint allusion to a failed expectancy is the more present constraint in the ironic utterances. It could be possible that its role in context to the comprehension of irony is so strong that its production is not influenced by linguistic constraint specific to the ironic target utterance. Allusion to failed expectancy (Kumon-Nakamura et al., 1995; Utsumi, 2000), or view by others as the presence of an echo (Kreuz & Glucksberg, 1989; Sperber & Wilson, 1986), seems to be the constraint whose role, played in the comprehension of an utterance as ironic, makes the most consensus in the literature. The sizeable role of this factor is reflected by the authors agreement and is probably the reason why the presence or absence of a lexical marker did not influence the production of this factor. Our results seemed to show that the different features as factors and ironic markers as lexical markers play intermingled roles in irony processing. As a conclusion, features of the ironic utterances such as the presence of a lexical marker could have an influence on the production of the factors present in the context and whose role is to induce an ironic interpretation of the utterance. Until now, the various features (i.e., factors and ironic markers as lexical markers) playing a role in irony processing have been studied separately but our results highlighted the need to study all types of features at the same time and in interactions. It will be very interesting, using the Satisfaction Constraint approach to evaluate the weight of each factor and to see if some ironic markers as the presence of a lexical marker have an influence on this weight. Moreover, the influence, on the production of factors, of other types of features such as the sociological ones, whose role on irony processing have been shown (Gibbs, 2000; Ivanko, Pexman, & Olineck, 2004) should also be taken into account.

References 1. Attardo, S. (2000). Irony markers and functions: Towards a goal-oriented theory of irony and its processing. Rask, 12, 3 20. 2. Attardo, S., Eisterhold, J., Hay, J., & Poggi, I. (2003). Multimodal markers of irony and sarcasm. Humor - International Journal of Humor Research, 16(2), 243 260. 3. Burgers, C., van Mulken, M., & Schellens, P. J. (2012). Type of evaluation and marking of irony: The role of perceived complexity and comprehension. Journal of Pragmatics, 44(3), 231 242. 4. Campbell, J. D., & Katz, A. N. (2012). Are there necessary conditions for inducing a sense of sarcastic irony? Discourse Processes, 49:6, 459 480. 5. Colston, H. L. (2000). On necessary conditions for verbal irony comprehension. Pragmatics and Cognition, (8), 277 324. 6. Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2000). Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and Symbol, (15), 5 27. 7. Ivanko, S. L., Pexman, P. M., & Olineck, K. M. (2004). How sarcastic are you? Individual Differences and Verbal Irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, (23(3)), 244 271. 8. Kreuz, R. J., & Glucksberg, S. (1989). How to be sarcastic: The echoic reminder theory of verbal irony. Journal of experimental Psychology : General, 118(4), 374 386. 9. Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about another piece of pie : the allusional pretense theory of discourse irony. Journal of experimental Psychology : General, 124(1), 3 21. 10. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. 11. Utsumi, A. (2000). Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment : distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1777 1806.