Reliability of Tinnitus Loudness Matches under Procedural Variation

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Reliability of Tinnitus Loudness Matches under Procedural Variation"

Transcription

1 J Am Acad Audiol 10 : (1999) Reliability of Tinnitus Loudness Matches under Procedural Variation James A. Henry*t Christopher L. Flick* Alison Gilbert* Roger M. Ellingson* Stephen A. Fausti*t Abstract Repeated tinnitus loudness matches (LMs) were obtained to determine response reliability using a computer-automated technique with two procedural variations, fixed or random step sizes, to increase output level during the initial ascending series of tones at each frequency. Twenty subjects with stable, tonal tinnitus were evaluated with both methods during each of two sessions. Response instructions were displayed on a portable computer, and a pen device was used to make response choices that appeared on the touch-sensitive video monitor. For each method, hearing thresholds and LMs were obtained, with 1-dB resolution, at '/s-octave frequencies from 1 to 16 khz. Analyses revealed reliability of LMs to be equivalent between methods. LM data are reported in both db SPL and db SL, with the SPL values providing greater reliability both within and between sessions (all is?.889, p's <_.0001). Key Words : Loudness, measurement, reliability, tinnitus Abbreviations : ANOVA = analysis of variance, LAN = local area network, LM = loudness match he study of tinnitus has become increasingly prevalent over the last few decades, T especially with regard to treatments and mechanisms (Tyler et al, 1992). Significant progress has been made, and there is currently a high level of interest in conducting tinnitus research. These auspicious developments are providing hope and encouragement for patients whose lives are severely impacted by tinnitus (Fortune et al, 1999). For tinnitus research involving humans, as well as for clinical tinnitus management, it is important to have methodology available for reliably quantifying the loudness and other perceptual attributes of tinnitus (Vernon and Fenwick, 1984 ; Coles and Baskill, 1996 ; Goldstein, 1997). Although not yet demonstrated, these measurements should provide agreement with treatment outcome and be diagnostic for select- *National VA RR&D Center for Rehabilitative Auditory Research, Portland VA Medical Center, Portland, Oregon ; tdepartment of Otolaryngology, Oregon Health Sciences University, Portland, Oregon Reprint requests : James A. Henry, RR&D Center for Rehabilitative Auditory Research, Portland VA Medical Center (R&D-16), PO Box 1034, Portland, OR ing treatment strategies (Jastreboff, 1996). Many adaptations of psychoacoustic matching techniques have been described for this purpose, but none have received general acceptance. Tinnitus "quantification" thus relies upon an individual's subjective report to determine if and how the tinnitus changes as the result of some experimental manipulation or treatment. The need exists for a technique to reliably identify acoustic stimuli that simulate an individual's tinnitus perception and that can be used in both research and clinical settings. In the clinic, tinnitus matching measurements are often made in an attempt to define a tone, noise band, or other sound that mimics the acoustic sensation that a patient experiences as tinnitus. This type of technique was first described by Fowler (1943), and many subsequent studies used the same basic technique with variations (Reed, 1960; Graham and Newby, 1962 ; Roeser and Price, 1980 ; Tyler et al, 1992). These and other studies have generally failed to show any relation between psychoacoustic tinnitus measures and subjective improvement resulting from therapy (Hazell et al, 1985 ; Scott et al, 1985 ; Kuk et al, 1989 ; Murai et al, 1992 ; Jastreboff, 1996). 502

2 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al There has been little progress in the area of tinnitus measurement since the mid 1980s, which is somewhat inexplicable and unfortunate since tinnitus research involving humans requires reliable techniques to quantify the disorder and to measure changes in the tinnitus perception over time. A battery of audiologic tests has long been standardized for the evaluation of auditory function ; however, a standardized test protocol does not exist for evaluating tinnitus. This is a detriment to both clinical care and to tinnitus research. It is the overall purpose of a series of ongoing experiments in this laboratory to develop clinical methodology for conducting tinnitus matching that can lead to procedural standardization. The requisite first step in developing a clinical technique for tinnitus matching is to determine that the method obtains reliable responses. We have chosen to use computer automation to conduct such measurements, standardize the protocol, and minimize operator intervention and potential biasing characteristics. A prototype version of the automated system showed that reliable tinnitus loudness matches (LMs) were feasible using this approach (Henry et al, 1996). The automated system has since received major new programming and hardware. The testing algorithms have been refined and programmatic changes have been implemented to improve testing efficiency and to allow all parameters to be easily adjusted. There were two objectives for the present study : (1) to document test-retest reliability of tinnitus LMs at a series of 13 frequencies using the upgraded system and (2) to evaluate if there was any decrease in response reliability when randomization was applied to the start level of loudness matching tones and to the step sizes for increasing the level of the tones. In the testing algorithm for the original computerized system, loudness matching tones were raised above threshold in fixed step sizes until the subject (usually) reported that the tone was louder than the tinnitus. At that point, the tone was decreased in level as part of a bracketing procedure. By presenting the initial series of ascending tones in fixed decibel increments, the patient can, consciously or subconsciously, count the number of steps to arrive at a consistent LM. The question, then, is whether the reliability of LMs is improved through the use of a fixed order of stimulus presentation. If so, the reliability of such a technique is at least partially ascribable to cues provided by the testing protocol. Patients, therefore, may be more reliable with their responses when the step sizes are presented in a series of predictable steps than if randomization is introduced into the protocol. To investigate this premise, tinnitus loudness matching was done using fixed and random methods, and these methods were evaluated independently and differentially for response reliability. Subjects METHOD Twenty subjects participated in this study. They included two females and 18 males with a mean age of 58.0 years and an age range of 24 to 78. These subjects were selected on the basis of having tonal, stable tinnitus in order to minimize any variability in the tinnitus that might confound interpretation of the reliability analyses. Eleven of the subjects were previously patients, with the primary complaint of tinnitus, at the Tinnitus Clinic, Oregon Health Sciences University. They were thus experienced in tinnitus evaluation using a clinical matching technique. The remaining nine subjects did not have previous experience and consisted of four subjects from an earlier hearing aid study, three veterans referred from the Audiology Clinic at the Portland VA Medical Center, and two individuals from the local community. Hardware The three major hardware components of the tinnitus testing system included (1) the main computer (Dell Dimension 166 MHz Pentium CPU) and signal generator ; (2) a custombuilt signal conditioning module for signal mixing, attenuation, and headphone buffering (Oregon Hearing Research Center, Portland, OR) ; and (3) the subject computer (Compaq Concerto 4/25). A block diagram of the system is shown in Figure 1. Acoustic Subsystem Instrumentation for signal generation, conditioning, and transduction was configured in the signal path depicted in Figure l. Pure-tone signal generation was accomplished using a precision signal generator (National Instruments, AT-DSP k) housed in the main computer. The pure-tone signal was routed to the signal conditioning module, which performed pulse shaping, attenuation, and headphone buffering 503

3 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 Main Computer Acoustic Signal Path LAN Serial Interface Subject Computer Sound Booth Signal Generator Signal Conditioning Module ER-4 Insert Phones Figure 1 Block diagram of hardware used for computer-automated tinnitus evaluation. LAN = local area network. functions. Insert earphones (Etymotic Corp. ER- 4) were used for transducers. The signal generator was a 16-bit, highperformance, high-accuracy audio bandwidth digital to analog I/O board, which was used to generate stimulus waveforms directly from computer-generated digital data. The analog signals generated by the board were lowdistortion, low-noise, and frequency-flat (http :// The signal generator board had both analog and digital anti-imaging filters to remove unwanted glitches and highfrequency signals. These filters limited the bandwidth of the analog output signal to half the digital-analog conversion rate of 44,100 Hz. A serial port from the main computer was used to control the signal conditioning module. This module provided programmable functions that included input mixing and gain, pulse windowing and muting, and overall attenuation. The attenuation range was 0.0 to db with 0.1-dB resolution. The Etymotic ER-4 insert earphones frequency response characteristics met the needs of this study : >_ 100 db SPL output from 1 to 16 khz and <_ 3 percent harmonic distortion. Subject Instruction & Response Subsystem The main computer and the subject computer were connected via a local area network (LAN) interface using standard networking protocols for two-way communication (see Fig. 1). The Subject Instruction & Response subsystem was used to display prompts, messages, and instructions on the subject computer and to relay subject test responses back to the main computer. The subject computer was an IBM PC compatible, Microsoft Windows for Pen enabled, slate-type, notebook computer located inside the testing booth. The subject used a penpointing device to indicate responses by pen "touching" the appropriate buttons. The subject computer was battery powered with a solid-state display to minimize potentially adverse acoustic and electromagnetic interference inside the testing booth. The computer was upgraded to 20 Mb of memory, allowing the operating system's virtual memory feature to be disabled, which eliminated sound interference from hard drive disk use while managing system memory. System Calibration The acoustic subsystem, which consisted of the signal generation board, the signal conditioning module, and insert earphones, was calibrated at the beginning of each test day. The same digital stimulus waveform generation software that was used during testing was used when calibrating the acoustic subsystem. A custom automated calibration application was developed to provide a multifrequency, multilevel calibration set. A computer-controlled calibration routine was developed with serial interface control of a Bruel & Kjaer 2231 sound level meter and Type 1625 octave filter set (B&K; http :// The ER-4 insert earphones were coupled to the sound level meter using a B&K Type 4157 ear simulator. The calibration values were stored in a database and later accessed while testing to provide attenuation settings for calibrated sound pressure stimulus levels. Software Both the main and subject computers used the Microsoft Windows 95 operating system. All software developed for this study was designed to be Microsoft Windows 95 compatible. Microsoft 504

4 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al Windows OLE remote automation technology was used to provide the virtual connection between the main computer and the subject computer at the application level for passing control, instruction, and response messages. Software was developed to simultaneously control (1) temporal parameters of pure-tone stimuli ; (2) stimulus frequency and attenuation settings ; (3) display of subject instructions and response touch-buttons ; (4) recording of subject responses for program control ; (5) logging of subject responses into data files ; and (6) main computer screen display for monitoring test status, progress, and results. The main computer software program consisted of dialog forms for examiner entry of subject information, test session information, parameters for the tinnitus-testing control loop, and visual displays for monitoring testing status, progress, and results. During testing, the control loop established frequency and attenuation settings, formulated and routed instruction messages to the subject computer, acquired the subject response, adjusted the stimulus waveform frequency and levels according to the response, and logged results. The examiner had the capability of overriding subject responses, pausing, or stopping testing during the test session. Overriding subject responses was not necessary for any of the subjects reported in this study. The only times testing was interrupted was when the subject touched the "Help" button, which was visible at all times on the subject computer display. This paused the test and allowed the examiner to answer the subject's question(s). Testing would then resume at the point of interruption. Database-Driven Architecture The main computer control program was designed from the ground up to be entirely driven by a Microsoft Access desktop database. Information stored in database tables controlled the main program operation including parameters for testing protocols, timing intervals, frequency sets, level-based step sizes, randomization ranges, numbers of steps, numbers of averages, instrumentation used, etc. The testing system was therefore easily configurable by nonprogrammers for modification of experimental protocols. Detailed testing data were logged directly into tables during program operation. Data logged included stimulus presentation history, responses, and intermediate and final averaged results. Results and associated testing parameters were easily queried out following testing for multiway variable analysis. Procedures All procedures were conducted over two testing sessions, separated by 1 to 14 days (one subject completed both sessions in 1 day, separated by 2 hours). Session 1 required 11/z to 2 hours of a subject's time, and Session 2 required about 11/a hours. During the first session, an initial evaluation was done to obtain a case history, gather noise exposure and tinnitus information, determine middle-ear function and hearing sensitivity, and identify the "tinnitus ear." Two experimental LM protocols, fixed and random, were completed during each session. Between protocols, the earphones were removed and the subject took an approximately 10-minute break. Testing order for the two protocols was counterbalanced by alternating the order for each successive subject. In that way, the fixed protocol was tested first for 10 of the subjects and the random first for the other 10. For each subject's second visit, the order of testing was reversed. During each session, otoscopy was done prior to placing earphones for audiometric testing to confirm unoccluded ear canals and the normal appearance of tympanic membranes. Initial Evaluation (First Session Only) At the start of the first session, a short case history was obtained to provide information regarding demographics, auditory and vestibular disorders, and family history of hearing loss. Subjects were also asked if they had been exposed to significant noise and, if so, they completed a noise exposure questionnaire. All subjects also completed a tinnitus questionnaire. For each questionnaire, the attending audiologist asked the questions and subjects responded verbally. Tympanometric screening was performed with a Grason-Stadler GSI-37 Auto Tymp to rule out active middle-ear pathology. Prior to testing with the automated technique, hearing thresholds were obtained manually with a clinical, high-frequency-capable audiometer (Virtual Corp., Model 320) at octave frequencies from 0.25 to 8 khz, and at 1.5, 3, 6, 9, 10, 11.2, and 12.5 khz. Instrumentation and procedures for manual threshold evaluation were as described in Fausti et al (1990). 505

5 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/ Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 Selection of Tinnitus Ear. One ear was chosen as the "tinnitus ear" for each subject. If one ear had more predominant tinnitus, that ear was chosen as the tinnitus ear. If the subject had symmetric tinnitus, the tinnitus ear was chosen randomly. a I 1. You will be listening for soft pulsing tones. 2. When you hear a tone, touch the 'I Hear It' box. 3. To start the test, touch'go'. Selection of Test Ear. Matching tones can be presented ipsilaterally or contralaterally to an ear with tinnitus. Both methods seem to have been used with about equal frequency, and we know of only one report comparing the two methods within patients (Ishikawa et al, 1991). In that study, it was seen that measurements were essentially equivalent when hearing was symmetric, although with asymmetric hearing sensitivity, LMs tended to be larger on the side with better hearing. For the present study, the ear contralateral to the tinnitus ear was chosen as the test ear. Contralateral presentation of pure tones was chosen partly to minimize the potential of producing residual inhibition, which is the common phenomenon of temporary tinnitus suppression following presentation of a suprathreshold stimulus (Vernon, 1987 ; Mitchell et al, 1993). This was a particular concern due to the large number of stimulus presentations each subject received. Furthermore, matching to contralateral tones has been reported to be a less difficult task than ipsilateral matching (Goodwin and Johnson, 1980). b Help Figure 2 Screen displays on subject's notebook computer for hearing thresholds : (a) instructions, (b) response screen. Experimental Protocols (Both Sessions) Instructions to Subjects. Because of the alternating nature of the response tasks, instructions for responding were required each time the task changed. This was accomplished for the automated procedure by displaying instruction screens prior to each task, as shown in Figures 2a and 3a. Test Frequencies. Test frequencies for hearing thresholds and tinnitus LMs (using the automated technique) were in the range of 1 to 16 khz and separated by 1/s octaves (rounded to accommodate the 20-Hz resolution of the test equipment). Frequencies included 1, 1.26, 1.6, 2, 2.52, 3.18, 4, 5.04, 6.36, 8, 10.08, 12.7, and 16 khz, and testing proceeded in a stepwise fashion in this frequency order. LMs were not obtained below 1 khz primarily because, for the majority of patients, tinnitus pitch matches are above 3 to 4 khz, with very few below 1 khz (Meikle and Walsh, 1984 ; Meikle et al, 1995). Thus, the frequency range of 1 to 16 khz would be most pertinent for clinical tinnitus matching, and additional frequencies would have increased subject fatigue in this study. Automated Hearing Thresholds. As part of both the fixed and random protocols, automated hearing thresholds were obtained, followed by loudness matching, at each frequency. The goal for obtaining hearing thresholds with the automated system was not to obtain hearing thresholds as normally defined (i.e., 50% response level). Rather, "threshold" was operationally defined as the average of two minimum response levels determined using an adaptation of the modified Hughson-Westlake audiometric test technique (Carhart and Jerger, 1959). The two responses were obtained during presentation of tones in ascending 1-dB increments. The threshold at each frequency served as the level from which to begin tinnitus loudness matching at that frequency, and also as the point from which to calculate LM sensation levels. 506

6 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al a b Tm,ftm Loud.... f arch!. t. You will hear a single tone lasting several seconds. 2. Decide how the loudness of the tone compares to the loudness of your tinnitus. 3. Touch the box which indicates your response choice. Tone On How should the tone's loudness be changed to make it match the loudness of your tinnitus? Go Tinnhus Loudnessld..h Much Louder Louder Equal Softer Much Softer Help Repeat Help Figure 3 Screen displays on subject's notebook computer for tinnitus loudness matching: (a) instructions, (b) response screen. At each new test frequency, subjects were first shown instructions for threshold testing. When they had read and understood the instructions, they touched the "Go" button on the screen with the pen device, the threshold testing screen appeared (see Fig. 2b), and testing proceeded. At each test frequency, the initial presentation level was fixed at 60 db SPL. Tone presentation levels were then raised or lowered, dependent upon whether the subject responded to the stimulus during a specific time window. Pulsed pure tones of 400-msec duration and rise-fall times of 25 to 45 msec were presented at a 50 percent duty cycle. Each series of pulsed tones was presented in a 2.4-sec window. Intervals between tone series were randomized from between 1 and 4 sec following a response and fixed at 1 sec following a no response. The algorithm for determining an automated hearing threshold consisted of a series of three bracketing procedures, each providing progressively smaller step sizes to finally result in threshold responses with 1-dB resolution (threshold-seeking algorithm summarized in Table 1). The initial bracketing series (Series 1) used step increments of up 10 db, down 20 db to quickly bracket the threshold level to within 10 db. To minimize the possibility of presenting tones at uncomfortably loud levels, the ascending step size during Series 1 (and Series 2) was limited to 3 db at levels? 90 db SPL. Subsequent bracketing series used algorithms of up 5 db, down 10 db (Series 2) and up 1 db, and down 2 db (Series 3). Each series was conducted until a minimum response level (lowest level where a response was obtained) was determined. One response was required for Series 1, and two responses were required for each of Series 2 and 3 (see Table 1). When two responses were required during a series, the responses were averaged to obtain the minimum response level for that series. Following the response for Series 1, the initial presentation level for Series 2 equaled the minimum response level determined from Series 1 minus 10 db (see Table 1). For Series 3, the initial level was the minimum response level from Series 2 minus 2 db. Only the Series 3 minimum response level was used for subsequent loudness matching and for data analysis. Design Considerations for Automated Tin. nitus Loudness Matching. There is currently no standard procedure for matching the loudness of tinnitus using pure tones at a series of frequencies. A well-defined clinical protocol was described by Vernon and Meikle (1981, 1988), who recommended first presenting the loudness matching tone at threshold, then increasing the Table 1 Level Step-Size Parameters for Pure Tones during Automated Threshold Testing Brac ket Serie s Start Level Ascending Step Size (db) Descending Step Size (db ) Repeated? 1 60 db SPL +10 (+3 if level? 90 db SPL) -20 No 2 Mean of previous series Result minus 10 db +5 (+3 if level? 90 db SPL) -10 Yes 3 Mean of previous series Result minus 2 db 1-2 Yes 507

7 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 tone in 1-dB increments until the patient reports a LM. Such a gradual increase was intended to reduce the possibility of producing residual inhibition (tinnitus suppression due to presentation of suprathreshold stimuli). However, for frequencies <_ 2 khz, LMs have been shown to be typically obtained at approximately 20 db SL and at about 10 db SL above 2 khz (Henry and Meikle, 1999). Raising the level in 1-dB steps might therefore require an average of 10 to 20 tone presentations (and subject responses directing the computer to increase the tone's loudness) to reach a LM level at each frequency. Because the present efforts were directed toward clinical expediency, it was necessary to design algorithms to obtain LMs rapidly while maintaining response reliability and minimizing residual inhibition and loudness discomfort. General Procedures for Loudness Matching. The computer algorithm selected was an interleaved design in which the subject was guided through threshold and subsequent LM testing at each frequency. Starting at the lowest frequency (1 khz), a threshold was obtained, as described above, followed by the LM procedure. When a LM was obtained at 1 khz, the computer stepped to 1.26 khz and obtained a threshold and subsequent LM. This interleaved protocol continued in ascending frequency order until thresholds and LMs had been obtained at all 13 test frequencies, or to a frequency where the subject's pure-tone threshold exceeded the maximum output of 100 db SPL. When the computer changed tasks from acquiring a threshold to acquiring a LM at the same frequency, subjects were first shown instructions for the loudness matching task (see Fig. 3a). When they understood the instructions, they touched the "Go" button on the screen and the testing/response screen for loudness matching appeared (see Fig. 3b). Tones for loudness matching were presented continuously for 4 seconds, and the subject made a decision regarding how to adjust the loudness of the tone to match the tinnitus loudness. More specifically, during the first 1 second of the tone presentation, only the "Tone On" message appeared on the screen (see Fig. 3b). After 1 second, the response choices appeared and were visible beyond termination of the tone or until the subject selected a response. Thus, a response could be made after the first 1 second of the tone presentation, and if the response occurred while the tone was on, the tone was immediately terminated. Response choices for loudness matching included "Much Louder," "Louder," "Equal," "Softer," and "Much Softer" (see Fig. 3b). Following the response choice, the computer adjusted the stimulus presentation level for the subsequent matching tone. Generally, a "Much Louder" or "Much Softer" response doubled the change in output level relative to a "Louder" or "Softer" response. At any point during LM testing, an "Equal" response choice directed the computer to record the previous tone's level as the LM for the active test frequency. Both the fixed and random protocols used a progression of three bracketing series to gradually reduce the step sizes of tones presented for loudness matching. The initial series was designed to quickly determine the general loudness of the subject's tinnitus, with two subsequent bracketing series to define the LM with increasing precision. Unlike threshold testing, LM test series were not repeated; the subject could determine a final LM following any tone presentation during any of the three series by selecting "Equal." This would immediately terminate the bracket series under test and direct the computer to move to the next frequency. The fixed and random procedures differed only in the first bracketing series-specifically, in how the initial presentation level was determined and in the step size adjustments following "Louder" and "Much Louder" responses. Following is a description of the fixed protocol and the variations that defined the random protocol. Computer Algorithm for "Fixed" Loudness Matching. For Series 1, the initial presentation level was 10 db SL (i.e., 10 db above the threshold determined immediately prior to the loudness matching task) (Table 2). However, the initial level was reduced to 3 db SL if the threshold was between 70 and 89 db SPL and further reduced to 1 db SL if the threshold was between 90 and 99 db SPL. If the threshold was 100 db SPL (the maximum output level), the initial LM tone was presented at 100 db SPL and could not be presented at a higher level. If the subject selected "Louder" or "Much Louder" as a response choice when 100 db SPL was presented for loudness matching, the computer logged the LM for that frequency as ">100 db SPL." Table 2 shows the changes in presentation level provided by the computer based upon the various response choices for changing the level. For Series 1, response choices of "Softer" and "Much Softer" resulted in reductions in output 508

8 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/ fenry et al Table 2 Presentation Levels of Pure Tones for Automated Tinnitus Loudness Matching Presentation Level Adjustment (db) Based Upon Response Choice Bracket Series Presentation Level Range (db SPL) Start Level (db SL) ouder Much Louder Softer Much Softer * N/A N/A * N/A N/A N/A * -2 N/A *The maximum output was 100 db SPL. If the threshold was 100 db SPL, the start level for loudness matching the subject was 100 db SPL indicated that a tone. If should be made louder than 100 db SPL for a LM, the computer logged the frequency LM for the active as ">100 db test SPL." level of-4 and-8 db, respectively, regardless of the previous presentation level. For increases in level, however, the step sizes were reduced if the previous presentation level was between 70 and 89 db SPL and further reduced when the previous level was between 90 and 99 db SPL. Thus, when the previous presentation level was within 69 db SPL, a choice of "Louder" resulted in an increase of 5 db, whereas a choice of "Much Louder" resulted in an increase of 10 db. If the previous presentation level was between 70 and 89 db SPL, increases in level were reduced to +3 db for a "Louder" response and to +5 db for a "Much Louder" response. Level increases were further reduced to +1 db and +2 db for "Louder" and "Much Louder" responses, respectively, when the previous presentation level was between 90 and 99 db SPL. Progression of the test (ascending or descending intensity of consecutive tone presentations) was determined based on the subject's response choices. A subject's LM was bracketed using the concept of response reversal. A reversal was determined each time the subject changed the direction of their responses on consecutive tone presentations (e.g., going from a "Louder" response to a "Softer" response). A response reversal defined either an upper or lower limit (depending on the direction of the reversal) for an expected range of where the final LM level may be found. By decreasing the ascending and descending step sizes following reversals, it was possible to narrow the intensity range to determine a final LM to within 1 db. This narrowing is indicated by the three bracket series shown in Table 2. Each successive series provided smaller step sizes to enable progressively finer control for precise loudness matching. The main computer program shifted from one series to the next following a reversal from a response choice directing the computer to present a softer tone to a louder tone. The loudness matching continued in Series 3 until the subject chose "Equal" to indicate a LM with the tinnitus. Computer Algorithm for "Random" Loudness Matching. The random loudness matching procedure was identical to the Fixed procedure described above except for the presentation of tones during Series 1, as shown in Table 3. With the random protocol, the level of the first tone presentation for loudness matching ("Start Level") was selected randomly by the computer between 1 and 10 db SL when the threshold was between 0 and 69 db SPL. If the threshold was between 70 and 89 db SPL, the randomized range was reduced to between 1 and 3 db SL. If the threshold was 90 db SPL or greater, the start level was 1 db SL, as for the fixed protocol. With the random protocol (Series 1 only), a selection of "Louder" or "Much Louder," following the start level presentation, resulted in randomized selection of increases in output level for loudness matching. If the previous presentation level was <_ 69 db SPL, a "Louder" selection resulted in an increase in level that was randomized between 1 and 5 db, and "Much Louder" resulted in a level within a randomized increase between 6 and 10 db (Table 3). These ranges 509

9 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 Presentation Level Range (db SPL) Table 3 Comparison of Series 1 Level Step Sizes for Pure Tones : Fixed versus Random Loudness Matching Protocols Fixed Start Lev el (db SL) Random Presentation Level Adjustment (db) Based Upon Response Choice Louder Response Fixed Random Much Louder Response Fixed Random * *See note for Table 2. were reduced when the previous presentation level was between 70 and 89 db SPL: a "Louder" response resulted in an increase randomized between 1 and 3 db, and a "Much Louder" response resulted in an increase randomized between 4 and 6 db. When the previous presentation level was between 90 and 99 db SPL, changes in levels were the same for both fixed and random protocols. Data Analysis Loudness Matches: db SPL as db SL In the literature, it is equivocal whether to express tinnitus LMs in db SPL or db SL. LMs in db SPL reflect the absolute sound pressure level of a tone that is perceived as having the same loudness as the tinnitus. LM measures in db SL are the difference between the db SPL LM and the hearing threshold at the same frequency. LMs expressed in db SL thus introduce a second source of within-subject variability. To allow a comparison of results between the two metrics, the data analyses are shown for both db SPL and db SL. Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance Data analyses focused on evaluating test-retest reliability of LMs obtained with both fixed and random protocols. Repeated measures of LMs were obtained within (between protocols) and between sessions. For each subject at each frequency, there were therefore four measurements for LMs (fixed and random during each of two sessions). Repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to evaluate how consistently subjects as a group responded from measure to measure, both within and between test sessions. ANOVAs were therefore used to compare the four across-subjects mean LMs at each of the 13 test frequencies. LM Differences Descriptive statistics were also used for LMs to provide another perspective of the test-retest reliability of these responses. Differences were calculated between measures obtained during the two sessions. Means of these differences were determined across subjects and were reported as actual differences (to determine if there were trends toward higher or lower responses during the second session) and absolute values of differences (to determine the absolute magnitude of the differences between sessions). Pearson Product-Moment Correlations To evaluate within-subject reliability of responses, Pearson product-moment correlations were calculated for hearing thresholds and for LMs. RESULTS Repeated Hearing Thresholds Figure 4 shows the mean thresholds, obtained using the automated technique, for the group of 20 subjects. There are four means at each frequency, reflecting the two thresholds (for fixed and random methods) obtained during each of the two sessions. Across-Subjects Means of LMs Across-subjects means of the LMs were calculated, as shown in Tables 4 (db SPL) and 5 (db SL) and Figures 5 (db SPL) and 6 (db SL). 510

10 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al ~ 70 a N m v 80 0 L 50 m 40 ~ _ - r r n e ~n ~ m -,d Frequency, Hz Frequency, Hz Figure 4 Mean hearing thresholds (as operationally defined for automated testing) for all subjects, test ear only (N = 20). S = Session. Figure 5 Mean loudness matches expressed in db SPL. S = Session. Repeated-measures ANOVAs were done for the four means at each frequency, and the p values are shown in the last columns of Tables 4 and 5. Because of the large number of ANOVAs that were run, the alpha level was reduced to.01. The only significant difference was at 2 khz (p =.0068) for the data expressed in db SPL. Means of Actual Differences in LMs: Within Subjects and Between Sessions The above analysis shows that the group of subjects as a whole provided reliable LMs between methods and between sessions. However, it did not address the within-subject reliability of responses. It was therefore determined how the two LM techniques compared for withinsubject, between-session reliability. For each subject, the differences in corresponding LMs were calculated. That is, for each method (fixed and random) and each test frequency, the Session 2 LM was subtracted from the Session 1 LM. The across-subject means of those differences are shown in Tables 6 and 7 and Figures 7 and 8. These data reflect the actual differences between each pair of LMs, thus indicating the directionality of the responses between Sessions 1 and 2. It is evident that there was no systematic trend in the directionality of responses between sessions (i.e., responses did not tend to get larger or smaller between sessions for the group of subjects). Thus, the variability associated with the LM responses was random in direction. Means of Absolute Values of Differences in LMs: Within Subjects and Between Sessions To determine the magnitude of the withinsubject differences in responses between sessions, the absolute values of the LMs were Table 4 Means of Loudness Matches Expressed in db SPL and p Values for Repeated-Measures ANOVAs at Each Test Frequency Session 1 Session 2 Frequency (Hz) N Fixed Random Fixed Random p ,080 15* , , *N = 14 for Session

11 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/ Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 Table 5 Means of Loudness Matches Expressed in db SL and p Values for Repeated-Measures ANOVAs at Each Test Frequency Session 1 Session 2 Frequency (Hz) N Fixed Random Fixed Random p ,080 15" , , `N = 14 for Session 2. determined for each difference. The means of the absolute values of the Session 1 minus Session 2 differences are shown in Tables 8 and 9 and Figures 9 and 10. These data reveal that there were no systematic trends in the magnitude of the LM differences between Session 1 and Session 2 for either the fixed or random methods. There was further no systematic variation in the magnitude of differences between the two methods. Means of Differences in LMs : Within Subjects and Within Sessions Since there were essentially no significant differences between means at each test frequency, the LM data obtained for the fixed and random methods could be considered as demonstrating that the group of patients provided reliable within-session LMs. The data were therefore evaluated further to determine within-subject, within-session reliability of responses. For each subject at each frequency, the LM from the random method was subtracted from the LM from the fixed method. This was done for each session, and the means of these differences were comparable to the data shown above for evaluating within-subject, between-session reliability (tables and figures are not shown to conserve space). Pearson Product-Moment Correlations : Within- and Between-Session Reliability For the final evaluation of LM reliability, Pearson is were calculated for all combinations of repeated LMs, both within and between sessions (Tables 10 and 11). As a standard of ref- Table 6 Means of Loudness Match Differences across Subjects, Session 1 Minus Session 2, Expressed in db SPL Frequency (Hz) Fixed Random Frequency, Hz Figure 6 Mean loudness matches expressed in db SL. S = Session , , ,

12 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al Table 7 Means of Loudness Match Differences across Subjects, Session 1 Minus Session 2, Expressed in db SL Frequency (Hz) Fixed Random , , erence, Pearson is were also calculated for the hearing thresholds obtained in each condition. Pearson is for all thresholds and LMs in db SPL were significant at p <_.0001 (indicating that the measures varied together systematically). For LMs in db SL, all Pearson is were significant at p <.01 except for those indicated in Tables 10 and 11 by the asterisks. DISCUSSION s treatment protocols for tinnitus are A becoming increasingly effective, the number of clinics offering treatment is correspondingly increasing. As part of the initial tinnitus evaluation, it is important to quantify the tinnitus characteristics that a patient experiences as an acoustic sensation. However, a clinical method with documented reliability of responses has not emerged for this purpose. A series of ongoing projects, conducted in this laboratory, Figure 8 Means of individual differences in loudness matches (db SL), Session 1 minus Session 2. is designed to systematically develop a clinical technique for matching an individual's tinnitus to acoustic stimuli. A prototype was used to demonstrate the feasibility of the automated technique for tinnitus matching (Henry et al, 1996). For the present study, hardware was upgraded and computer programming rewritten to improve features, enable user control, and add flexibility to testing protocols. Despite these differences, for both studies, testing was repeated over two sessions and LMs were obtained at 12 of the same frequencies. Data analyses showed that LM response reliability was comparable between studies. It is important to minimize redundancies in the testing protocol that might enable subject responses to be cued by the redundant features, Table 8 Means of Loudness Match Differences (Absolute Values) across Subjects, Session 1 Minus Session 2, Expressed in db SPL* Frequency (Hz) Fixed Random p Frequency, Hz Figure 7 Means of individual differences in loudness matches (db SPL), Session 1 minus Session , , , *p values are for t-tests between fixed and random means at each test frequency. 513

13 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 Table 9 Means of Loudness Match Differences (Absolute Values) across Subjects, Session 1 Minus Session 2, Expressed in db SL* Frequency (Hz) Fixed Random P , , , *p values are for t-tests between fixed and random means at each test frequency. which can lead to spurious responses. This is a common concern for all audiologic testing, and sophisticated examiners will appropriately incorporate randomization into their personal testing style. For threshold testing with the automated system, intertone intervals were randomized to reduce the possibility of subjects responding to fixed temporal patterns. During loudness matching, however, it was possible that the fixed level changes relative to threshold could lead subjects to respond on the basis of counting the number of tone presentations. A malingering patient could use such a strategy to provide spuriously consistent responses. It was therefore necessary to evaluate whether patients could respond with equal reliability when outputlevel step sizes were randomized. Frequency, Hz Figure 10 Means of absolute values of individual differences in loudness matches (db SL), Session 1 minus Session 2. Two versions, fixed and random, of a computer-automated protocol for conducting tinnitus loudness matching were evaluated. All data analyses were directed toward determining response reliability of the LMs. Results showed that the fixed and random methods both provided reliable responses and that reliability was comparable between methods. Thus, either method could be used with equal confidence for tinnitus loudness matching. A further implication of these results is that they support a method for loudness matching that has been used in a tinnitus clinic for many years (Vernon, 1982 ; Vernon and Fenwick, 1984 ; Vernon and Meikle, 1981, 1988). The tinnitus loudness matching literature reveals that there is no standard procedure for obtaining LMs, and further that there is controversy regarding the need for conducting such tests. In the following discussion, some relevant points regarding these issues are discussed. "Paradoxically Small" Tinnitus LMs Frequency, Hz Figure 9 Means of absolute values of individual differences in loudness matches (db SPL), Session 1 minus Session 2. Early studies revealed a seemingly paradoxical relationship between patients' subjectively reported tinnitus loudness and the LMs that were typically less than 10 db SL at the tinnitus frequency (Fowler, 1943 ; Reed, 1960 ; Graham and Newby, 1962). This was first noted by Fowler (1943), who concluded that tinnitus was a faint signal that should not be perceived as loud and therefore should not cause such distress. Numerous investigators suggested that these inordinately low-level LMs might be explained by the phenomenon of loudness recruitment (Vernon, 1976 ; Goodwin and Johnson, 1980 ; Tyler and Conrad-Armes, 1983 ; Meikle et al, 514

14 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al Table 10 Pearson is for Within-Session Thresholds and Loudness Matches Session 1 Session 2 Frequency (Hz) Thresholds LMs db SPL LMs db SL Thresholds LMs db SPL LMs db SL , * , * 16, * *p > 01. All p's <_.0001 except as indicated. 1984). This led to studies attempting to define more appropriate ways to measure tinnitus loudness. One way was to obtain LMs at a frequency(s) where hearing sensitivity was normal or near-normal, resulting in an increased dynamic range and larger LM relative to threshold (Goodwin and Johnson, 1980 ; Penner, 1984 ; ftisey et al, 1989 ; Tyler et al, 1992). Other studies considered the premise that decibels are not a measure of loudness and used different approaches to transform tinnitus LMs into units of loudness (Hinchcliffe and Chambers, 1983 ; Tyler and Conrad-Armes, 1983 ; Hallam et al, 1985 ; Matsuhira et al, 1992). Penner (1986) pointed out the equivocal results of these efforts and tested the idea independently of any mathematical description of the loudness function. She concluded that the size of the LM bore a simple relationship with the slope of the loudness function at any test frequency. Henry (1996) used direct loudness-growth measurements in patients with tinnitus in an attempt to correct the measurements, which was hypothesized to increase the correlation between the LMs and subjective tinnitus loudness ratings. Even after the corrections, the correlation remained small. The discrepancy between LMs and subjective judgments of tinnitus loudness remains unresolved, and further work is needed to elucidate this relationship. Table ll Pearson is for Between-Session Thresholds and Loudness Matches Fixed Method Random Method Frequency (Hz) Thresholds LMs db SPL LMs db SL Thresholds LMs db SPL LMs db SL , , * * * *p > 01. All p's <_.0001 except as indicated. 515

15 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 Implications for Assessment of Tinnitus Malingering In the present study, obtaining LMs at the series of test frequencies effectively created LM functions (curves). Meikle et al (1996) reviewed data from 121 patients (from four separate studies) and reported that consistent LM curves emerged despite different measurement techniques. Thus, when LMs were plotted as db SL against frequency, as also shown in the present study (see Fig. 6), there was a majority of monotonically decreasing functions ranging from large negative slopes to slopes near zero. An additional group of patients had LM curves that could not be fit with a linear regression function. These cumulative data showed that LM curves can be used to separate the clinical population into several major categories according to the shapes of their LM curves. Such categories may have some yet undiscovered relationship to tinnitus etiology. These curves may have further value in that their distinctiveness for each patient may serve as a tinnitus "fingerprint," which could be useful in assessing the validity of compensation claims. The use of randomized step increases during loudness matching is particularly important in the design of a "tinnitus malingering" test that requires the patient to provide reliable responses. Such a test has not yet been formalized, but for medicolegal purposes, repeated LMs have been the basis for documenting the presence of a plaintiff's tinnitus (Vernon, 1996). The test is described as "five or six" repeated LMs at the tone that is best matched to the tinnitus pitch. The tone is presented in 1-dB ascending increments, and if the results of all repeated tests agree to within 2 db of each other, that is considered evidence for the presence of tinnitus. (These tests are not repeated consecutively but rather interspersed among other tinnitus and audiometric tests over a period of about 1 hour.) Jacobson and Henderson (1999) recently conducted a study evaluating the ability of listeners without tinnitus to imagine a constant tonal tinnitus and attempt to match the tinnitus loudness with the loudness of tones. These subjects were seen to provide LMs that were at a higher level than actual tinnitus patients, but within- and between-session reliability of the LMs was not significantly different. These results led the authors to conclude that validating a patient's tinnitus may depend more upon the overall LM levels than upon the reliability of the LMs. For the tinnitus malingerer, it would be expected to present a greater challenge to provide reliable LMs at multiple frequencies versus at a single. frequency. The preliminary results of Jacobson and Henderson (1999), however, suggest that loudness matching may be repeatable even without tinnitus. If such an ability exists for the average person, it might be surmised that normal listeners can produce equal loudness contours (Robinson and Dadson, 1956) without the use of a reference tone. There is some evidence for this in the saucer-shaped audiogram that some authors have suggested is typically seen with the patient who provides false or exaggerated hearing thresholds (Doerfler, 1951 ; Carhart, 1958 ; Goetzinger and Proud, 1958). Such an audiogram appears similar to an equal loudness contour, which would suggest that patients are able to use some kind of internal reference to produce responses at equal loudness levels across frequencies (Martin, 1994). Chaiklin et al (1959), however, reported that saucershaped audiograms are seen infrequently in cases of functional hearing loss. It thus remains to be determined if a tinnitus malingering test can be based on repeating LMs at a series of frequencies. For the tinnitus malingerer, it may present a greater challenge to provide reliable LMs at multiple frequencies, especially if the output-level step sizes are randomized. Results of the present study will therefore be used to design a similar test to specifically evaluate tinnitus malingering by establishing a tinnitus "fingerprint" that should be easily repeatable only if tinnitus is present. Reliability of Tinnitus LMs The literature concerning the repeatability of tinnitus LMs is sparse. Tinnitus clinicians have reported that within-session repeated LMs within ± 1 db could be routinely obtained from their patients (Bailey, 1979 ; Vernon et al, 1980). Goodwin and Johnson (1980) reported "extremely consistent" within-session LM data for nine patients. Using traditional psychoacoustic procedures, Penner (1983) and Burns (1984) each reported high variability in responses across sessions. Penner reported that the within-session responses had "substantially smaller" variability. Variability of responses would be expected to be smaller within than between sessions because of tinnitus fluctuations over time. Penner postulated, however, that patients may be using auditory memory within a session to provide consistent matches. 516

16 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al Penner and Bilger (1992) used two psychoacoustic procedures to match tinnitus loudness and pitch : a method of adjustment and a forced-choice double-staircase procedure. For obtaining LMs, the within-session variability of the two methods was comparable and similar to the variability when using the same procedures but with a pure tone that simulated the tinnitus. It was concluded that tinnitus could be matched reliably within a session but that measurements between sessions were not reliable. Mitchell et al (1993) used a computerassisted technique to obtain tinnitus loudness and pitch matches and tinnitus masking curves. The test-retest variability of LMs using this procedure was reported to agree with the small variability reported by some (Bailey, 1979 ; Vernon et al, 1980), but not with the large variations reported by others (Penner, 1983 ; Burns, 1984). Henry and Meikle (1999) conducted a study in which tinnitus LMs were measured in 26 subjects with chronic tinnitus using both pulsed and continuous tones. LMs were determined at 11 frequencies between 0.5 to 10 khz. No significant differences were found between pulsed versus continuous measures ; thus, the results could be used as a demonstration of within-session reliability. With an intertest interval of 10 to 15 minutes, these subjects were capable of reproducing their LMs to within ±2 to 3 db. The study evaluating the prototype automated technique for reliability of tinnitus LMs was discussed above (Henry et al, 1996). The previous and current studies reported Pearson is to evaluate between-session reliability of LMs. The Pearson is were comparable between studies, and almost all were statistically significant. The present study thus corroborates results of the preliminary study. It is clear that there are few studies from which to draw conclusions about the test-retest reliability of tinnitus LMs. Since each study has used somewhat different procedures to obtain LM measurements, results between studies cannot be easily compared. A standardized procedure would facilitate the comparison of measurements between investigators. Tinnitus LMs Expressed in db SPL or db SL The present study reports all LM data in both db SPL and db SL. Tables 10 and 11 reveal a clear difference between the two metrics when evaluating test-retest reliability of LMs. LMs expressed in db SPL were consistently more reliable than their db SL counterparts, both within and between sessions. The Pearson is for the db SPL loudness matches were almost universally > 0.90 at each test frequency (all p's <-.0001), and correlations were comparable to those obtained for hearing thresholds. Reliability of LMs expressed in db SL, however, was generally below 0.90, with correspondingly poorer p values. In the interpretation of these apparent reliability differences, it is known that narrowing the range of scores can markedly reduce the correlation between two variables (Welkowitz et al, 1976). When the LM measures are transformed to the db SL metric, the variability of the scores is restricted for both variables, which effectively decreases the correlation between the two. A further factor that could reduce this correlation is that the db SL metric introduces a second source of variability : hearing thresholds. Thus, determining the correlation of repeated LMs transformed to db SL uses a single measure derived from two measures that each vary from test to test. For purposes of obtaining reliable tinnitus LMs, the present data indicate that expressing the measures in db SPL will provide good response reliability. This seems to be true regardless of which frequency is used to obtain the measures. This point has relevance to any application in which tinnitus LMs must be obtained serially, such as determining the effect of treatment on tinnitus loudness or evaluating for malingering by obtaining repeated measures. Clinical Value of Tinnitus Matching Tests For clinical application, it is controversial whether tinnitus matching has any diagnostic value (Jastreboff, 1996). However, which side one takes in this issue may depend upon the type of treatment endorsed. If the treatment is masking, which is still routinely used in many clinics, identifying the spectrum of the tinnitus may have relevance for determining the bandwidth of the masking noise. This enables the application of "minimal masking," which would tailor the masking noise to the individual's tinnitus spectrum and level (Penner, 1983 ; Vernon, 1997). The effect would be similar to the critical bandwidth concept in which a restricted bandwidth of noise, centered around a pure-tone frequency, is sufficient to effectively mask the pure tone (Fletcher, 1940 ; Hawkins and Stevens, 1950 ; 517

17 Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 9, October 1999 Bilger and Hirsh, 1956 ; Goldstein and Newman, 1994). The masking approach to tinnitus includes the use of partial masking to provide relief, and the minimal masking concept would apply to both complete and partial masking (Vernon, 1997). Minimal masking would reduce the amount of sound presented to the patient, which would assist in making the masking noise more acceptable to the patient. Tinnitus Test Battery General guidelines for the clinical evaluation of tinnitus were established at a symposium sponsored by the CIBA Foundation in London (Evered and Lawrenson, 1981). They recommended a battery of tests to include pitch matching, loudness matching, tinnitus maskability, and residual inhibition. Vernon and Meikle (1981) provided procedural details for conducting these tests. These recommendations are still appropriate for today, but standardization of the procedures has not been accomplished. There are at least two major concerns for development of a standardized clinical measurement technique. First, the method must be conducted rapidly so that all measurements can be obtained within a period of time that is appropriate for a typical clinical schedule. For tinnitus patients, a series of tinnitus measurements in one ear should not require over 30 minutes. Second, within-patient measurements must be shown to be consistent over repeated tests. Between-patient reliability is not currently a concern because standard measurement techniques have not been established ; thus, population norms are unknown. Future Work The present study provides further documentation of the reliability of an automated technique to measure tinnitus LMs and of the comparability of results using the fixed and random methods. Future work will be directed toward (1) reducing testing time while maintaining optimal test-retest reliability of responses ; (2) using more complex stimuli for tinnitus matching when the tinnitus is not tonal ; (3) developing a "tinnitus malingering" test, possibly based on tinnitus LM curves and randomization of different testing parameters ; and (4) conducting clinical trials to document the utility of the technique in the clinical setting. Hopefully, this research will significantly impact the clinical management of tinnitus as well as tinnitus research involving humans. Acknowledgment. Funding for this study was provided by Veterans Affairs Rehabilitation Research and Development (RR&D) Service. REFERENCES Bailey Q. (1979). Audiological aspects of tinnitus. Aust J Audiol 1: Bilger RC, Hirsh LJ. (1956). Masking of tones by bands of noise. JAcoust Soc Am 28: Burns EM. (1984). A comparison of variability among measurements of subjective tinnitus and objective stimuli. Audiology 23: Carhart R. (1958). Audiometry in diagnosis. Laryngoscope 68: Carhart R, Jerger JF. (1959). Preferred method for clinical determination of pure-tone thresholds. J Speech Hear Disord 24: Chaiklin JB, Ventry IM, Barrett LS, Skalbeck GA. (1959). Pure-tone threshold patterns observed in functional hearing loss. Laryngoscope 69 : Coles RRA, Baskill JL. (1996). Absolute loudness of tinnitus : tinnitus clinic data. In : Reich GE, Vernon JA, eds. Proceedings of the Fifth International Tlnnitus Seminar Portland, OR : American Tinnitus Association, Doerfler LG. (1951). Psychogenic deafness and its detection. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 60 : Evered D, Lawrenson G (1981). Appendix II. Guidelines for recommended procedures in tinnitus testing. In : Evered D, Lawrenson G, eds. CIBA Foundation Symposium 85. 7Ennitus. London : Pitman, Fausti SA, Frey RH, Henry JA, Knutsen JM, Olson DJ. (1990). Reliability and validity of high-frequency (8-20 khz) thresholds obtained on a computer-based audiometer as compared to a documented laboratory system. J Am Acad Audiol 1: Fletcher H. (1940). Auditory patterns. Rev Mod Phys 12 : Fortune DS, Haynes DS, Hall JWI. (1999). Tinnitus: current evaluation and management. Otolaryngol Internist 83 : Fowler EP (1943). Control of head noises. Their illusions of loudness and of timbre. Arch Otolaryngol 37: Goetzinger CP, Proud GO. (1958). Deafness : examination techniques for evaluating malingering and psychogenic disabilities. J Kans Med Soc 59 : Goldstein B. (1997). Psychophysical and psychoacoustic correlates of tinnitus. In : ShulmanA, ed. TEnnitus Diagnosis ltreatment. San Diego : Singular, Goldstein BA, Newman CW (1994). Clinical masking : a decision-making process. In : Katz J, ed. Handbook of Clinical Audiology. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, Goodwin PE, Johnson RM. (1980). The loudness of tinnitus. Acta Otolaryngol 90 :

18 Tinnitus Loudness Matching/Henry et al Graham JT, Newby HA. (1962). Acoustical characteristics of tinnitus : an analysis. Arch Otolaryngol 75 : Hallam RS, Jakes SC, Chambers C, Hinchcliffe R. (1985). A comparison of different methods for assessing the `intensity' of tinnitus. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh) 99 : Hawkins JEJ, Stevens SS. (1950). The masking of pure tones and of speech by white noise. J Acoust Soc Am 22 :6-13. Hazell JW Wood SM, Cooper HR, Stephens SD, Corcoran AL, Coles RR, Baskill JL, Sheldrake JB. (1985). A clinical study of tinnitus maskers. Br JAudiol 19 : Henry JA. (1996). Loudness recruitment only partially explains the small size of tinnitus loudness-matches. In : Reich GE, Vernon JA, eds. Proceedings of the Fifth International Tinnitus Seminar Portland, OR : American Tinnitus Association, Henry JA, Fausti SA, Mitchell CR, Flick CL, Helt WJ. (1996). An automated technique for tinnitus evaluation. In : Reich GE, Vernon JA, eds. Proceedings of the Fifth International Tinnitus Seminar Portland, OR: American Tinnitus Association, Henry JA, Meikle MB. (1999). Techniques for measuring the loudness of tinnitus : pulsed versus continuous stimuli. J Am Acad Audiol 10 : Hinchcliffe R, Chambers C. (1983). Loudness of tinnitus : an approach to measurement. Adv Otorhinolaryngol 29 : Ishikawa T, Kusano H, Ogasawara M, Murai K, Tauiki T. (1991). Investigation of tinnitus tests : comparison of ipsilateral and contralateral testing. In : Aran J-M, Dauman R, eds. Fourth International Tinnitus Seminar. Bordeaux, France : Kugler, Jacobson GP, Henderson JA. (1999). Toward the development of a subjective measure of the presence of tinnitus : preliminary observations. In : Popelka GR, ed. Association for Research in Otolaryngology [abstract 682]. Mt. Royal, NJ : Association for Research in Otolaryngology. Jastreboff PJ. (1996). Usefulness of the psychoacoustical characterization of tinnitus. In : Reich GE, Vernon JA, eds. Proceedings of the Fifth International Unnitus Seminar Portland, OR: American Tinnitus Association, Kuk FK, Tyler RS, Rustad N, Harker LA, Tye-Murray N. (1989). Alternating current at the eardrum for tinnitus reduction. J Speech Hear Res 32 : Martin FN. (1994). Pseudohypacusis. In : Katz J, ed. Handbook of Clinical Audiology. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, Matsuhira T, Yamashita K, Yasuda M. (1992). Estimation of the loudness of tinnitus from matching tests. Br JAudiol 26 : Meikle MB, Henry JA, Mitchell CM. (1996). Methods for matching the loudness of tinnitus using external tones. In : Reich GE, Vernon JA, eds. Proceedings of the Fifth International Tinnitus Seminar Portland, OR: American Tinnitus Association, Meikle MB, Johnson RM, Griest SE, Press LS, Charnell MG. (1995). Oregon TFnnitus Data Archive. World Wide Web: http// Meikle MB, Vernon J, Johnson RM. (1984). The perceived severity of tinnitus. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 92 : Meikle M, Walsh ET. (1984). Characteristics of tinnitus and related observations in over 1800 tinnitus patients. J Laryngol Otol 9(Suppl) : Mitchell CR, Vernon JA, Creedon TA. (1993). Measuring tinnitus parameters : loudness, pitch, and maskability. J Am Acad Audiol 4: Murai K, Tyler RS, Harker LA, Stouffer JL. (1992). Review of pharmacologic treatment of tinnitus. Am J Otol 13: Penner MJ. (1983). Variability in matches to subjective tinnitus. J Speech Hear Res 26 : Penner MJ. (1984). Equal-loudness contours using subjective tinnitus as the standard. J Speech Hear Res 27 : Penner MJ. (1986). Magnitude estimation and the "paradoxical" loudness of tinnitus. J Speech Hear Res 29 : Penner MJ, Bilger RC. (1992). Consistent within-session measures of tinnitus. J Speech Hear Res 35 : Reed GF. (1960). An audiometric study of two hundred cases of subjective tinnitus. Arch Otolaryngol 71 : Risey J, Briner W, Guth PS, Norris CH. (1989). The superiority of the Goodwin procedure over the traditional procedure in measuring the loudness level of tinnitus. Ear Hear 10 : Robinson DW Dadson RS. (1956). A redetermination of the equal loudness relations for puretones. Br J Appl Phys 7: Roeser RJ, Price DR. (1980). Clinical experience with tinnitus maskers. Ear Hear 1 : Scott B, Lindberg P, Lyttkens L, Melin L. (1985). Psychological treatment of tinnitus. An experimental group study. Scand Audiol 14 : Tyler RS, Aran J-M, Dauman R. (1992). Recent advances in tinnitus. Am JAudiol 1 : Tyler RS, Conrad-Armes D. (1983). The determination of tinnitus loudness considering the effects of recruitment. J Speech Hear Res 26 : Vernon JA. (1976). The loudness (?) of tinnitus. Hear Speech Action 44: Vernon JA. (1982). Relief of tinnitus by masking treatment. In : English GM, ed. Otolaryngology. Philadelphia : Harper and Row, Vernon JA. (1987). Assessment of the tinnitus patient. In : Hazell JWP, ed. Tinnitus New York : Churchill Livingstone, Vernon JA. (1996). Is the claimed tinnitus real and is the claimed cause correct? In : Reich GE, Vernon JA eds. Proceedings of the Fifth International Unnitus Seminar 519

Tinnitus Assessment Appointment

Tinnitus Assessment Appointment Tinnitus Assessment Appointment Documents and Items Needed for Appointment Patient Record: Assessment Tab Forms Loudness Level Chart Demo Device TRQ, THQ, Tympanometry, Otoscopy, Tinnitus Ear 1. Tinnitus

More information

Beltone True TM with Tinnitus Breaker Pro

Beltone True TM with Tinnitus Breaker Pro Beltone True TM with Tinnitus Breaker Pro Beltone True Tinnitus Breaker Pro tinnitus datasheet How to use tinnitus test results It is important to remember that tinnitus is a symptom, not a disease. It

More information

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION

MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION MEASURING LOUDNESS OF LONG AND SHORT TONES USING MAGNITUDE ESTIMATION Michael Epstein 1,2, Mary Florentine 1,3, and Søren Buus 1,2 1Institute for Hearing, Speech, and Language 2Communications and Digital

More information

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes

DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring Week 6 Class Notes DAT335 Music Perception and Cognition Cogswell Polytechnical College Spring 2009 Week 6 Class Notes Pitch Perception Introduction Pitch may be described as that attribute of auditory sensation in terms

More information

Tinnitus Quick Guide

Tinnitus Quick Guide Tinnitus Quick Guide MADSEN Astera² offers a new module for tinnitus assessment. This new module is available free of charge in OTOsuite versions 4.65 and higher. Its objective is to assist clinicians

More information

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high.

Pitch. The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high. Pitch The perceptual correlate of frequency: the perceptual dimension along which sounds can be ordered from low to high. 1 The bottom line Pitch perception involves the integration of spectral (place)

More information

Tinnitus Assessment The key to successful tinnitus patient management

Tinnitus Assessment The key to successful tinnitus patient management Tinnitus Assessment The key to successful tinnitus patient management Wendy Switalski, Au.D Clement Sanchez, Aud Msc M M T 1. Introduction An audiologist meeting a patient complaining of tinnitus needs

More information

Experiments on tone adjustments

Experiments on tone adjustments Experiments on tone adjustments Jesko L. VERHEY 1 ; Jan HOTS 2 1 University of Magdeburg, Germany ABSTRACT Many technical sounds contain tonal components originating from rotating parts, such as electric

More information

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH '

EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH ' Journal oj Experimental Psychology 1972, Vol. 93, No. 1, 156-162 EFFECT OF REPETITION OF STANDARD AND COMPARISON TONES ON RECOGNITION MEMORY FOR PITCH ' DIANA DEUTSCH " Center for Human Information Processing,

More information

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is

We realize that this is really small, if we consider that the atmospheric pressure 2 is PART 2 Sound Pressure Sound Pressure Levels (SPLs) Sound consists of pressure waves. Thus, a way to quantify sound is to state the amount of pressure 1 it exertsrelatively to a pressure level of reference.

More information

Precise Digital Integration of Fast Analogue Signals using a 12-bit Oscilloscope

Precise Digital Integration of Fast Analogue Signals using a 12-bit Oscilloscope EUROPEAN ORGANIZATION FOR NUCLEAR RESEARCH CERN BEAMS DEPARTMENT CERN-BE-2014-002 BI Precise Digital Integration of Fast Analogue Signals using a 12-bit Oscilloscope M. Gasior; M. Krupa CERN Geneva/CH

More information

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1

Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses Tonality (Hearing Model) 1 02/18 Using the new psychoacoustic tonality analyses 1 As of ArtemiS SUITE 9.2, a very important new fully psychoacoustic approach to the measurement of tonalities is now available., based on the Hearing

More information

SigPlay User s Guide

SigPlay User s Guide SigPlay User s Guide . . SigPlay32 User's Guide? Version 3.4 Copyright? 2001 TDT. All rights reserved. No part of this manual may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or

More information

Signal Stability Analyser

Signal Stability Analyser Signal Stability Analyser o Real Time Phase or Frequency Display o Real Time Data, Allan Variance and Phase Noise Plots o 1MHz to 65MHz medium resolution (12.5ps) o 5MHz and 10MHz high resolution (50fs)

More information

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Volume 19, 2013 http://acousticalsociety.org/ ICA 2013 Montreal Montreal, Canada 2-7 June 2013 Psychological and Physiological Acoustics Session 4aPPb: Binaural Hearing

More information

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng

The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng The Research of Controlling Loudness in the Timbre Subjective Perception Experiment of Sheng S. Zhu, P. Ji, W. Kuang and J. Yang Institute of Acoustics, CAS, O.21, Bei-Si-huan-Xi Road, 100190 Beijing,

More information

Electrical Stimulation of the Cochlea to Reduce Tinnitus. Richard S. Tyler, Ph.D. Overview

Electrical Stimulation of the Cochlea to Reduce Tinnitus. Richard S. Tyler, Ph.D. Overview Electrical Stimulation of the Cochlea to Reduce Tinnitus Richard S., Ph.D. 1 Overview 1. Mechanisms of influencing tinnitus 2. Review of select studies 3. Summary of what is known 4. Next Steps 2 The University

More information

Savant. Savant. SignalCalc. Power in Numbers input channels. Networked chassis with 1 Gigabit Ethernet to host

Savant. Savant. SignalCalc. Power in Numbers input channels. Networked chassis with 1 Gigabit Ethernet to host Power in Numbers Savant SignalCalc 40-1024 input channels Networked chassis with 1 Gigabit Ethernet to host 49 khz analysis bandwidth, all channels with simultaneous storage to disk SignalCalc Dynamic

More information

Inhibition of Oscillation in a Plastic Neural Network Model of Tinnitus Therapy Using Noise Stimulus

Inhibition of Oscillation in a Plastic Neural Network Model of Tinnitus Therapy Using Noise Stimulus Inhibition of Oscillation in a Plastic Neural Network Model of Tinnitus Therapy Using Noise timulus Ken ichi Fujimoto chool of Health ciences, Faculty of Medicine, The University of Tokushima 3-8- Kuramoto-cho

More information

Switching Solutions for Multi-Channel High Speed Serial Port Testing

Switching Solutions for Multi-Channel High Speed Serial Port Testing Switching Solutions for Multi-Channel High Speed Serial Port Testing Application Note by Robert Waldeck VP Business Development, ASCOR Switching The instruments used in High Speed Serial Port testing are

More information

Quantify. The Subjective. PQM: A New Quantitative Tool for Evaluating Display Design Options

Quantify. The Subjective. PQM: A New Quantitative Tool for Evaluating Display Design Options PQM: A New Quantitative Tool for Evaluating Display Design Options Software, Electronics, and Mechanical Systems Laboratory 3M Optical Systems Division Jennifer F. Schumacher, John Van Derlofske, Brian

More information

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures

Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures Modeling sound quality from psychoacoustic measures Lena SCHELL-MAJOOR 1 ; Jan RENNIES 2 ; Stephan D. EWERT 3 ; Birger KOLLMEIER 4 1,2,4 Fraunhofer IDMT, Hör-, Sprach- und Audiotechnologie & Cluster of

More information

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern

Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern The 33 rd International Congress and Exposition on Noise Control Engineering Temporal summation of loudness as a function of frequency and temporal pattern I. Boullet a, J. Marozeau b and S. Meunier c

More information

A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL STUDY OF LOW AMPLITUDE SUPERSONIC AIRCRAFT NOISE AND OTHER TRANSIENT SOUNDS

A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL STUDY OF LOW AMPLITUDE SUPERSONIC AIRCRAFT NOISE AND OTHER TRANSIENT SOUNDS 19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 A SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL STUDY OF LOW AMPLITUDE SUPERSONIC AIRCRAFT NOISE AND OTHER TRANSIENT SOUNDS PACS: 43.28.Mw Marshall, Andrew

More information

ICaD 2013 DETERMINATION OF SUBJECTIVE TINNITUS CHARACTERISTICS BY MEANS OF SOUND SYNTHESIS CONTROLLED BY THE TOUCH SCREEN INTERFACE

ICaD 2013 DETERMINATION OF SUBJECTIVE TINNITUS CHARACTERISTICS BY MEANS OF SOUND SYNTHESIS CONTROLLED BY THE TOUCH SCREEN INTERFACE ICaD 2013 6 10 july, 2013, Łódź, Poland international Conference on auditory Display DETERMINATION OF SUBJECTIVE TINNITUS CHARACTERISTICS BY MEANS OF SOUND SYNTHESIS CONTROLLED BY THE TOUCH SCREEN INTERFACE

More information

Precision testing methods of Event Timer A032-ET

Precision testing methods of Event Timer A032-ET Precision testing methods of Event Timer A032-ET Event Timer A032-ET provides extreme precision. Therefore exact determination of its characteristics in commonly accepted way is impossible or, at least,

More information

UNDERSTANDING TINNITUS AND TINNITUS TREATMENTS

UNDERSTANDING TINNITUS AND TINNITUS TREATMENTS UNDERSTANDING TINNITUS AND TINNITUS TREATMENTS What is Tinnitus? Tinnitus is a hearing condition often described as a chronic ringing, hissing or buzzing in the ears. In almost all cases this is a subjective

More information

Noise Detector ND-1 Operating Manual

Noise Detector ND-1 Operating Manual Noise Detector ND-1 Operating Manual SPECTRADYNAMICS, INC 1849 Cherry St. Unit 2 Louisville, CO 80027 Phone: (303) 665-1852 Fax: (303) 604-6088 Table of Contents ND-1 Description...... 3 Safety and Preparation

More information

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair

Table 1 Pairs of sound samples used in this study Group1 Group2 Group1 Group2 Sound 2. Sound 2. Pair Acoustic annoyance inside aircraft cabins A listening test approach Lena SCHELL-MAJOOR ; Robert MORES Fraunhofer IDMT, Hör-, Sprach- und Audiotechnologie & Cluster of Excellence Hearing4All, Oldenburg

More information

Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Fan Noise

Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Fan Noise Psychoacoustic Evaluation of Fan Noise Dr. Marc Schneider Team Leader R&D - Acoustics ebm-papst Mulfingen GmbH & Co.KG Carolin Feldmann, University Siegen Outline Motivation Psychoacoustic Parameters Psychoacoustic

More information

SREV1 Sampling Guide. An Introduction to Impulse-response Sampling with the SREV1 Sampling Reverberator

SREV1 Sampling Guide. An Introduction to Impulse-response Sampling with the SREV1 Sampling Reverberator An Introduction to Impulse-response Sampling with the SREV Sampling Reverberator Contents Introduction.............................. 2 What is Sound Field Sampling?.....................................

More information

TransitHound Cellphone Detector User Manual Version 1.3

TransitHound Cellphone Detector User Manual Version 1.3 TransitHound Cellphone Detector User Manual Version 1.3 RF3 RF2 Table of Contents Introduction...3 PC Requirements...3 Unit Description...3 Electrical Interfaces...4 Interface Cable...5 USB to Serial Interface

More information

White Paper. Uniform Luminance Technology. What s inside? What is non-uniformity and noise in LCDs? Why is it a problem? How is it solved?

White Paper. Uniform Luminance Technology. What s inside? What is non-uniformity and noise in LCDs? Why is it a problem? How is it solved? White Paper Uniform Luminance Technology What s inside? What is non-uniformity and noise in LCDs? Why is it a problem? How is it solved? Tom Kimpe Manager Technology & Innovation Group Barco Medical Imaging

More information

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics)

Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics) 1 Musical Acoustics Lecture 15 Pitch & Frequency (Psycho-Acoustics) Pitch Pitch is a subjective characteristic of sound Some listeners even assign pitch differently depending upon whether the sound was

More information

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU

LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU The 21 st International Congress on Sound and Vibration 13-17 July, 2014, Beijing/China LOUDNESS EFFECT OF THE DIFFERENT TONES ON THE TIMBRE SUBJECTIVE PERCEPTION EXPERIMENT OF ERHU Siyu Zhu, Peifeng Ji,

More information

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch

Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch Measurement of overtone frequencies of a toy piano and perception of its pitch PACS: 43.75.Mn ABSTRACT Akira Nishimura Department of Media and Cultural Studies, Tokyo University of Information Sciences,

More information

Sound Quality Analysis of Electric Parking Brake

Sound Quality Analysis of Electric Parking Brake Sound Quality Analysis of Electric Parking Brake Bahare Naimipour a Giovanni Rinaldi b Valerie Schnabelrauch c Application Research Center, Sound Answers Inc. 6855 Commerce Boulevard, Canton, MI 48187,

More information

PRODUCT SHEET

PRODUCT SHEET ERS100C EVOKED RESPONSE AMPLIFIER MODULE The evoked response amplifier module (ERS100C) is a single channel, high gain, extremely low noise, differential input, biopotential amplifier designed to accurately

More information

Choosing an Oscilloscope

Choosing an Oscilloscope Choosing an Oscilloscope By Alan Lowne CEO Saelig Company (www.saelig.com) Post comments on this article at www.nutsvolts.com/ magazine/article/october2016_choosing-oscilloscopes. All sorts of questions

More information

Auditory Illusions. Diana Deutsch. The sounds we perceive do not always correspond to those that are

Auditory Illusions. Diana Deutsch. The sounds we perceive do not always correspond to those that are In: E. Bruce Goldstein (Ed) Encyclopedia of Perception, Volume 1, Sage, 2009, pp 160-164. Auditory Illusions Diana Deutsch The sounds we perceive do not always correspond to those that are presented. When

More information

Understanding PQR, DMOS, and PSNR Measurements

Understanding PQR, DMOS, and PSNR Measurements Understanding PQR, DMOS, and PSNR Measurements Introduction Compression systems and other video processing devices impact picture quality in various ways. Consumers quality expectations continue to rise

More information

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland

PREPARED FOR: U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command Fort Detrick, Maryland AWARD NUMBER: W81XWH-13-1-0491 TITLE: Default, Cognitive, and Affective Brain Networks in Human Tinnitus PRINCIPAL INVESTIGATOR: Jennifer R. Melcher, PhD CONTRACTING ORGANIZATION: Massachusetts Eye and

More information

Vocoder Reference Test TELECOMMUNICATIONS INDUSTRY ASSOCIATION

Vocoder Reference Test TELECOMMUNICATIONS INDUSTRY ASSOCIATION TIA/EIA STANDARD ANSI/TIA/EIA-102.BABC-1999 Approved: March 16, 1999 TIA/EIA-102.BABC Project 25 Vocoder Reference Test TIA/EIA-102.BABC (Upgrade and Revision of TIA/EIA/IS-102.BABC) APRIL 1999 TELECOMMUNICATIONS

More information

Tinnitus. Treatment for Professionals

Tinnitus. Treatment for Professionals Expand your hearing care with Oticon tinnitus treatment Tinnitus SoundSupport TM with ocean sounds Easy fitting Tinnitus Treatment for Professionals Tinnitus Sound App Counselling tools Patient support

More information

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England

Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EB, England Asymmetry of masking between complex tones and noise: Partial loudness Hedwig Gockel a) CNBH, Department of Physiology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EG, England Brian C. J. Moore

More information

The Distortion Magnifier

The Distortion Magnifier The Distortion Magnifier Bob Cordell January 13, 2008 Updated March 20, 2009 The Distortion magnifier described here provides ways of measuring very low levels of THD and IM distortions. These techniques

More information

Getting Started with the LabVIEW Sound and Vibration Toolkit

Getting Started with the LabVIEW Sound and Vibration Toolkit 1 Getting Started with the LabVIEW Sound and Vibration Toolkit This tutorial is designed to introduce you to some of the sound and vibration analysis capabilities in the industry-leading software tool

More information

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM)

TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM) TO HONOR STEVENS AND REPEAL HIS LAW (FOR THE AUDITORY STSTEM) Mary Florentine 1,2 and Michael Epstein 1,2,3 1Institute for Hearing, Speech, and Language 2Dept. Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology (133

More information

5/8/2013. Tinnitus Population. The Neuromonics Sanctuary. relief. 50 Million individuals suffer from tinnitus

5/8/2013. Tinnitus Population. The Neuromonics Sanctuary. relief. 50 Million individuals suffer from tinnitus Fitting the Sanctuary Device: A New Tinnitus Management Tool Casie Keaton, AuD, CCC-A Clinical Sales Manager casie.keaton@neuromonics.com Marta Hecocks, AuD, CCC-A Clinical Specialist marta.hecocks@neuromonics.com

More information

University of Groningen. Tinnitus Bartels, Hilke

University of Groningen. Tinnitus Bartels, Hilke University of Groningen Tinnitus Bartels, Hilke IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below.

More information

Precision DeEsser Users Guide

Precision DeEsser Users Guide Precision DeEsser Users Guide Metric Halo $Revision: 1670 $ Publication date $Date: 2012-05-01 13:50:00-0400 (Tue, 01 May 2012) $ Copyright 2012 Metric Halo. MH Production Bundle, ChannelStrip 3, Character,

More information

Interface Practices Subcommittee SCTE STANDARD SCTE Measurement Procedure for Noise Power Ratio

Interface Practices Subcommittee SCTE STANDARD SCTE Measurement Procedure for Noise Power Ratio Interface Practices Subcommittee SCTE STANDARD SCTE 119 2018 Measurement Procedure for Noise Power Ratio NOTICE The Society of Cable Telecommunications Engineers (SCTE) / International Society of Broadband

More information

APP USE USER MANUAL 2017 VERSION BASED ON WAVE TRACKING TECHNIQUE

APP USE USER MANUAL 2017 VERSION BASED ON WAVE TRACKING TECHNIQUE APP USE USER MANUAL 2017 VERSION BASED ON WAVE TRACKING TECHNIQUE All rights reserved All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in

More information

Loudness and Sharpness Calculation

Loudness and Sharpness Calculation 10/16 Loudness and Sharpness Calculation Psychoacoustics is the science of the relationship between physical quantities of sound and subjective hearing impressions. To examine these relationships, physical

More information

DSA-1. The Prism Sound DSA-1 is a hand-held AES/EBU Signal Analyzer and Generator.

DSA-1. The Prism Sound DSA-1 is a hand-held AES/EBU Signal Analyzer and Generator. DSA-1 The Prism Sound DSA-1 is a hand-held AES/EBU Signal Analyzer and Generator. The DSA-1 is an invaluable trouble-shooting tool for digital audio equipment and installations. It is unique as a handportable,

More information

Chapter 2 Tinnitus Treatment as a Problem Area

Chapter 2 Tinnitus Treatment as a Problem Area Chapter 2 Tinnitus Treatment as a Problem Area Abstract This chapter presents the decision problem area which will be supported with a recommender system technology, that is, tinnitus diagnosis and treatment.

More information

BER MEASUREMENT IN THE NOISY CHANNEL

BER MEASUREMENT IN THE NOISY CHANNEL BER MEASUREMENT IN THE NOISY CHANNEL PREPARATION... 2 overview... 2 the basic system... 3 a more detailed description... 4 theoretical predictions... 5 EXPERIMENT... 6 the ERROR COUNTING UTILITIES module...

More information

Improving methods for tinnitus-matching in patients with noise-like tinnitus - STSM Report

Improving methods for tinnitus-matching in patients with noise-like tinnitus - STSM Report Improving methods for tinnitus-matching in patients with noise-like tinnitus - STSM Report Patrick Neff March 2017 1 Purpose of mission Tinnitus matching in patients with noise-like tinnitus is a complex

More information

HBI Database. Version 2 (User Manual)

HBI Database. Version 2 (User Manual) HBI Database Version 2 (User Manual) St-Petersburg, Russia 2007 2 1. INTRODUCTION...3 2. RECORDING CONDITIONS...6 2.1. EYE OPENED AND EYE CLOSED CONDITION....6 2.2. VISUAL CONTINUOUS PERFORMANCE TASK...6

More information

Techniques for Extending Real-Time Oscilloscope Bandwidth

Techniques for Extending Real-Time Oscilloscope Bandwidth Techniques for Extending Real-Time Oscilloscope Bandwidth Over the past decade, data communication rates have increased by a factor well over 10X. Data rates that were once 1Gb/sec and below are now routinely

More information

Client centred sound therapy selection: Tinnitus assessment into practice. G D Searchfield

Client centred sound therapy selection: Tinnitus assessment into practice. G D Searchfield Client centred sound therapy selection: Tinnitus assessment into practice G D Searchfield Definitions Sound (or Acoustic) therapy is a generic term used to describe the use of sound to have a postive effect

More information

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults

Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults Noise evaluation based on loudness-perception characteristics of older adults Kenji KURAKATA 1 ; Tazu MIZUNAMI 2 National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology (AIST), Japan ABSTRACT

More information

An Introduction to the Spectral Dynamics Rotating Machinery Analysis (RMA) package For PUMA and COUGAR

An Introduction to the Spectral Dynamics Rotating Machinery Analysis (RMA) package For PUMA and COUGAR An Introduction to the Spectral Dynamics Rotating Machinery Analysis (RMA) package For PUMA and COUGAR Introduction: The RMA package is a PC-based system which operates with PUMA and COUGAR hardware to

More information

Resound TS: An Innovative Tinnitus Sound Generator Device to Assist in Tinnitus Management

Resound TS: An Innovative Tinnitus Sound Generator Device to Assist in Tinnitus Management Resound TS: An Innovative Tinnitus Sound Generator Device to Assist in Tinnitus Management Michael Piskosz, M.S., Board Certified in Audiology Snehal Kulkarni, Au.D. Tinnitus is a concern for many people,

More information

Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant

Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant Temporal Envelope and Periodicity Cues on Musical Pitch Discrimination with Acoustic Simulation of Cochlear Implant Lichuan Ping 1, 2, Meng Yuan 1, Qinglin Meng 1, 2 and Haihong Feng 1 1 Shanghai Acoustics

More information

Brain-Computer Interface (BCI)

Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) Brain-Computer Interface (BCI) Christoph Guger, Günter Edlinger, g.tec Guger Technologies OEG Herbersteinstr. 60, 8020 Graz, Austria, guger@gtec.at This tutorial shows HOW-TO find and extract proper signal

More information

NOTICE: This document is for use only at UNSW. No copies can be made of this document without the permission of the authors.

NOTICE: This document is for use only at UNSW. No copies can be made of this document without the permission of the authors. Brüel & Kjær Pulse Primer University of New South Wales School of Mechanical and Manufacturing Engineering September 2005 Prepared by Michael Skeen and Geoff Lucas NOTICE: This document is for use only

More information

Psychoacoustics. lecturer:

Psychoacoustics. lecturer: Psychoacoustics lecturer: stephan.werner@tu-ilmenau.de Block Diagram of a Perceptual Audio Encoder loudness critical bands masking: frequency domain time domain binaural cues (overview) Source: Brandenburg,

More information

Informational Masking and Trained Listening. Undergraduate Honors Thesis

Informational Masking and Trained Listening. Undergraduate Honors Thesis Informational Masking and Trained Listening Undergraduate Honors Thesis Presented in partial fulfillment of requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of the Arts by Erica Laughlin The Ohio State University

More information

Summary Table Voluntary Product Accessibility Template. Supporting Features. Supports. Supports. Supports. Supports

Summary Table Voluntary Product Accessibility Template. Supporting Features. Supports. Supports. Supports. Supports Date: 15 November 2017 Name of Product: Lenovo 500 Wireless Combo Keyboard and Mouse Summary Table Voluntary Product Accessibility Template Section 1194.21 Software Applications and Operating Systems Section

More information

NOTICE. The information contained in this document is subject to change without notice.

NOTICE. The information contained in this document is subject to change without notice. NOTICE The information contained in this document is subject to change without notice. Toontrack Music AB makes no warranty of any kind with regard to this material, including, but not limited to, the

More information

Natural Radio. News, Comments and Letters About Natural Radio January 2003 Copyright 2003 by Mark S. Karney

Natural Radio. News, Comments and Letters About Natural Radio January 2003 Copyright 2003 by Mark S. Karney Natural Radio News, Comments and Letters About Natural Radio January 2003 Copyright 2003 by Mark S. Karney Recorders for Natural Radio Signals There has been considerable discussion on the VLF_Group of

More information

ENGINEERING COMMITTEE

ENGINEERING COMMITTEE ENGINEERING COMMITTEE Interface Practices Subcommittee SCTE STANDARD SCTE 45 2017 Test Method for Group Delay NOTICE The Society of Cable Telecommunications Engineers (SCTE) Standards and Operational Practices

More information

Full Disclosure Monitoring

Full Disclosure Monitoring Full Disclosure Monitoring Power Quality Application Note Full Disclosure monitoring is the ability to measure all aspects of power quality, on every voltage cycle, and record them in appropriate detail

More information

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE Copyright SFA - InterNoise 2000 1 inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering 27-30 August 2000, Nice, FRANCE I-INCE Classification: 7.9 THE FUTURE OF SOUND

More information

Hearing Aids for Tinnitus Patients: It s Not Just About Speech. Steve Benton, Au.D. VA Medical Center Decatur, GA

Hearing Aids for Tinnitus Patients: It s Not Just About Speech. Steve Benton, Au.D. VA Medical Center Decatur, GA Hearing Aids for Tinnitus Patients: It s Not Just About Speech Steve Benton, Au.D. VA Medical Center Decatur, GA 30033 steve.benton@va.gov Introduction Progressive Tinnitus Management, or PTM (Henry et

More information

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction

The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds. Introduction Music-Perception Winter 1990, Vol. 8, No. 2, 203-214 I990 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA The Tone Height of Multiharmonic Sounds ROY D. PATTERSON MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge,

More information

Using the BHM binaural head microphone

Using the BHM binaural head microphone 11/17 Using the binaural head microphone Introduction 1 Recording with a binaural head microphone 2 Equalization of a recording 2 Individual equalization curves 5 Using the equalization curves 5 Post-processing

More information

Transmitter Interface Program

Transmitter Interface Program Transmitter Interface Program Operational Manual Version 3.0.4 1 Overview The transmitter interface software allows you to adjust configuration settings of your Max solid state transmitters. The following

More information

Critical Review: Is there evidence to support that hearing aids benefit adults in the reduction of tinnitus perception?

Critical Review: Is there evidence to support that hearing aids benefit adults in the reduction of tinnitus perception? Critical Review: Is there evidence to support that hearing aids benefit adults in the reduction of tinnitus perception? Lam, H. M.Cl.Sc (AUD.) Candidate The University of Western Ontario: School of Communication

More information

Government Product Accessibility Template for Servers

Government Product Accessibility Template for Servers Government Product Accessibility Template for Servers Summary Column one includes all the Sections of the Standard that may apply to any deliverable. The total number of provisions within each Section

More information

2. AN INTROSPECTION OF THE MORPHING PROCESS

2. AN INTROSPECTION OF THE MORPHING PROCESS 1. INTRODUCTION Voice morphing means the transition of one speech signal into another. Like image morphing, speech morphing aims to preserve the shared characteristics of the starting and final signals,

More information

Contents. Welcome to LCAST. System Requirements. Compatibility. Installation and Authorization. Loudness Metering. True-Peak Metering

Contents. Welcome to LCAST. System Requirements. Compatibility. Installation and Authorization. Loudness Metering. True-Peak Metering LCAST User Manual Contents Welcome to LCAST System Requirements Compatibility Installation and Authorization Loudness Metering True-Peak Metering LCAST User Interface Your First Loudness Measurement Presets

More information

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS

SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS SHORT TERM PITCH MEMORY IN WESTERN vs. OTHER EQUAL TEMPERAMENT TUNING SYSTEMS Areti Andreopoulou Music and Audio Research Laboratory New York University, New York, USA aa1510@nyu.edu Morwaread Farbood

More information

Beltone Tinnitus Breaker Pro: Breaking the tinnitus cycle. Snehal Kulkarni, Au.D. Michael Piskosz, M.S.

Beltone Tinnitus Breaker Pro: Breaking the tinnitus cycle. Snehal Kulkarni, Au.D. Michael Piskosz, M.S. Beltone Tinnitus Breaker Pro: Breaking the tinnitus cycle Snehal Kulkarni, Au.D. Michael Piskosz, M.S. Beltone Tinnitus Breaker Pro: Breaking the tinnitus cycle Beltone Tinnitus Breaker Pro: Breaking the

More information

Voluntary Product Accessibility Template

Voluntary Product Accessibility Template Date: May 18th, 2015 Product Name: Samsung DB D Series Large Format Displays Product Version Number: DB22D-T Vendor Company Name: Samsung Electronics America, Inc. Vendor Contact Name: Kevin Schroll Vendor

More information

Music 175: Pitch II. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015

Music 175: Pitch II. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015 Music 175: Pitch II Tamara Smyth, trsmyth@ucsd.edu Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) June 2, 2015 1 Quantifying Pitch Logarithms We have seen several times so far that what

More information

Please feel free to download the Demo application software from analogarts.com to help you follow this seminar.

Please feel free to download the Demo application software from analogarts.com to help you follow this seminar. Hello, welcome to Analog Arts spectrum analyzer tutorial. Please feel free to download the Demo application software from analogarts.com to help you follow this seminar. For this presentation, we use a

More information

Loudness of pink noise and stationary technical sounds

Loudness of pink noise and stationary technical sounds Loudness of pink noise and stationary technical sounds Josef Schlittenlacher, Takeo Hashimoto, Hugo Fastl, Seiichiro Namba, Sonoko Kuwano 5 and Shigeko Hatano,, Seikei University -- Kichijoji Kitamachi,

More information

Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones

Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones Equal Intensity Contours for Whole-Body Vibrations Compared With Vibrations Cross-Modally Matched to Isophones Sebastian Merchel, M. Ercan Altinsoy and Maik Stamm Chair of Communication Acoustics, Dresden

More information

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE

UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE UNIVERSITY OF DUBLIN TRINITY COLLEGE FACULTY OF ENGINEERING & SYSTEMS SCIENCES School of Engineering and SCHOOL OF MUSIC Postgraduate Diploma in Music and Media Technologies Hilary Term 31 st January 2005

More information

Assessing and Measuring VCR Playback Image Quality, Part 1. Leo Backman/DigiOmmel & Co.

Assessing and Measuring VCR Playback Image Quality, Part 1. Leo Backman/DigiOmmel & Co. Assessing and Measuring VCR Playback Image Quality, Part 1. Leo Backman/DigiOmmel & Co. Assessing analog VCR image quality and stability requires dedicated measuring instruments. Still, standard metrics

More information

The high-end network analyzers from Rohde & Schwarz now include an option for pulse profile measurements plus, the new R&S ZVA 40 covers the

The high-end network analyzers from Rohde & Schwarz now include an option for pulse profile measurements plus, the new R&S ZVA 40 covers the GENERAL PURPOSE 44 448 The high-end network analyzers from Rohde & Schwarz now include an option for pulse profile measurements plus, the new R&S ZVA 4 covers the frequency range up to 4 GHz. News from

More information

Voluntary Product Accessibility Template

Voluntary Product Accessibility Template Date: October 12, 2016 Product Name: Samsung NE Smart HealthCare TV series Product Version Number: HG43NE593SFXZA Vendor Company Name: Samsung Electronics America, Inc. Vendor Contact Name: Sylvia Lee

More information

Boonton 4540 Remote Operation Modes

Boonton 4540 Remote Operation Modes Application Note Boonton 4540 Remote Operation Modes Mazumder Alam Product Marketing Manager, Boonton Electronics Abstract Boonton 4540 series power meters are among the leading edge instruments for most

More information

Experiment 13 Sampling and reconstruction

Experiment 13 Sampling and reconstruction Experiment 13 Sampling and reconstruction Preliminary discussion So far, the experiments in this manual have concentrated on communications systems that transmit analog signals. However, digital transmission

More information

How To Demonstrate Improved ACLR Dynamic Range With FSU and Noise Correction

How To Demonstrate Improved ACLR Dynamic Range With FSU and Noise Correction Product: Spectrum Analyzer FSU How To Demonstrate Improved ACLR Dynamic Range With FSU and Noise Correction Application Note This application note provides information about the ACLR measurement with noise

More information

Summary Table Voluntary Product Accessibility Template. Supporting Features

Summary Table Voluntary Product Accessibility Template. Supporting Features Date: 05/14/2010 Name of Product: Oxygen Forensic Software 2010 Pro Contact for more Information: Christine Young, Teel Technologies Inc. (203) 855-5387 Summary Table Section 1194.21 Software Applications

More information

Out of Band Spurious Measurement for Bluetooth Modules

Out of Band Spurious Measurement for Bluetooth Modules Products: Signal Analyser FSIQ26/FSP13/FSU8/FSQ26 Out of Band Spurious Measurement for Bluetooth Modules This application notes describes the out of band Spurious emission measurement for Bluetooth modules

More information

High Performance TFT LCD Driver ICs for Large-Size Displays

High Performance TFT LCD Driver ICs for Large-Size Displays Name: Eugenie Ip Title: Technical Marketing Engineer Company: Solomon Systech Limited www.solomon-systech.com The TFT LCD market has rapidly evolved in the last decade, enabling the occurrence of large

More information